Psych essay overview 9

write a 2-3-page-long essay overviewing this paper, and one paragraph of personal feelings

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Psych essay overview 9
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

Copyright © 2002 American Psychological Society

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 19

1

These basic questions are central to
any evolutionary approach. Psy-
chologists who do not like the sim-
plicity of the answers currently
coming out of evolutionary psy-
chology should make an effort to
improve them, to broaden its intel-
lectual horizon, because all of psy-
chology would stand to gain from
a more enlightened evolutionary
psychology.

Recommended Reading

de Waal, F.B.M. (1999). The end of

nature versus nurture.

Scientific
American

,

281

, 94–99.
de Waal, F.B.M. (2001).

The ape and
the sushi master: Cultural reflections
by a primatologist

. New York: Basic
Books.

Mayr, E. (2001).

What evolution is

.
New York: Basic Books.

Zimmer, C. (2001).

Evolution: The tri-
umph of an idea

. New York: Harper
Collins.

Acknowledgments—

I thank Allison
Berger and Virginia Holt for providing

the transcript of my 2001 Focus on Sci-
ence Plenary Address, which was pre-
s e n t e d a t t h e a n n u a l m e e t i n g o f t h e
American Psychological Association in
San Francisco and was on the topic of this
essay. I am also grateful to Mauricio Pap-
ini and Scott Lilienfeld for comments on
previous versions of the manuscript.

Notes

1. Address correspondence to Frans
B.M. de Waal, Living Links, Yerkes Pri-
mate Research Center, Emory Univer-
sity, 954 N. Gatewood Rd., Atlanta, GA
30322.

2. Theory of mind means that one
understands the mental states of others
(a capacity that may be limited to hu-
mans and apes).

References

Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988).

Homicide

. Haw-
thorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

de Waal, F.B.M. (1996).

Good natured: The origins of
right and wrong in humans and other animals

.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

de Waal, F.B.M. (1998).

Chimpanzee politics

. Balti-

more: Johns Hopkins University Press. (Origi-
nal work published 1982)

de Waal, F.B.M. (2000, April 2). Survival of the
rapist [Review of the book

A natural history of
rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion

].

New York
Times Book Review

, pp. 24–25.

Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes
sense except in the light of evolu

tion.

American
Biology Teacher,

35

, 125–129.

Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M.J. (1997). Becoming a “Gree-
ble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face
recognition.

Vision Research

,

37

, 1673–1682.

McDougall, W. (1908).

An introduction to social psy-
chology

. New York: Putnam.

Muscarella, F., & Cunningham, M.R. (1996). The
evolutionary significance and social percep-
tion of male pattern baldness and facial hair.

Ethology & Sociobiology,

17

, 99–117.

Panksepp, J., & Panksepp, J.B. (2000). The seven
sins of evolutionary psychology.

Evolution and
Cognition,

6

, 108–131.

Staats, A.W. (1991). Unified positivism and unifi-
cation psychology: Fad or new field?

American
Psychologist,

46

, 899–912.

Thornhill, R., & Palmer, C.T. (2000).

A natural his-
tory of rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion

.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychologi-
cal foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L.
Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.),

The adapted mind:
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of cul-
ture

(pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.

Williams, G. (1966).

Adaptation and natural selection

.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Wilson, E.O. (1998).

Consilience: The unity of knowl-
edge

. New York: Knopf.

Mutually Responsive Orientation
Between Mothers and Their Young
Children: A Context for the Early
Development of Conscience

Grazyna Kochanska

1

Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa

Abstract

Some parent-child dyads es-
tablish a mutually responsive
orientation (MRO), a relation-
ship that is close, mutually
binding, cooperative, and af-

fectively positive. Such rela-
t i o n s h i p s h a v e t w o m a i n
characteristics—mutual re-
sponsiveness and shared posi-
tive affect—and they foster the

development of conscience in
young children. Children grow-
ing up with parents who are re-
sponsive to their needs and
whose interactions are infused
with happy emotions adopt a
willing, responsive stance to-
ward parental influence and be-
come eager to embrace parental
values and standards for behav-
ior. The concurrent and longitu-

dinal beneficial effects of

MRO

for early development of con-
science have been replicated
across studies, for a broad range
of developmental periods from
infancy through early school
age, and using a wide variety of
behavioral, emotional, and cog-
nitive measures of conscience in
the laboratory, at home, and in
school. These findings highlight
the importance of the early par-
ent-child relationship for subse-
quent moral development.

Keywords

relationships; mutuality; con-
science

How do young children become
aware of rules, values, and standards
of behavior accepted within their

192 VOLUME 11, NUMBER 6, DECEMBER 2002

Published by Blackwell Publishing Inc.

families and cultures? How do they
gradually come to internalize those
values and make them their own?
Why do some children adopt soci-
etal norms wholeheartedly and with
ease, and become conscientious citi-
zens, whereas others do not?

The emergence of an individual
conscience, a reliable internal guid-
ance system that regulates conduct
without the need for external con-
trol, is the endpoint of the process
of integrating a child into a broader
network of values. How this pro-
cess works continues to be debated
as one of the perennial and central
i s s u e s i n h u m a n s o c i a l i z a t i o n
(Grusec, 1997).

Research on conscience was once
dominated by a cognitive approach,
focused on children’s abstract under-
standing of societal rules, measured
by their ability to reason about hypo-
thetical moral dilemmas. Moral de-
velopment was seen as a product of
cognitive maturation, aided by peer
interactions, but fundamentally un-
related to parental influence. In con-
trast, other theories acknowledged
parental contributions. Parents and
other socializing agents were seen as
critical in several versions of learning
theory. Those approaches empha-
sized the importance of parental dis-
cipline and modeling as instruments
that modify and shape children’s be-
havior. Somewhat later, attributional
theories underscored the importance
of children’s perceptions of parental
discipline, and revealed surprising,
often paradoxical effects of salient
parental rewards and punishments.

More recently, many scholars
have come to appreciate an ap-
proach grounded in psychoana-
lytic and neo-psychoanalytic theo-
ries. Although Freud’s views on
t h e e a r l y d e v e l o p m e n t o f c o n –
science as linked to the Oedipus or
Electra complex have long been
discarded, his general emphasis on
the role of early emotions and early
relationships in emerging morality
has proven insightful. That ap-
proach has been strongly reinvigo-

rated and modernized by John
Bowlby and the burgeoning re-
search on attachment. From that
perspective, moral emotions, moral
conduct, and moral thought are all
components of an internal guid-
ance system, or conscience, whose
f o u n d a t i o n s a r e e s t a b l i s h e d i n
early childhood in the context of
socialization in the family. The
early parent-child relationship,
which encompasses but is not lim-
ited to control and discipline, can
substantially foster or undermine
that process (Emde, Biringen, Cly-
man, & Oppenheim, 1991).

THE RELATIONSHIP
PERSPECTIVE: MUTUALLY

RESPONSIVE ORIENTATION

In 1951, Robert Sears argued for
a shift in psychological research
f r o m s t u d y i n g i n d i v i d u a l s t o
studying dyads. Over the past two
or three decades, the science of re-
lationships has blossomed in per-
sonality, social, and developmental
psychology (Collins & Laursen, 1999;
Reis, Collins, & Berscheid, 2000). Sev-
eral scholars have proposed that

when relationship partners—whether
two adults or a parent and a child—
are responsive and attuned to each
other, are mutually supportive,
and enjoy being together, they
form an internal model of their re-
lationship as a cooperative enter-
prise, and develop an eager, recep-
tive stance toward each other’s
influence and a compelling sense
of obligation to willingly comply
with the other. For example, Clark
(1984) referred to “communal rela-
tionships” in adults as contexts in
which the partners are invested in
each other’s well-being, are em-
p a t h i c a n d r e s p o n s i v e t o e a c h
other, and experience an internal
sense of mutual obligation.

In developmental research, those
resurging perspectives afford a pro-
ductive vantage point for exploring

social development. Socialization is
seen as a process jointly constructed
by parents and children over time
(Collins & Laursen, 1999; Collins,
Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington,
& Bornstein, 2000; Maccoby, 1999;
Reis et al., 2000). Maccoby (1999)
referred to parent-child mutuality
as a positive socialization force that
engenders a spirit of cooperation in
the child. Attachment scholars be-
lieve that children raised in a loving,
responsive manner become eager
to cooperate with their caregivers
and to embrace their values.

To describe such relationships
between parents and children, my
colleagues and I have proposed a
construct of

mutually responsive ori-
entation

(

MRO

). MRO is a positive,
close, mutually binding, and coop-
erative relationship, which encom-
passes two components:

responsive-
n e s s

a n d

s h a r e d p o s i t i v e a f f e c t

.
Responsiveness refers to the par-
ent’s and the child’s willing, sensi-
tive, supportive, and developmen-
tally appropriate response to one
another’s signals of distress, un-
happiness, needs, bids for atten-
tion, or attempts to exert influence.
Shared positive affect refers to the
“good times” shared by the parent
and the child—pleasurable, harmo-
nious, smoothly flowing interac-
tions infused with positive emo-
tions experienced by both.

We further proposed that chil-
dren who grow up in mutually re-
sponsive dyads, compared with
those who do not, become more ea-
ger to embrace their parents’ val-
ues and more likely to develop a
s t r o n g c o n s c i e n c e . T h e i r e a g e r
stance to embrace parental values
reflects an internal sense of obliga-
tion to respond positively to paren-
tal influence, and emerges from a
history of mutually gratifying, mu-
tually accommodating experiences.
A child who has developed a mu-
tually responsive relationship with
the parent comes to trust the par-
ent and to expect that the parent
will be responsive and supportive;

Copyright © 2002 American Psychological Society

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 193

at the same time, the child comes to
feel motivated to cooperate will-
ingly with the parent, to embrace
the parent’s values, and to adopt
parental standards for behavior
and make them his or her own. In
this view, the parent-child relation-
ship influences the child’s con-
science mainly through a gradually
evolving shared working model of
the relationship as a mutually co-
operative enterprise rather than
through the cumulative history of
parental discipline as the instru-
ment of behavior modification.

MOTHER-CHILD MRO AND
CHILDREN’S CONSCIENCE:

EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE

In two large studies, we mea-
sured the qualities of the mother-
child relationship and the child’s
emerging conscience for more than
200 mother-child dyads. To assess
the strength of MRO for the indi-
vidual dyads, we observed the
mothers and children interacting in
multiple lengthy, naturalistic yet
carefully scripted contexts at home
and in the laboratory. The situa-
tions we observed included care-
giving routines, preparing and eat-
ing meals, playing, relaxing, and
doing household chores. We coded
each mother’s responsiveness to her
child’s numerous signals of needs,
signs of physical or emotional dis-
tress or discomfort, bids for atten-
tion, and social overtures. We also
assessed shared positive affect by
coding the flow of emotion expres-
sion for both the mother and the
child over the course of each inter-
action, focusing particularly on the
times when they both displayed
positive emotion. We obtained
these measures repeatedly, follow-
ing the same families over a period
of several years.

In the individual dyads, the de-
gree of MRO was significantly con-

sistent across separate sessions
close in time, and significantly sta-
ble over several years. This indi-
cates that our observational mark-
ers captured a robust quality of the
relationships that unfolded along a
fairly stable dyadic trajectory.

Using a broad variety of labora-
tory paradigms, we also observed
rich manifestations of the young
children’s conscience: moral emo-
tions, moral conduct, and moral
cognition. These assessments took
place at many points in the chil-
dren’s development—starting in their
2nd year and continuing until early
school age. The children’s moral
emotions, including guilt, discom-
fort, concern, and empathy, were
observed when they were led to
believe that they had violated a
standard of conduct, or when they
witnessed others’ distress. While
they were unsupervised, either
alone or with peers, their moral
c o n d u c t w a s a s s e s s e d i n m a n y
types of situations in which they
faced strong temptations to break
various rules and were coaxed to
v i o l a t e s t a n d a r d s o f b e h a v i o r .
Their moral cognition was mea-
sured by presenting them with
a g e – a p p r o p r i a t e , h y p o t h e t i c a l
moral dilemmas and asking them
to express their thoughts and feel-
ings about rules and transgres-
sions, and consider moral deci-
sions. We also asked their mothers
and teachers to evaluate the chil-
dren’s moral emotions and con-
duct displayed in environments
outside the laboratory—at home
and at school.

Both studies supported the view
that children who grow up in a
context of a highly mutually re-
sponsive relationship with their
mothers develop strong consciences
(Kochanska, 1997; Kochanska, For-
man, & Coy, 1999; Kochanska &
Murray, 2000). The strength of the
replicated findings was striking,
given the broad range of the chil-
dren’s ages and the wide variety of
conscience measures used.

In both studies, the links be-
tween MRO and the development
of conscience were both concurrent
and longitudinal. The concurrent
links were found for both toddlers
and preschoolers. The longitudinal
findings were robust: MRO in in-
fancy predicted conscience devel-
opment in the 2nd year, and MRO
i n t o d d l e r h o o d p r e d i c t e d c h i l –
dren’s conscience at preschool age
and again at early school age. The
history of MRO in the first 2 years
predicted conscience at age 5. In
short, the beneficial effect of MRO
on the development of conscience
was evident across diverse measures
of conscience involving emotions,
conduct, and cognition. It was also
evident whether conscience was
assessed by observations in the lab-
oratory or reports from mothers
and teachers. These results have
been replicated by other research-
ers (Laible & Thompson, 2000).

HOW DOES MRO EXERT
ITS IMPACT?

What causal mechanisms may
be responsible for these well-estab-
lished empirical findings? Using sta-
tistical approaches (sequences of
multiple regressions, as well as
structural equations modeling, or
SEM) to analyze the causal factors
that accounted for the associations
in our data, we determined that
MRO exerts its influence through
at least two mechanisms.

The first mechanism involves
p r o m o t i n g t h e c h i l d ’ s p o s i t i v e
mood. Early MRO between the
parent and the child contributes to
the child’s positive, happy disposi-
tion, and that, in turn, increases his
or her broad eagerness to behave
prosocially. This finding is consis-
tent with a large body of research
in social and developmental psy-
chology (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998).
Adults and children who are in a
positive mood have often been

194 VOLUME 11, NUMBER 6, DECEMBER 2002

Published by Blackwell Publishing Inc.

found to be more prosocial, altruis-
tic, cooperative, rule abiding, and
socially responsive than those who
are in neutral or negative moods.

T h e s e c o n d m e c h a n i s m i n –
volves promoting the child’s re-
sponsive stance toward parental
influence. We have found that in
playlike teaching situations, chil-
dren in mutually responsive rela-
t i o n s h i p s a r e a t t u n e d t o t h e i r
mothers and eagerly follow their
lead (Forman & Kochanska, 2001;
Kochanska et al., 1999). In disci-
pline situations, they show what we
called

committed compliance

—will-
ing, eager, wholehearted coopera-
tion with the parent (Kochanska,
Coy, & Murray, 2001). Such a gen-
eralized responsive stance may be
an intermediate step between sim-
ple cooperation with the parent
and genuine internalization of pa-
rental rules, evident even in the
parent’s absence. We believe it re-
flects the child’s emerging working
model of a cooperative, reciprocal,
mutually accommodating relation-
ship in which partners naturally do
things for one another without ab-
rogating their autonomy.

FUTURE RESEARCH
DIRECTIONS

MRO and Qualities
of Individuals

It takes two to develop dyadic
MRO. Although the relationship be-
tween a parent and child—like any
relationship—is more than a sim-
ple sum of their characteristics,
those characteristics may neverthe-
less foster or impede the formation
of MRO. Recent advances in re-
search on the role of genetics in be-
havior and on the biological foun-
dations of children’s temperament
are beginning to be reflected in sci-
entific work in what has been tradi-
tionally conceived as the domain of
relationships. For example, Deater-

Deckard and O’Connor (2000),
studying identical and fraternal
twins, and biological and adoptive
siblings, found that parent-child
MRO was driven, in part, by the
child’s genetically based qualities.
In addition, a child’s biologically
based traits, such as being prone to
anger or joy, or being hard or easy
to soothe, may facilitate or under-
mine the evolution of the child’s re-
lationships within particular dyads.
Being responsive to and having en-
joyable interactions with a child
may be more challenging if the
child is temperamentally difficult
than if he or she is easygoing and
mellow.

Mothers’ traits, some also bio-
logically based, may be important
as well. We have found that the
more empathic mothers are, the
better able they are to form MRO
with their children (Kochanska,
1997). A large body of research in-
dicates that depression and high
levels of negative emotion in moth-
ers reduce their responsiveness
and positive behavior when inter-
acting with their young children.

M o r e c o m p l e x i n t e r p l a y b e –
tween biological and relationship
factors also deserves future re-
search attention. Our findings indi-
cate that MRO may be particularly
beneficial for children with certain
temperaments, particularly fear-
less, thrill-seeking children whose
behavior is not easily modified by
actual or anticipated punishments
and threats. Other interactions be-
tween temperament and relation-
ships are also possible.

MRO as a Developmentally
Changing System

A mutually responsive relation-
ship between a parent and an in-
fant differs from a mutually re-
sponsive relationship between a
parent and a preschooler, or be-
tween a parent and an adolescent.
The contexts and currency of par-

ent-child interactions change. In in-
fancy, those contexts include mostly
the contexts of caregiving, play, and
daily routines, and the currency of
exchange is often nonverbal. Grad-
ually, the contexts expand to in-
clude parent-child discussions of
events and ideas, and the exchanges
are increasingly verbal (Laible &
Thompson, 2000). The child’s and
the parent’s relative contributions
to the relationship change over time,
and so do their cognitive represen-
tations, perceptions, and expecta-
tions of the relationship and of each
other. Psychologists’ understanding
of the child’s side of MRO lags con-
siderably behind their understand-
ing of the parent’s side of MRO. How
MRO can be assessed in a manner
that is developmentally sensitive
and yet captures stable qualities of
the parent-child dyad over time is
one of the future challenges.

MRO and Internal
Representations

In research to date, MRO has been
inferred from parents’ and chil-
dren’s observed behavior and af-
fect during interactions. This outer
layer, however, only partially cap-
tures the essence of a relationship.
Scholars studying relationships
have adopted Bowlby’s premise
that, over time, the parent and the
child gradually form inner repre-
sentations, or internal working
models, of their relationship (Col-
lins & Laursen, 1999). Those evolv-
ing models include generalized
memories of each other’s behavior,
implicit beliefs and feelings about
each other and the relationship,
and a sense of what the relation-
ship is like and what to expect
from one another. Those general-
ized products of an individual’s ex-
perience serve to organize and bias
his or her future information pro-
cessing, behavior, and emotions. In
the case of MRO, the parent’s and

Copyright © 2002 American Psychological Society

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 195

child’s internal models entail mu-
tual cooperation and implicit reci-
procity, and the child’s internal
model is thought to underlie his or
her willingness to embrace paren-
tal rules. Those inner representa-
tions, however, are difficult to access
and to study. To develop sensitive
yet rigorous methodologies that
will provide insights into the rep-
resentational aspect of MRO is an
important future challenge.

MRO and the Family System

The relationship between a par-
ent and child is itself nested in a
network of family relationships.
The importance of studying devel-
opment in the context of the entire
family system has been increas-
ingly acknowledged. In particular,
f u t u r e r e s e a r c h s h o u l d s t u d y
m o t h e r – c h i l d a n d f a t h e r – c h i l d
MRO, both separately and as a tri-
adic interconnected system. More
generally, family-level variables
such as stress, conflict, support,
and affective ambience may be sig-
nificant dimensions of the context
in which mutually responsive rela-
tionships with the child may flour-
ish or fail.

Recommended Reading

Collins, W.A., & Laursen, B. (Eds.).
(1999). (See References)

Kochanska, G. (1997). (See References)
K o c h a n s k a , G . , & M u r r a y , K . T .

(2000). (See References)

Acknowledgments—

This research has
been sponsored by grants from the Na-
tional Institute of Mental Health (RO1
MH63096, KO2 MH01446) and National
Science Foundation (DBS-9209559, SBR-
9510863) to the author. I gratefully ac-
knowledge the comments of Nazan Ak-
san, David Forman, and Robert Siegler,
and contributions of numerous students,
staff, and the families who participated in
the studies.

Note

1. Address correspondence to Gra-
zyna Kochanska, Department of Psy-
chology, University of Iowa, Iowa City,
IA 52242-1447.

References

Clark, M.S. (1984). Record keeping in two types of
relationships.

Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology

,

47

, 549–557.
Collins, W.A., & Laursen, B. (Eds.). (1999).

Minne-
sota Symposia on Child Psychology:

Vol.

30.

Rela-
tionships as developmental contexts.

Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.

Collins, W.A., Maccoby, E.E., Steinberg, L., Heth-
erington, E.M., & Bornstein, M.H. (2000). Con-
temporary research on parenting: The case for
nature and nurture.

American Psychologist

,

55

,
218–232.

Deater-Deckard, K., & O’Connor, T.G. (2000). Par-
ent-child mutuality in early childhood: Two
behavioral genetic studies.

Developmental Psy-
chology

,

36

, 561–570.

Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R.A. (1998). Prosocial de-
velopment. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N.
Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.),

Handbook of child psychol-
ogy: Vol. 3.

Social, emotional, and personality de-
velopment

(pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley.

Emde, R.N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R.B., & Oppen-
heim, D. (1991). The moral self of infancy: Af-
f e c t i v e c o r e a n d p r o c e d u r a l k n o w l e d g e .

Developmental Review

,

11

, 251–270.

Forman, D.R., & Kochanska, G. (2001). Viewing im-
itation as child responsiveness: A link between
teaching and discipline domains of socializa-

tion.

Developmental Psychology

,

37

, 198–206.

Grusec, J.E. (1997). A history of research on
parenting strategies and children’s internaliza-
tion of values. In J.E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski
(Eds.),

Parenting and children’s internalization of

values: A handbook of contemporary theory

(pp.
3–22). New York: Wiley.

Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orien-
tation between mothers and their young chil-
dren: Implications for early socialization.

Child
Development

,

68

, 94–112.

Kochanska, G., Coy, K.C., & Murray, K.T. (2001).
The development of self-regulation in the first
four years of life.

Child Development

,

72

, 1091–
1111.

Kochanska, G., Forman, G., & Coy, K.C. (1999).
Implications of the mother-child relationship
in infancy for socialization in the second year
of life.

Infant Behavior and Development

,

22

, 249–
265.

Kochanska, G., & Murray, K.T. (2000). Mother-
child mutually responsive orientation and con-
science development: From toddler to early

school age.

Child Development

,

71

, 417–431.

Laible, D.J., & Thompson, R.A. (2000). Mother-
child discourse, attachment security, shared
positive affect, and early conscience develop-
ment.

Child Development

,

71

, 1424–1440.

Maccoby, E.E. (1999). The uniqueness of the par-
ent-child relationship. In W.A. Collins & B.
Laursen (Eds.),

Minnesota Symposia on Child
Psychology:

Vol.

30. Relationships as developmen-
tal contexts

(pp. 157–175). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
baum.

Reis, H.T., Collins, W.A., & Berscheid, E. (2000).
Relationships in human behavior and devel-
opment.

Psychological Bulletin

,

126

, 844–872.

What Will You Get?

We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

Premium Quality

Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

Experienced Writers

Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

On-Time Delivery

Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

24/7 Customer Support

Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

Complete Confidentiality

Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

Authentic Sources

We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

Moneyback Guarantee

Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

Order Tracking

You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

image

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

image

Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

Preferred Writer

Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

Grammar Check Report

Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

One Page Summary

You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

Plagiarism Report

You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

Free Features $66FREE

  • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
  • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
  • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
  • Paper Formatting $05FREE
  • Cover Page $05FREE
  • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
  • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
  • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
  • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
image

Our Services

Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

  • On-time Delivery
  • 24/7 Order Tracking
  • Access to Authentic Sources
Academic Writing

We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

Professional Editing

We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

Thorough Proofreading

We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

image

Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

Check Out Our Sample Work

Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

Categories
All samples
Essay (any type)
Essay (any type)
The Value of a Nursing Degree
Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
Nursing
2
View this sample

It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

0+

Happy Clients

0+

Words Written This Week

0+

Ongoing Orders

0%

Customer Satisfaction Rate
image

Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

image

We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

  • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
  • Customized writing as per your needs.

We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

  • Proactive analysis of your writing.
  • Active communication to understand requirements.
image
image

We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

  • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
  • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
Place an Order Start Chat Now
image

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy