Discussion: Memory and Learning

Within 250-300 words for each please elaborate:

Topic 3 DQ 1:

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Discussion: Memory and Learning
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

·  See attachment to refer to this reading

Topic 3 DQ 2:

Neuro

n

NeuroView

  • The Legacy of Patient H.M. for Neuroscience
  • Larry R. Squire1,2,*
    1Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, San Diego, CA 92161, USA
    2Departments of Psychiatry, Neurosciences, and Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
    *Correspondence: lsquire@ucsd.edu
    DOI 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023

    H.M. is probably the best known single patient in the history of neuroscience. His severe memory impairment,
    which resulted from experimental neurosurgery to control seizures, was the subject of study for five decades
    until his death in December 2008. Work with H.M. established fundamental principles about how memory
    functions are organized in the brain.

    In 1952, Brenda Milner was completing

    her doctoral research at McGill University

    under the direction of Donald Hebb. At

    about this time, she encountered two

    patients (P.B. and F.C.) who had become

    severely amnesic following unilateral

    removal of the medial structures of the

    left temporal lobe for the treatment of

    epileptic seizures (Penfield and Milner,

    1958). This unfortunate outcome was

    entirely unexpected, and it was proposed

    that in each case there had been a preex-

    istent, but unsuspected, atrophic lesion in

    the medial temporal lobe of the opposite

    hemisphere. In that way, the unilateral

    surgery would have resulted in a bilateral

    lesion, an idea that was confirmed at

    autopsy some years later for patient P.B.

    After the two cases were presented at

    the 1955 meeting of the American Neuro-

    logical Association, Wilder Penfield (the

    neurosurgeon in both cases) received

    a call from William Scoville, a neurosur-

    geon in Hartford, Connecticut. Scoville

    told Penfield that he had seen a similar

    memory impairment in one of his own

    patients (H.M.) in whom he had carried

    out a bilateral medial temporal lobe resec-

    tion in an attempt to control epileptic

    seizures. As a result of this conversation,

    Brenda Milner was invited to travel to

    Hartford to study H.M.

    H.M. had been knocked down by

    a bicycle at the age of 7, began to have

    minor seizures at age 10, and had major

    seizures after age 16. (The age of the

    bicycle accident is given as 9 in some

    reports; for clarification see Corkin,

    1984.) He worked for a time on an

    assembly line but, finally, in 1953 at the

    age of 27 he had become so incapaci-

    tated by his seizures, despite high doses

    6 Neuron 61, January 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsev

    of anticonvulsant medication, that he

    could not work or lead a normal life. Sco-

    ville offered H.M. an experimental proce-

    dure that he had carried out previously in

    psychotic patients, and the surgery was

    then performed with the approval of the

    patient and his family.

    When Milner first visited H.M., she saw

    that the epilepsy was now controlled but

    that his memory impairment was even

    more severe than in Penfield’s two

    patients, P.B. and F.C. What she

    observed was someone who forgot daily

    events nearly as fast as they occurred,

    apparently in the absence of any general

    intellectual loss or perceptual disorder.

    He underestimated his own age, apolo-

    gized for forgetting the names of persons

    to whom he had just been introduced, and

    described his state as ‘‘like waking from

    a dream . every day is alone in itself.’’
    (Milner et al., 1968, p. 217).

    The first observations of H.M., and the

    results of formal testing, were reported

    a few years later (Scoville and Milner,

    1957). This publication became one of

    the most cited papers in neuroscience

    (nearly 2500 citations) and is still cited

    with high frequency. H.M. continued to

    be studied for five decades, principally

    by Brenda Milner, her former student

    Suzanne Corkin, and their colleagues

    (Corkin, 1984, 2002; Milner et al., 1968).

    He died on December 2, 2008, at the

    age of 82. It can be said that the early

    descriptions of H.M. inaugurated the

    modern era of memory research. Before

    H.M., due particularly to the influence of

    Karl Lashley, memory functions were

    thought to be widely distributed in the

    cortex and to be integrated with

    intellectual and perceptual functions.

    ier Inc.

    The findings from H.M. established the

    fundamental principle that memory is

    a distinct cerebral function, separable

    from other perceptual and cognitive abili-

    ties, and identified the medial aspect of

    the temporal lobe as important for

    memory. The implication was that the

    brain has to some extent separated its

    perceptual and intellectual functions

    from its capacity to lay down in memory

    the records that ordinarily result from

    engaging in perceptual and intellectual

    work.

    The Medial Temporal Lobe Memory
    System
    The early paper is sometimes cited incor-

    rectly as evidence that the hippocampus

    is important for memory, but this partic-

    ular point could not of course be estab-

    lished from a lesion that, by the surgeon’s

    description, included the hippocampus,

    amygdala, and the adjacent parahippo-

    campal gyrus. As Milner subsequently

    wrote, ‘‘Despite the use of the word

    ‘hippocampal’ in the titles of my papers

    with Scoville and Penfield, I have never

    claimed that the memory loss was solely

    attributable to the hippocampal lesions’’

    (Milner, 1998). Indeed, the original paper

    ends, quite appropriately, with the state-

    ment:

    It is concluded that the anterior

    hippocampus and hippocampal

    gyrus, either separately or together,

    are critically concerned in the

    retention of current experience. It

    is not known whether the amygdala

    plays any part in this mechanism,

    since the hippocampal complex

    has not been removed alone, but

    mailto:lsquire@ucsd.edu

    Neuron

    NeuroView

    always together with uncus and

    amygdala. (Scoville and Milner,

    1957, p. 21).

    The findings from H.M. were initially

    met with some resistance, especially

    because of the difficulty for many years

    of demonstrating anything resembling

    his impairment in the experimental animal.

    Efforts to establish an animal model in fact

    began almost immediately when Scoville

    himself came to Montreal and did the

    same surgery in monkeys that he had

    done with H.M. But these monkeys and

    others with medial temporal lesions

    seemed able to learn tasks that H.M.

    could not learn. Only much later did it

    become understood that apparently

    similar tasks can be learned in different

    ways by humans and monkeys. For

    example, the visual discrimination task,

    which is learned gradually by the monkey

    over hundreds of trials, proved to involve

    what one would now call habit learning.

    In the monkey, this kind of learning

    depends on the basal ganglia, not the

    medial temporal lobe. Eventually, tasks

    were developed for the monkey that

    were exquisitely sensitive to medial

    temporal lobe lesions (for example, the

    one-trial, delayed nonmatching to sample

    task), and an animal model of human

    memory impairment thereby became

    available (Mishkin, 1978).

    Cumulative work with the animal model

    over the next decade, together with

    neuroanatomical studies, succeeded in

    identifying the anatomical components

    of what is now termed the medial

    temporal lobe memory system (Squire

    and Zola-Morgan, 1991): the hippo-

    campus and the adjacent perirhinal, ento-

    rhinal, and parahippocampal cortices that

    make up much of the parahippocampal

    gyrus. This information showed which

    structures within H.M.’s large lesion

    were important for understanding his

    impairment and, more broadly, what

    structures are important for memory.

    A few years later, an improved description

    of H.M.’s lesion was obtained with

    magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Cor-

    kin et al., 1997). MRI had been delayed

    because of concerns that clips placed

    on the dura during surgery made H.M.

    ineligible for imaging. However, thorough

    inquiry revealed that the dural clips

    constituted no risk.

    At this juncture, several points became

    clear. First, H.M.’s lesion was less

    extensive than described originally by the

    surgeon in that it extended a little more

    than 5 cm caudally from the temporal

    pole (not 8 cm). As a result the posterior

    parahippocampal gyrus was largely

    spared (specifically, the parahippocampal

    cortex or what in the monkey is termed

    area TH TF). Second, the reason that

    H.M.’s memory impairment was so severe

    was that the bilateral damage included the

    parahippocampal gyrus (anteriorly) and

    was not restricted to the hippocampus.

    Damage limited to the hippocampus

    causes significant memory impairment

    but considerably less impairment than in

    H.M. Third, memory impairment more

    severe than H.M.’s could now be under-

    stood, as when the damage includes the

    structures damaged in H.M. but also

    extends far enough posteriorly to involve

    the parahippocampal cortex (patients

    E.P. and G.P.; Kirwan et al., 2008).

    In the early years, the anatomy of the

    medial temporal lobe was poorly under-

    stood, and terms like hippocampal zone

    and hippocampal complex were often

    used to identify the area of damage. With

    the elucidation of the boundaries and

    connectivity of the structures adjacent to

    the hippocampus and the discovery that

    these structures are important for

    memory, vague terms like hippocampal

    complex became unnecessary (though

    one can still find them in contemporary

    writing). It is now possible to achieve care-

    ful descriptions based on anatomical

    measurement and modern terminology.

    H.M. not only motivated the develop-

    ment of an animal model of human

    memory impairment and the subsequent

    delineation of the medial temporal lobe

    memory system. As described next, the

    study of H.M. also led to fundamental

    insights into the function of the medial

    temporal lobe and the larger matter of

    how memory is organized in the brain.

    Immediate Memory and Long-Term
    Memory
    H.M.’s intact intellectual and perceptual

    functions, and similar findings in other

    patients with large medial temporal

    lesions, have been well documented.

    A key additional finding was that H.M.

    had a remarkable capacity for sustained

    attention, including the ability to retain

    Neuron

    information for a period of time after it

    was presented. Thus, he could carry on

    a conversation, and he exhibited an intact

    digit span (i.e., the ability to repeat back

    a string of six or seven digits). Indeed,

    information remained available so long

    as it could be actively maintained by

    rehearsal. For example, H.M. could retain

    a three-digit number for as long as 15 min

    by continuous rehearsal, organizing the

    digits according to an elaborate

    mnemonic scheme. Yet when his atten-

    tion was diverted to a new topic, he forgot

    the whole event. In contrast, when the

    material was not easy to rehearse (in the

    case of nonverbal stimuli like faces or

    designs), information slipped away in

    less than a minute. These findings sup-

    ported a fundamental distinction between

    immediate memory and long-term

    memory (what William James termed

    primary memory and secondary memory).

    Primary memory [immediate memory]

    .comes to us as belonging to the
    rearward portion of the present

    space of time, and not to the

    genuine past (James, 1890, p. 647).

    Secondary memory [long-term memory] is

    quite different.

    An object which has been recol-

    lected . is one which has been
    absent from consciousness alto-

    gether, and now revives anew. It is

    brought back, recalled, fished up,

    so to speak, from a reservoir in

    which, with countless other

    objects, it lay buried and lost from

    view. (James, 1890, p. 648).

    Notably, time is not the key factor that

    determines how long patients like H.M.

    can retain information in memory. The rele-

    vant factors are the capacity of immediate

    memory and attention, i.e., the amount of

    material that can be held in mind and

    how successfully it can be rehearsed.

    The work with H.M. demonstrated that

    the psychological distinction between

    immediate memory and long-term

    memory is a prominent feature of how the

    brain has organized its memory functions.

    Multiple Memory Systems
    Perhaps the most unexpected discovery

    about H.M., given his profound and global

    61, January 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 7

    Neuron
    NeuroView

    memory impairment, came when Brenda

    Milner tested his ability to acquire a visuo-

    motor skill (Milner, 1962). H.M. was shown

    a five-pointed star, with a double contour,

    and asked to trace its outline with a pencil,

    but in a condition when he could only

    see his hand and the star as reflected

    in a mirror. H.M. acquired this mirror-

    drawing skill during ten trials and

    exhibited excellent retention across 3

    days. Yet at the end of testing, he had

    no recollection of having done the task

    before. This demonstration provided the

    first hint that there was more than one

    kind of memory in the brain and sug-

    gested that some kinds of memory (motor

    skills) must lie outside the province of the

    medial temporal lobe.

    For a time, it was rather thought that

    motor skills were a special case and that

    all the rest of memory is impaired in

    patients like H.M. Later it became appre-

    ciated that motor skills are but a subset

    of a larger domain of skill-like abilities, all

    of which are preserved in amnesia. The

    demonstration of a fully preserved ability

    to learn the perceptual skill of mirror

    reading suggested a distinction between

    two broad classes of knowledge: declara-

    tive and procedural (Cohen and Squire,

    1980). Declarative memory is what is

    meant when the term ‘‘memory’’ is used

    in everyday language, i.e., conscious

    knowledge of facts and events. Proce-

    dural memory refers to skill-based knowl-

    edge that develops gradually but with little

    ability to report what is being learned.

    In the years that followed, other

    preserved learning abilities began to be

    reported for amnesic patients, and the

    perspective shifted to a framework that

    accommodated multiple (i.e., more than

    two) memory systems. As Endel Tulving

    wrote:

    But even if we accept the broad

    division of memory into procedural

    and propositional forms . there
    are phenomena that do not seem

    to fit readily into such a taxonomy

    (Tulving et al., 1982, p.336).

    Subsequently, the terms declarative

    and nondeclarative were introduced with

    the idea that declarative memory refers

    to the kind of memory that is impaired in

    H.M. and is dependent on the medial

    temporal lobe. Nondeclarative memory

    8 Neuron 61, January 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsev

    is an umbrella term referring to additional

    memory systems. These include systems

    that support skill learning, habit learning,

    simple conditioning, emotional learning,

    as well as priming and perceptual

    learning. The structures with special

    importance for these kinds of memory

    include the basal ganglia, the cerebellum,

    the amygdala, and the neocortex. The

    starting point for these developments

    was the early discovery that motor skill

    learning was preserved in H.M. This

    finding revealed that memory is not

    a single faculty of the mind and led ulti-

    mately to the identification of the multiple

    memory systems of the mammalian brain.

    Remote Memory
    H.M.’s memory impairment has generally

    been taken as reflecting a failure to convert

    transient, immediate memory into stable

    long-term memory. A key insight about

    the organization of memory, and medial

    temporal lobe function, came with

    a consideration of his capacity to

    remember information that he had

    acquired before his surgery. The first

    exploration of this issue with formal tests

    asked H.M. to recognize faces of persons

    who had become famous in different

    decades, 1920–1970 (Marslen-Wilson

    and Teuber, 1975). As expected, H.M.

    was severely impaired at recognizing faces

    from his postmorbid period (the 1950s and

    1960s), but he performed as well as or

    better than age-matched controls at

    recognizing faces of persons who were in

    the news before his surgery. This important

    finding implied that the medial temporal

    lobe is not the ultimate storage site for

    previously acquired knowledge. The early

    descriptions of H.M. conform to this view.

    Thus, H.M. was described as having

    a partial loss of memory (retrograde

    amnesia) for the 3 years leading up to his

    surgery, with early memories ‘‘seemingly

    normal’’ (Scoville and Milner, 1957, p. 17).

    Similarly, about 10 years later it was

    remarked that there did not appear

    to have been any change in H.M.’s

    capacity to recall remote events

    antedating his operation, such as

    incidents from his early school

    years, a high-school attachment,

    or jobs he had held in his late teens

    and early twenties (Milner et al.,

    1968, p. 216).

    ier Inc.

    Subsequently, a particular interest

    developed in the status of autobiograph-

    ical memories for unique events, which

    are specific to time and place, and

    methods were developed to assess the

    specificity and the detail with which such

    recollections could be reproduced. In

    the earliest efforts along these lines, as

    summarized by Suzanne Corkin (Corkin,

    1984), H.M. produced well-formed auto-

    biographical memories, from age 16 years

    or younger. It was concluded that H.M’s

    remote memory impairment now

    extended back to 11 years before his

    surgery. The situation seemed to change

    further as H.M. aged. In an update

    prepared nearly 20 years later (Corkin,

    2002), H.M. (now 76 years old) was

    described as having memories of child-

    hood, but his memories appeared more

    like remembered facts than like memories

    of specific episodes. It was also said that

    he could not narrate a single event that

    occurred at a specific time and place.

    Essentially the same conclusion was

    reached a few years later when new

    methods, intended to be particularly

    sensitive, were used to assess H.M.’s

    remote memory for autobiographical

    events (Steinvorth et al., 2005). These

    later findings led to the proposal that,

    whatever might be the case for fact

    memory, autobiographical memories,

    i.e., memories that are specific to time

    and place, depend on the medial temporal

    lobe so long as the memories persist.

    There are reasons to be cautious about

    this idea. In 2002–2003, new MRI scans of

    H.M. were obtained (Salat et al., 2006).

    These scans documented a number of

    changes since his first MRI scans from

    1992–1993 (Corkin et al., 1997), including

    cortical thinning, subcortical atrophy,

    large amounts of abnormal white matter,

    and subcortical infarcts. These findings

    were thought to have appeared during

    the past decade, and they complicate

    the interpretation of neuropsychological

    data collected during the same time

    period. Another consideration is that

    remote memories could have been intact

    in the early years after surgery but then

    have faded with time because they could

    not be strengthened through rehearsal

    and relearning. In any case, the optimal

    time to assess the status of past memory

    is soon after the onset of memory

    impairment.

    Neuron
    NeuroView

    Other work has tended to support the

    earlier estimates that H.M.’s remote

    memories were intact. First, Penfield’s

    two patients described above, P.B. and

    F.C., were reported after their surgeries

    to have memory loss extending back

    a few months and 4 years, respectively,

    and intact memory from before that time

    (Penfield and Milner, 1958). Second,

    methods like those used recently to

    assess H.M. have also been used to eval-

    uate autobiographical memory in other

    patients, including patients like E.P. and

    G.P. who have very severe memory

    impairment (Kirwan et al., 2008). In these

    cases, autobiographical recollection was

    impaired when memories were drawn

    from the recent past but fully intact when

    memories were drawn from the remote

    past.

    Memory loss can sometimes extend

    back for decades in the case of large

    medial temporal lobe lesions (though

    additional damage to anterolateral

    temporal cortex may be important in this

    circumstance). In any case, memories

    from early life appear to be intact unless

    the damage extends well into the lateral

    temporal lobe or the frontal lobe. These

    findings are typically interpreted to mean

    that the structures damaged in H.M. are

    important for the formation of long-term

    memory and its maintenance for a period

    of time after learning. During this period

    gradual changes are thought to occur in

    neocortex (memory consolidation) that

    increase the complexity, distribution,

    and connectivity among multiple cortical

    regions. Eventually, memory can be sup-

    ported by the neocortex and becomes

    independent of the medial temporal

    lobe. The surprising observation that

    H.M. had access to old memories, in the

    face of an inability to establish new

    ones, motivated an enormous body of

    work, both in humans and experimental

    animals, on the topic of remote memory

    and continues to stimulate discussion

    about the nature and significance of retro-

    grade amnesia.

    Perspective
    H.M. was likely the most studied

    individual in the history of neuroscience.

    Interest in the case can be attributed to

    a number of factors, including the unusual

    purity and severity of the memory impair-

    ment, its stability, its well-described

    anatomical basis, and H.M.’s willingness

    to be studied. He was a quiet and cour-

    teous man with a sense of humor and

    insight into his condition. Speaking of his

    neurosurgeon, he once said, ‘‘What he

    learned about me helped others, and I’m

    glad about that.’’ (Corkin, 2002, p. 159).

    An additional aspect of H.M.’s circum-

    stance, which assured his eventual place

    in the history of neuroscience, was the

    fact that Brenda Milner was the young

    scientist who first studied him. She is

    a superb experimentalist with a strong

    conceptual orientation that allowed her

    to draw from her data deep insights about

    the organization of memory. Because he

    was the first well-studied patient with

    amnesia, H.M. became the yardstick

    against which other patients with memory

    impairment would be compared. It is now

    clear that his memory impairment was not

    absolute and that he was able to acquire

    significant new knowledge (Corkin,

    2002). Thus, memory impairment can be

    either more severe or less severe than in

    H.M. But the study of H.M. established

    key principles about how memory is orga-

    nized that continue to guide the discipline.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    Supported by the Medical Research Service of the
    Department of Veterans Affairs, The National Insti-
    tute of Mental Health (MH24600), and the Metro-

    Neuro

    politan Life Foundation. I thank Nicola Broadbent,
    Robert Clark, Christine Smith, Ryan Squire, and
    Wendy Suzuki for their helpful comments.

    REFERENCES

    Cohen, N., and Squire, L.R. (1980). Science 210,
    207–209.

    Corkin, S. (1984). Semin. Neurol. 4, 249–259.

    Corkin, S. (2002). Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 153–160.

    Corkin, S., Amaral, D.G., Gonzalez, R.G., Johnson,
    K.A., and Hyman, B.T. (1997). J. Neurosci. 17,
    3964–3979.

    James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology, Dover
    Edition, Volume One (New York: Holt).

    Kirwan, C.B., Bayley, P.J., Galván, V.V., and
    Squire, L.R. (2008). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
    105, 2676–2680.

    Marslen-Wilson, W.D., and Teuber, H.L. (1975).
    Neuropsychologia 13, 353–364.

    Milner, B. (1962). Physiologie de l’hippocampe,
    P. Passouant, ed. (Paris: Centre National de la
    Recherche Scientifique), pp. 257–272.

    Milner, B. (1998). The History of Neuroscience in
    Autobiography, Volume 2, L.R. Squire, ed. (San
    Diego: Academic Press), pp. 276–305.

    Milner, B., Corkin, S., and Teuber, H.L. (1968).
    Neuropsychologia 6, 215–234.

    Mishkin, M. (1978). Nature 273, 297–298.

    Penfield, W., and Milner, B. (1958). AMA Arch.
    Neurol. Psychiatry 79, 475–497.

    Salat, D.H., van der Kouwe, A.J.W., Tuch, D.S.,
    Quinn, B.T., Fischl, B., Dale, A.M., and Corkin, S.
    (2006). Hippocampus 16, 936–945.

    Scoville, W.B., and Milner, B. (1957). J. Neurol.
    Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11–21.

    Squire, L.R., and Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). Science
    253, 1380–1386.

    Steinvorth, S., Levine, B., and Corkin, S. (2005).
    Neuropsychologia 43, 479–496.

    Tulving, E., Schacter, D.L., and Stark, H.A. (1982).
    J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 8, 336–342.

    n 61, January 15, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Inc. 9

      The Legacy of Patient H.M. for Neuroscience
      The Medial Temporal Lobe Memory System
      Immediate Memory and Long-Term Memory
      Multiple Memory Systems
      Remote Memory
      Perspective
      Acknowledgments
      References

    What Will You Get?

    We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

    Premium Quality

    Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

    Experienced Writers

    Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

    On-Time Delivery

    Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

    24/7 Customer Support

    Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

    Complete Confidentiality

    Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

    Authentic Sources

    We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

    Moneyback Guarantee

    Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

    Order Tracking

    You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

    image

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    image

    Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

    From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

    Preferred Writer

    Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

    Grammar Check Report

    Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

    One Page Summary

    You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

    Plagiarism Report

    You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

    Free Features $66FREE

    • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
    • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
    • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
    • Paper Formatting $05FREE
    • Cover Page $05FREE
    • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
    • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
    • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
    • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
    image

    Our Services

    Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

    • On-time Delivery
    • 24/7 Order Tracking
    • Access to Authentic Sources
    Academic Writing

    We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

    Professional Editing

    We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

    Thorough Proofreading

    We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

    image

    Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

    Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

    Check Out Our Sample Work

    Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

    Categories
    All samples
    Essay (any type)
    Essay (any type)
    The Value of a Nursing Degree
    Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
    Nursing
    2
    View this sample

    It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

    Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

    0+

    Happy Clients

    0+

    Words Written This Week

    0+

    Ongoing Orders

    0%

    Customer Satisfaction Rate
    image

    Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

    We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

    See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

    image

    We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

    We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

    • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
    • Customized writing as per your needs.

    We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

    We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

    • Proactive analysis of your writing.
    • Active communication to understand requirements.
    image
    image

    We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

    We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

    • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
    • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
    Place an Order Start Chat Now
    image

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy