Article Review on Managing Boundaries and Multiple relationships/ Professional Competence and training in counseling

100% No plagiarism

Article Review Instructions

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Article Review on Managing Boundaries and Multiple relationships/ Professional Competence and training in counseling
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

You will write three article reviews and if you choose, one extra credit article review. You will select the article yourself by searching the UWA Library Databases. The article you choose should be a research article (has a hypothesis that is empirically tested). Pick an article relevant to a topic covered in the weekly readings. Each review is worth 20 points. The review should be 1-2 single-spaced pages in a 12-point font. It is in your best interest to submit your review before it is due so you may check your originality report and correct any spelling and grammatical errors identified by the software program. 

The purpose of the review is to provide students knowledge of how research is conducted and reported. The main part of your review needs to include the following information. Please comment on these aspects of the article as part of your review. Provide only the briefest summary of content. What I am most interested in is your critique and connection to weekly readings. 

Reference. Listed at the top of the paper in APA style. 

Introduction. Read the introduction carefully. The introduction should contain:

· A thorough literature review that establishes the nature of the problem to be addressed in the present study (the literature review is specific to the problem)

· The literature review is current (generally, articles within the past 5 years)

· A logical sequence from what we know (the literature review) to what we don’t know (the unanswered questions raised by the review and what this study intended to answer

· The purpose of the present study

· The specific hypotheses/research questions to be addressed. 

  • State the overall purpose of the paper. What was the main      theme of the paper? 
  • What      new ideas or information were communicated in the paper? 
  • Why was it important to publish these ideas? 

Methods. The methods section has three subsections. The methods sections should contain: 

· The participants and the population they are intended to represent (are they described as well in terms of relevant demographic characteristics such as age, gender, ethnicity, education level, income level, etc?).

· The number of participants and how the participants were selected for the study

· A description of the tools/measures used and research design employed. 

· A detailed description of the procedures of the study including participant instructions and whether incentives were given.

Results. The results section should contain a very thorough summary of results of all analyses. This section should include: 

· Specific demographic characteristics of the sample

· A thorough narrative description of the results of all statistical tests that addressed specific hypotheses

· If there are tables and figures, are they also described in the text?

· If there are tables and figures, can they be interpreted “stand alone” (this means that they contain sufficient information in the title and footnotes so that a reader can understand what is being presented without having to go back to the text)?

Discussion. The discussion is where the author “wraps up the research”. This section should include: 

· A simple and easy to understand summary of what was found

· Where the hypotheses supported or refuted?

· A discussion of how the author’s findings compares to those found in prior research

· The limitations of the study

· The implications of the findings to basic and applied researchers and to practitioners 

Critique.

In your opinion, what were the strengths and weaknesses of the paper or document? Be sure to think about your impressions and the reasons for them. Listing what the author wrote as limitations is not the same thing as forming your own opinions and justifying them to the reader. 

· Were the findings important to a reader? 

· Were the conclusions valid? Do you agree with the conclusions? 

· If the material was technical, was the technical material innovative? 

Conclusion.

Once you provide the main critique of the article, you should include a final paragraph that gives me your overall impression of the study. Was the study worthwhile? Was it well-written and clear to those who may not have as much background in the content area? What was the overall contribution of this study to our child development knowledge base?  

APA Format Review

If you are unfamiliar or a bit “rusty” on your APA format, you may want to use the tutorial available through the APA website which is listed on your syllabus. 

Grading Criteria

I will grade your paper based upon:

· How well you followed directions (as indicated in this page) 

· How thoroughly you used examples to support the critique 

· How accurately you used APA format 

· your organization, grammar, and spelling 

· Integration of assigned weekly readings 

Article Review Instructions

You will write three article reviews and if you choose, one extra credit article review. You will select the article yourself by searching the UWA Library Databases. The article you choose should be a research article (has a hypothesis that is empirically tested). Pick an article relevant to a topic covered in the weekly readings. Each review is worth 20 points. The review should be 1-2 single-spaced pages in a 12-point font. It is in your best interest to submit your review before it is due so you may check your originality report and correct any spelling and grammatical errors identified by the software program.
The purpose of the review is to provide students knowledge of how research is conducted and reported. The main part of your review needs to include the following information. Please comment on these aspects of the article as part of your review. Provide only the briefest summary of content. What I am most interested in is your critique and connection to weekly readings.
Reference. Listed at the top of the paper in APA style.
Introduction. Read the introduction carefully. The introduction should contain:
· A thorough literature review that establishes the nature of the problem to be addressed in the present study (the literature review is specific to the problem)
· The literature review is current (generally, articles within the past 5 years)
· A logical sequence from what we know (the literature review) to what we don’t know (the unanswered questions raised by the review and what this study intended to answer
· The purpose of the present study
· The specific hypotheses/research questions to be addressed.
· State the overall purpose of the paper. What was the main theme of the paper?
· What new ideas or information were communicated in the paper?
· Why was it important to publish these ideas?
Methods. The methods section has three subsections. The methods sections should contain:
· The participants and the population they are intended to represent (are they described as well in terms of relevant demographic characteristics such as age, gender, ethnicity, education level, income level, etc?).
· The number of participants and how the participants were selected for the study
· A description of the tools/measures used and research design employed.
· A detailed description of the procedures of the study including participant instructions and whether incentives were given.
Results. The results section should contain a very thorough summary of results of all analyses. This section should include:
· Specific demographic characteristics of the sample
· A thorough narrative description of the results of all statistical tests that addressed specific hypotheses
· If there are tables and figures, are they also described in the text?
· If there are tables and figures, can they be interpreted “stand alone” (this means that they contain sufficient information in the title and footnotes so that a reader can understand what is being presented without having to go back to the text)?
Discussion. The discussion is where the author “wraps up the research”. This section should include:
· A simple and easy to understand summary of what was found
· Where the hypotheses supported or refuted?
· A discussion of how the author’s findings compares to those found in prior research
· The limitations of the study
· The implications of the findings to basic and applied researchers and to practitioners

Critique.
In your opinion, what were the strengths and weaknesses of the paper or document? Be sure to think about your impressions and the reasons for them. Listing what the author wrote as limitations is not the same thing as forming your own opinions and justifying them to the reader.
· Were the findings important to a reader?
· Were the conclusions valid? Do you agree with the conclusions?
· If the material was technical, was the technical material innovative?

Conclusion.
Once you provide the main critique of the article, you should include a final paragraph that gives me your overall impression of the study. Was the study worthwhile? Was it well-written and clear to those who may not have as much background in the content area? What was the overall contribution of this study to our child development knowledge base?  
 

APA Format Review

 
If you are unfamiliar or a bit “rusty” on your APA format, you may want to use the tutorial available through the APA website which is listed on your syllabus.
 
  
Grading Criteria

 
I will grade your paper based upon:
1. How well you followed directions (as indicated in this page)
1. How thoroughly you used examples to support the critique
1. How accurately you used APA format
1. your organization, grammar, and spelling
1. Integration of assigned weekly readings

Cultural Competence and Poverty:
Exploring Play Therapists’ Attitudes

Lauren Chase and Kristie Opiola
Department of Counseling, University of North Carolina at Charlotte

This article reports the findings of a survey that investigated attitudes toward poverty
among play therapists (N � 390) and its relation to demographic information. Multi-
variate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were used to measure the relationship
between play therapists’ demographics and their attitudes toward poverty, specifically
their structural, personal deficiency, and stigma scores. Results indicated that both
region and age resulted in differing views on povert

y.

Participants living in the
Northeast held stronger structural views of poverty than participants in the South.
Similarly, participants in the

50

–59 and 60 plus age groups disagreed to strongly
disagree with a personal explanation toward poverty than participants in the 30–39 age
group. The importance of play therapists’ examining their attitudes toward poverty and
the direct impact on their work is discussed. Finally, implications of the results,
including overall findings, are explained.

Keywords: play therapy, attitudes of poverty, cultural competence

Culturally competent training is an element
of credentialing requirements that ensures men-
tal health providers offer adequate and respon-
sive care to diverse populations. Although the
mental health field has embedded cultural com-
petence in their standards and guidelines, there
are discrepancies in the way the profession as-
sesses and measures competence (Sue et al.,
1996). Researchers have investigated attitudes
toward poverty in the helping professions
(Levin & Schwartz-Tayri, 2017; Noone et al.,
2012; van Heerde & Hudson, 2010; & Wit-
tenauer et al., 2015), but no study has focused
on play therapists’ attitudes toward povert

y.

The purpose of this study is to fill a gap in the
literature regarding play therapist’s attitudes to-
ward poverty because awareness and knowl-
edge are key elements to implement culturally
responsive services and skills with diverse chil-
dren in a variety of settings.

Cultural Competence

Cultural competence is an important compo-
nent of professional practice, and practitioners
are expected to develop skills and understand-
ing pertaining to diverse clientele. Researchers
define cultural competence as the set of beliefs,
knowledge, and skills mental health providers
possess in order to deliver effective interven-
tions and services to members of various cul-
tures (Gilbert et al., 2007; Sue, 2006). The New
Freedom Commission on Mental Health (2003)
recognized disparities in mental health delivery
and viewed the lack of cultural competence for
minority populations as a persistent problem.
Culturally competent health care is essential to
providing effective care to all populations. To
aid practitioners in their ability to increase their
cultural competence, leading professional men-
tal health associations have published profes-
sional cultural competency standards and ethi-
cal codes (American Counseling Association
[ACA], 2014; American Psychological Associ-
ation [APA], 2017a, 2017b; National Associa-
tion of Social Workers [NASW], 2015; Ratts et
al., 2015) to promote and guide clinical prac-
tice.

Lauren Chase X https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9811-0520
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-

dressed to Lauren Chase, Department of Counseling, Uni-
versity of North Carolina at Charlotte, 9201 University City
Boulevard, Charlotte, NC 28223, United States. Email:
lchase6@uncc.edu

T
hi

s
do

cu
m

en
t

is
co

py
ri

gh
te

d
by

th
e

A
m

er
ic

an
Ps

yc
ho

lo
gi

ca
l

A
ss

oc
ia

tio
n

or
on

e
of

its
al

lie
d

pu
bl

is
he

rs
.

T
hi

s
ar

tic
le

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r

th
e

pe
rs

on
al

us
e

of
th

e
in

di
vi

du
al

us
er

an
d

is
no

t
to

be
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
br

oa
dl

y.

International Journal of

Play Therapy

© 2021 Association for Play Therapy 2021, Vol. 30, No. 1, 50–60
ISSN: 1555-6824 https://doi.org/10.1037/pla0000144

50

https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9811-0520

mailto:lchase6@uncc.edu

https://doi.org/10.1037/pla0000144

Cultural competence is fundamental to effec-
tively building relationships with clients and
helping them achieve their mental health goals
(Capell et al., 2007). Sue et al. (1996) identified
three key components of cultural competence:
cultural awareness, cultural knowledge, and
cultural skill. Cultural awareness is founda-
tional and necessary for individuals to develop
culturally competent attitudes, knowledge, and
skills (Gilbert et al., 2007). Furthermore, cultur-
ally aware clinicians are individuals who are
sensitive to their personal values and biases and
acknowledge how their values and biases may
influence their view of the client, client’s prob-
lem, and counseling relationship (Sue, 2006;
Sue & Sue, 2016). Sue and Sue (2016) further
urged culturally competent mental health to
know beliefs, attitudes, principles, and world-
views common to the particular culture of peo-
ple they serve. Additionally, culturally skilled
clinicians benefit from reading relevant diverse
cultural research, pursue educational opportuni-
ties on diverse cultural skills, and involve them-
selves with diverse cultures outside of counsel-
ing relationships (Sue et al., 1992; Sue & Sue,
2016).

Sue and Sue (2016) advocated mental health
providers to assess their own cultural beliefs,
cultural values, and behaviors to increase their
self-awareness and cultural understanding to
decrease barriers in client-provider relation-
ships and increase comfort in working with
diverse clients. Mental health professionals gain
personal awareness when they are curious, re-
flective, vulnerable, and observant (Gilbert et
al., 2007). A lack of cultural awareness and
competence can have negative consequences
for the profession, as well as the clients. Siegel
et al. (2005) cautioned professionals that a lack
of cultural competence on their part may lead to
incorrect assessments and diagnoses and can
cause minority and marginalized populations,
such as clients with low socioeconomic status
and living in poverty, to underutilize services
and prematurely terminate care.

Poverty

Cultural competence includes working with
clients of diverse backgrounds, such as clients
in poverty. The culture of poverty can be a
controversial topic because some practitioners
believe people in poverty play a role in perpet-

uating causes of poverty and sustain the cycle
across generations (Cummins, 2018; Garrett,
2018). The culture of poverty is important in
understanding the concept of poverty because
human action is constrained and enabled by
how people define their actions. As the root of
poverty culture is a multifaceted concept and is
challenging to define, poverty culture dynamics
help researchers examine the cycle of poverty
and social inequality. Researchers and public
agencies often highlight personal and environ-
mental factors that contribute to the definition of
poverty, with a lack of income and resources
being the most defining factors (Ciment, 2013;
U.S. Census Bureau, 2018; World Bank Insti-
tute, 2005). People living in poverty often lack
material essentials, such as shelter, water, cloth-
ing, and food, which can impact one’s overall
well-being (Haughton & Khandker, 2009;
Wolff, 2019). The lack of essential material and
inability to establish daily living patterns and
activities lead individuals in poverty to feel
confined by their financial situation (Gordon &
Townsend, 2000). When society values finan-
cial wealth, people who meet the standard of
poverty may feel lesser than those with financial
stability; therefore, a lack of autonomy, educa-
tion, and self-worth are obstacles keeping many
in poverty.

Poverty rates can be surprisingly high in de-
veloped countries. The United States (US) is
one of the wealthiest nations in the world but
has one of the highest rates of poverty, with one
in eight Americans living in poverty (Poverty
Programs, 2017). While poverty affects people
of all ages and demographics, minority individ-
uals disproportionately exceed the national av-
erage (Koball & Jiang, 2018). Those at greatest
risk of living in poverty are families headed by
single mothers with children (U.S. Census Bu-
reau, 2018).

Growing up in poverty is a contributing fac-
tor to illness, disability, and adverse mental
health outcomes, which can have devastating
effects on children that can extend into adult-
hood (Moore et al., 2009). It appears the longer
a child lives in poverty, the more devastating
the impact (Wickham et al., 2014). Cooper and
Stewart (2013) completed a systemic review of
the literature on household income and health
outcomes and found family income makes a
significant impact on all areas of children’s de-
velopment and their level of academic achieve-

51CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

ment. They posited children living in poverty
have lower cognitive ability, lower social en-
gagement, greater behavioral problems, and
poorer health outcomes. Furthermore, poverty
impacts children’s participation in common
childhood activities, such as access to extracur-
ricular activities and peer social activities
(Turner, 2011). In addition, family poverty im-
pacts living and community environments, so-
cial supports, and parental mental health, which
can impact the level of stress children experi-
ence (Farthing, 2014; National Scientific Coun-
cil on the Developing Child, 2015; Turner &
Rawlings, 2005). High levels of stress can have
detrimental impacts on children’s holistic de-
velopment (National Scientific Council on the
Developing Child, 2015) and increases the risk
of children developing mental health and be-
havioral problems (Van Allen & Sterling,
2011).

Poverty Attitudes

Attitude is an important predictor of how a
mental health professional behaves toward cli-
ents and effectively treat clients (Sturm, 2008).
Researchers have concluded that attitudes are
related to behavior, especially when the atti-
tudes are held with a high degree of assurity, are
constant, available, and formed by direct expe-
riences (Appelbaum et al., 2006; Kraus, 1995;
Morrow & Deidan, 1992; Shapiro, 2004). There
are competing explanations for the rationality
for one’s attitudes toward poverty (Brady, 2019;
Hunt & Bullock, 2016; Jordan, 2004) with two
overarching themes: structural cause of poverty
or personal causes of poverty (Beeghley, 1988;
Feagin, 1975; Howard et al., 2017).

Structural causes of poverty include the be-
lief that poverty is caused by low government
investment in education, health, and employ-
ment opportunities (Beeghley, 1988; Davids &
Gouws, 2013; Feagin, 1975; Goldsmith &
Blakely, 2010; Royce, 2018; Weiss-Gal et al.,
2009). Structural causes focus on how society
limits one’s opportunities through low wages,
lack of education, and lack of affordable health
care (Wittenauer et al., 2015). Society is blamed
as a cause in the structural view (Noone et al.,
2012), and a person is not responsible or in
control of the cause of poverty. In contrast,
personal causes of poverty are viewed as exhib-
iting low motivation, possessing passive or lazy

characteristics, or growing up in a “poverty
culture” (Levin & Schwartz-Tayri, 2017; Mead,
2011; Royce, 2018). Personal causes focus on
the person’s inability to pull themselves out of
poverty and attribute causes to negative money
management or health behaviors. According to
Bray and Schommer-Aikins (2015), personal
causes of poverty imply that people are respon-
sible and have sufficient chances to succeed if
they work hard; therefore, people are viewed as
being in control of their own destiny.

Mental health providers’ attitudes toward
poverty have not been thoroughly examined in
the research. Previous research has focused on a
single region of the country or a specific popu-
lation. Three specific studies explored helping
professionals’ view of poverty: human service
practitioners (Anderson, 2018), school counsel-
ors (Ricks, 2014), and social workers (Weiss-
Gal et al., 2009). Weiss-Gal et al. (2009) studied
the views of social workers and service users in
the midwestern United States, and they con-
cluded participants held comparable levels of
understanding for motivational and psycholog-
ical causes. Furthermore, they found service
users credited more significance to social/
structural causes and fatalistic causes compared
to the social workers in the study. Additionally,
Ricks (2014) examined school counselors’ atti-
tudes toward poverty and found school counsel-
ors living in the southeastern United States held
individualistic attitudes toward poverty and at-
tributed poverty to fatalistic causes. Further-
more, Anderson (2018) interviewed human ser-
vice practitioners from the southern United
States on their view of poverty, and he found
human service practitioners held both structural
and individualistic views of poverty. Overall,
the exploration of poverty attitudes in mental
health providers is sparse and more studies are
needed to improve cultural competence.

Play Therapy

Play therapists represent diverse professional
licensures, including counseling, social work,
psychology, and marriage and family therapy
(Siu, 2010). The Association for Play Therapy
(APT) addresses the importance of diversity in
their mission statement, credentialing guide-
lines and documents but lacks specific cultural
competency guidelines. Currently APT (2019)
refers to multicultural competence as the pro-

52 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

cess of play therapists becoming aware of one’s
own culture and biases. For play therapists,
instruction occurs in a variety of settings, such
as university classrooms, professional confer-
ences, and regional trainings. The variety of
educational backgrounds, clinical training, and
supervised experience acquired by Registered
Play Therapist/Supervisors (RPT/S) and School
Based-Registered Play Therapists (SB-RPT)
generates unique perspectives to play therapy
and creates service delivery options to clients
from diverse backgrounds, such as poverty.

Play therapists striving to increase multicultural
competence can work with children in poverty
more effectively by becoming aware of their own
biases toward poverty and learning culturally re-
sponsive skills to respond to needs of children in
poverty. By exploring biases and beliefs about
clients from low socioeconomic status and pov-
erty, play therapists can obtain knowledge to de-
velop active cultural competence (Gil, 2005). Play
therapists obtain knowledge when they gain infor-
mation about their attitudes toward poverty to
provide more responsive services. Therefore, to
provide high quality services, play therapists need
to explore their beliefs to best attend to the needs
of children and their families who are experienc-
ing poverty. In addition, the field of play therapy
can benefit from exploring play therapists’ atti-
tudes on poverty as a means of identifying areas
where further instruction and exposure are needed.

Specifically, the researchers were interested in
exploring participant’s overall attitudes toward
poverty and the relationship between participant’s
demographic information and their scores on the
Attitudes Toward Poverty-Short Form (ATP-SF).
The demographic characteristics were age, gen-
der, race, region, highest educational degree, pro-
fessional discipline, and years of experience. The
research questions were (a) What are play thera-
pists’ attitudes toward poverty? (b) Are there dif-
ferences in ATP subscales (personal deficiency,
stigma, structural perspective, and total) based on
play therapist demographic characteristics?

Method

Procedure

Participants were recruited from the APT mail-
ing list, Counselor Education and Supervision
Network (CESNET) listserv, and invited mem-
bers of four play therapy-related Facebook groups.

Participants in the APT mailing list were recruited
via email invitation to the online SurveyShare
questionnaire. Participants in the play therapy-
related Facebook groups and CESNET Listserv
were invited to participate from the first author
posting in respective groups and Listserv. Upon
entry to the survey, respondents first read the
informed consent form approved by the universi-
ty’s institutional review board. Participants either
accepted or rejected participation in the study. If
respondents accepted, they were directed to the
survey items. Participants completed the ATP-SF
and a demographic survey, and they could access
the survey only by using the unique email invita-
tion sent to them. Participants utilized email ad-
dresses to sign into SurveyShare, which allowed
the researchers to control for one code per person.

Participants

Initially, over 5,000 invitations to participate
were sent over one month, and 391 survey pack-
ets were received. After removing one incom-
plete survey, the final sample size for data anal-
ysis was 390. The sample size for the present
study was deemed sufficient, based on exceed-
ing Cohen’s (1988) and Tabachnick and Fi-
dell’s (2013) recommendations for the minimal
sample sizes needed to achieve adequate power
(.80) at a significance level of p � .05 (i.e., 120
and 113, respectively). Specific to cultural de-
mographics, participants’ ages ranged from 23
to 78 years (M � 46.13, SD � 11.94). In
addition, 367 participants identified as female
(94.1%), 18 as male (4.6%), and five identified
as other (1.3%). Ethnoculturally, 321 partici-
pants identified as White (82.3%), 18 as African
American (4.6%), 25 as Hispanic/Latinx
(6.4%), and 26 as other cultures (6.7%).

Participants identified as play therapists or
play therapy trainees from multiple disciplines:
mental health (n � 181, 46.4%), social workers
(n � 95, 24.4%), marriage and family therapists
(n � 46, 11.8%), and other (e.g., psychologist,
international professionals; n � 68, 17.5%). In
terms of regions of the country, the profession-
als lived in the following regions: Northeast
(n � 42, 10.8%), Midwest (n � 85, 21.8%),
South (n � 154, 39.5%), West (n � 88, 22.6%),
or internationally (n � 21, 5.4%). Play thera-
pists’ experience levels included: 0–2 years
(n � 48, 12.3%), 3–5 years (n � 79, 20.3%),

53CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

6–9 years (n � 80, 20.5%), 10–13 years (n �
51, 13.1%), and 14 plus years (n � 132, 33.8%).

Instrument

The Attitudes Toward Poverty Scale-Short
Form (ATP-SF; Yun & Weaver, 2010) is a
self-report instrument based on the Attitudes
Toward Poverty Scale (ATP; Atherton et al.,
1993). The ATP-SF is a multidimensional scale
measuring attitudes toward poverty, leaning to-
ward a structural cause or individual deficit
cause. Each item is based on a 5-point Likert
scale (1 � strongly disagree to 5 � strongly
agree). An example of a structural cause-item
on the ATP-SF is, “People are poor due to
circumstances beyond their control.” An indi-
vidual deficit-item is, “Poor people are dishon-
est,” and a stigma-item is “Welfare makes peo-
ple lazy.” High scores indicate a belief that
structural determinants are the primary causes
of poverty while low scores indicate a personal
explanation of poverty (Atherton et al., 1993).
The Total score can range from 21 to 105.

The ATP-SF includes three subfactors: Struc-
tural Perspective, Personal Deficiency, and
Stigma. Structural causes of poverty include the
belief that poverty is caused by low government
investment in education, health, and employ-
ment opportunities (Beeghley, 1988; Feagin,
1975; Weiss-Gal et al., 2009). The Structural
Perspective subscale ranges from 6 to 30. Per-
sonal causes focus on the person’s inability to
pull themselves out of poverty by managing
money poorly or poor health behaviors. The
Personal Deficiency subscale ranges from 7 to
35. Stereotyping people in poverty adds to the
stigma of poverty. The subscale of Stigma is
important because members of marginalized
groups, such as those in poverty, are at a higher
likelihood of being stigmatized (Major &
O’Brien, 2005). Members of groups with less
power often evoke negative responses from
those with more power for those in power to
keep their position (Jost & Banaji, 1994). The
Stigma subscale ranges from 8 to 40. The
ATP-SF has a high level of internal consistency
with � � .87. All of the subscales of the
ATP-SF exceeded the minimum acceptable
level for internal consistency with all scales
being between .50 and .70.

Data Analysis

To examine the impact of play therapists’
demographics on their attitudes toward poverty,
a series of multivariate analyses of variances
(MANOVAs) were conducted. The independent
variables were (a) race, (b) age, (c) marital
status, (d) gender, (e) region, (f) setting, (g)
occupation, (h) years practicing play therapy,
and (i) RPT status, and the dependent variables
were (a) Personal Deficiency, (b) Stigma, (c)
Structural Perspective, and (d) Total Score. Be-
fore conducting the MANOVAs, the data were
screened for missing data, outliers, noncol-
linearity, equality of variance/covariance matri-
ces, and normality. Additionally, to examine
play therapist’s overall attitudes toward pov-
erty, researchers used average scores on the
ATP-SF Total, and Structural Perspective, Per-
sonal Deficiency, and Stigma subscales.

Results

The researchers strived to explore diverse
demographics. Due to the lack of diversity in
the sample, specifically regarding gender, race,
marital status, setting, occupation, RPT status,
and socioeconomic status, the researchers were
unable to explore potential differences. A large
majority of participants were white masters’
level, middle-class women. The average of the
Total score was 86.40 (SD � 10.63). The aver-
age Structural Perspective scale was 23.52
(SD � 3.94). The average of the Personal De-
ficiency scale was 30.32 (SD � 3.55). The
average of the Stigma scale found was 32.44
(SD � 5.53). Table 1 presents the mean scores
and standard deviations for the demographic
information for the ATP-SF Deficiency,
Stigma, Structural Perspective, and Total
scores.

Differences by Region and Age

A MANOVA was conducted to explore the
impact of geographic regions on attitudes to-
ward poverty. Participants were divided into
five regions (Northwest, Midwest, South, West,
and International). Using Pillai’s criterion, the
combined dependent variables were statistically
affected by the region, F(12, 1155) � 2.71, p �
.001. There was a statistically significant differ-
ence at the p � .05 level in Structural score

54 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

based on region (F(4, 385) � 2.184, p � .026),
but not in discipline and years of experience.
Post hoc comparison using Scheffé test indi-
cated that the South was significantly lower
(M � 23.21, SD � 3.87) than the Northeast
(M � 25.33, SD � 3.43) in regard to Structural
Perspective score (p � .046). Researchers did
not have statistically significant findings among
participants’ scores in the Midwest, West, and
International.

A MANOVA was conducted to explore the
impact of age on Total score and subscales of
Stigma, Structural Perspective, and Deficiency.
Using the Pillai’s criterion, the combined de-
pendent variables were statistically affected by
age (F(12, 1143) � 2.419, p � .004). There was a
statistically significant difference at the Defi-
ciency score based on age [F(4, 60.24) � 4.995,
p � .001]. Post hoc comparison using the
Scheffé test indicated that the 30–39 group
(M � 29.42, SD � 3.60) was statistically sig-
nificant from the 60 � group (M � 31.48, SD �
3.19) in regard to Deficiency (p � .011). The
30–39 group is statistically significant from the
50–59 group (M � 31.02, SD � 31.9) in regard
to Deficiency (p � .045). Participants in the

30–39 group scored significantly lower on the
Deficiency subscale than the 50–59 and 60 �
groups. Researchers did not have statistically
significant findings among participants who
were 20–29 and 40–49. Table 2 presents par-
ticipants’ differences in their attitudes toward
poverty by region and age.

Discussion

This study aims to help play therapists explore
their attitudes toward poverty and the conscious
and unconscious patterns of beliefs, or stereo-
types, toward clients who are poor. The findings
from this study begin the exploration of play ther-
apists’ attitudes toward poverty and open the door
for discussion about the impact attitudes have on
mental health care delivery. Aiming at an explo-
ration of these attitudes in our scholarship, we
found, on average, play therapists disagreed or
strongly disagreed with individualistic explana-
tions for poverty, as demonstrated by their mean
ATP-SF Total score (M � 86.40, SD � 10.63),
but more telling are the average subscale scores.

The subscale scores range from 23.64 to 32.44.
The average of the Stigma scale was the highest,

Table 1
Attitude Toward Poverty Total and Subscale Scores for
Demographic Information

Average deficiency
score (SD)

Average stigma
score (SD)

Average structural
score (SD)

Total
score (SD)

Age
20–29 29.05 (3.30) 30.73 (6.47) 24.37 (3.43) 84.18 (11.51)
30–39 29.42 (3.60) 32.28 (5.59) 23.37 (4.16) 85.20 (10.84)
40–49 30.31 (3.70) 32.49 (5.49) 23.33 (3.74) 86.21 (10.43)
50–59 31.02 (3.19) 32.88 (5.39) 23.74 (4.03) 87.78 (10.54)
60 � 31.48 (3.19) 32.83 (5.21) 23.57 (4.04) 88.03 (10.00)

Region
Northeast 30.67 (3.97) 32.95 (5.78) 25.33 (3.43) 88.98 (11.30)
Midwest 30.25 (3.10) 33.71 (4.17) 23.65 (3.28) 87.71 (8.30)
South 30.38 (3.58) 31.62 (5.84) 23.21 (8.87) 85.28 (11.14)
West 30.18 (3.65) 32.72 (5.75) 23.10 (4.74) 86.23 (11.21)
International 30.10 (4.10) 31.14 (5.87) 23.52 (3.44) 84.90 (10.86)

Table 2
MANOVA Differences in Attitudes Toward Poverty-SF of Demographic Variables

Pillai’s trace F Hypothesis df Error df Sig. Partial eta squared

Region 0.082 2.72 12 1155 0.001� 0.027
Age 0.074 2.42 12 1143 0.004� 0.025

� significant � .005.

55CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

32.44 (SD � 5.53), and indicates play therapists
tend not to believe in stereotypes or myths about
individuals in poverty. Examples of stigma state-
ments were “poor people think they deserve to be
supported” and “Welfare mothers have babies to
get more money” (Yun & Weaver, 2010). The
average score for the Personal Deficiency scale
was 30.32 (SD � 3.55) and indicates participants
did not agree or strongly disagreed with state-
ments blaming individuals for being poor. Partic-
ipants did not feel a person’s individual deficit is
the primary cause of poverty and did not hold
individuals personally responsible for their pov-
erty. Personal deficiency statements include “poor
people are dishonest” and “poor people act differ-
ently.” The average score for Structural Perspec-
tive scale was the lowest, 23.64 (SD � 3.71), and
indicates play therapy participants more often dis-
agreed with statements that individuals are re-
sponsible for their financial problems and believe
policies or resources are needed to help those who
are poor. Examples of structural attitudes include
“poor people are discriminated against” and “peo-
ple are poor due to circumstances beyond their
control.” In comparison to other helping profes-
sionals, play therapists appear to have a more
favorable attitudes toward poverty than nurses
(e.g., Wittenauer et al., 2015) or school counselors
(e.g., Ricks, 2014) on the Total, Stigma, Personal
Deficiency, and Structural Perspective scales.

As a profession, participants’ scores in relation
to region and age indicated that there were varying
views on the cause of poverty. When exploring
regions in the United States, there were differ-
ences in how strongly participants believed struc-
tural causes influenced their attitudes and expla-
nations toward poverty. Participants living in the
Northeast leaned more strongly toward a struc-
tural determinant of poverty than participants in
the South. This finding may be due to variances in
poverty rates between the South and Northeast.
The South has the highest poverty rates in the
United States, 13.6%, while the Northeast ranks
the lowest, 10.3%. Nine of the 10 states with the
highest poverty rates in 2017 were Mississippi,
Louisiana, West Virginia, Kentucky, Alabama,
Arkansas, Oklahoma, South Carolina, and Ten-
nessee (Moore, 2018). Additionally, poverty rates
in the South have been continuously high for
decades (Jung et al., 2015), and the metro-
nonmetro poverty rate gap in the South has his-
torically been the largest (Farrigan, 2020). From
2014–2018, the South’s nonmetro poverty rate

was 20.5%, nearly 6% higher than in the region’s
metro areas. In contrast, the Northeast has some of
the lowest poverty rates, under 10.5%, in New
Hampshire, Massachusetts, Connecticut, Rhode
Island, and New Jersey (U.S. Census, 2018). Ad-
ditionally, regional poverty rates for nonmetro and
metro areas in the Northeast were more alike from
2014 to 2018 (Farrigan, 2020). Results might in-
dicate that play therapists’ poverty attitudes are
potentially impacted by poverty rates in their re-
gion of the country. Play therapists may benefit
from understanding the impact of socioeconomic
and regional differences to work best with eco-
nomically disadvantaged clients.

Age was the second factor that resulted in vary-
ing views on poverty. Specifically, participants
who in the 50–59 and 60 plus age groups more
strongly disagreed with a personal explanation
toward poverty than participants in the 30–39 age
group. Life experience and exposure to varying
environmental influences may play an important
factor in poverty attitudes between the 30–39
years old and 50–59 and 60 plus groups. Partici-
pants who are 50–59 and 60 plus were born in the
“Baby Boomers” generation. This period was a
time of prosperity and optimism potentially lead-
ing participants to hold a more positive view of
people experiencing poverty (Watts, 2010). Peo-
ple in the Baby Boomer generation may believe
that individuals who are poor are not the cause of
their poverty and need assistance to work their
way out of poverty. Baby Boomers’ experiences
are different than participants in their 30s, “Mil-
lennials.” Millennials experienced the largest eco-
nomic decline since the great depression, the end
of the Cold War, and deindustrialization which
encouraged people to become reliant on higher
education (Brooks, 2008). Millennials reliance on
higher education and the belief that education will
offer them greater prosperity has caused many to
accrue debt with reduced job opportunities and
Millennials living at or below the poverty line.
Their strong belief in education as a path out of
poverty may explain their slightly lower personal
explanation of poverty. Generational patterns and
exposures may help explain the differences in play
therapists’ attitudes toward poverty. As we gain a
better understanding of play therapist’s attitudes
toward poverty, this may allow us to better pin-
point opportunities to empower and partner with
individuals and communities to seek solutions that
will improve the health of those living in poverty.

56 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

Looking at poverty attitudes is important be-
cause the way play therapists and mental health
professionals view poverty potentially affects the
way they work with people in poverty (Anderson,
2018; Clark et al., 2017; Krumer-Nevo & Lev-
Wiesel, 2005). Play therapists who view poverty
from a systemic perspective may treat clients with
more respect and compassion. For instance, a play
therapist who holds a structural view of poverty
may provide traveling services to a homeless shel-
ter to support the potentially transient nature of
some families in poverty. In addition, play thera-
pists with a positive understanding of poverty may
create structures and policies that support the basic
and expanded needs of clients in poverty, such as
offering snacks during playtime or providing mul-
tidisciplinary resources in the community. Addi-
tionally, play therapists who hold a systemic view
of poverty may feel comfortable advocating for
their clients through social justice advocacy. Play
therapists can systemically advocate for their cli-
ents by offering availability of resources, increas-
ing involvement in governmental policies and
laws, and promoting positive relationships among
clients and resources (Crethar et al., 2008; Crethar
& Winterowd, 2012). Furthermore, play therapists
can be active through social justice advocacy by
going to client’s environments, such as their
schools and neighborhoods (Ceballos & Bratton,
2010; Sheely-Moore & Bratton, 2010), increasing
their self-awareness of their work with oppressed
children (Baggerly, 2006), and being aware of
oppression when looking at their client’s emotions
(O’Connor, 2005). The reasons discussed above
are just a few examples of how varied views of
poverty may impact the depth and extent play
therapists’ support clients in poverty. Play thera-
pists may benefit from professional development
that explores and challenges their attitudes toward
poverty on an ongoing basis.

Limitations and Future Directions

The limitations found in this study help inform
and shape the implications for future studies. A
limitation of the study was the use of a single
measure to evaluate the play therapists’ attitudes
toward poverty. Attitudes and poverty are com-
plex concepts that might benefit from being
looked at through multiple lenses. A future study
including multiple measures of attitudes toward
poverty and people who are poor would be bene-
ficial and give more breadth and depth to under-

standing play therapist’s attitudes toward poverty.
Additionally, qualitative data would allow respon-
dents to elaborate on their ideas and experiences.

The authors recruited from two consortiums
(APT and CESNET) and several play therapy-
related Facebook groups, which may have limited
the participation pool. We sought APT members,
as APT is the flagship professional organization in
the US for play therapists. Unfortunately, not all
play therapists are members of APT. Although we
expanded the search to sites that may reach other
potential participants (CESNET and Facebook),
recruitment through other mental health organiza-
tions, such as ACA, NASW, and APA, may
bridge gaps of play therapists who are not mem-
bers of APT. A narrow pool of applicants may
limit participation and bias the results. Future re-
searchers could recruit through multiple profes-
sional organizations to increase the diversity of
potential participants.

A few participants in this study had a strong
reaction to the ATP-SF. Participants voluntarily
emailed the researchers their thoughts and ex-
periences taking the survey. Some participants
had a negative reaction to the ATP-SF, such as
a dislike for the wording of the measure, and
they reported that the ATP-SF made false as-
sumptions and generalizations. The purpose of
this research was to evaluate the quantitative
responses to the survey, and participants’ strong
reactions indicate a qualitative study may be
needed as well.

Conclusion

The current study is the first study exploring
play therapists’ attitudes toward poverty. Overall,
the authors found that region and age play a factor
in how play therapists view poverty. The findings
affirm the importance of continued professional
development around cultural competence when
working with diverse populations. Play therapist’s
awareness and knowledge impacts the way they
work with diverse clients, particularly clients in
poverty. Based on findings from this study, we
believe it is important for play therapists to exam-
ine their own views and explore factors that im-
pact their views of others to ensure they are pro-
viding culturally informed practices and
ultimately respond with more sensitivity for those
struggling in poverty.

57CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

References

American Counseling Association. (2014). 2014
ACA code of ethics. https://www.counseling.org/
knowledge-center

American Psychological Association. (2017a). Ethi-
cal principles of psychologists and code of con-
duct. https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/

American Psychological Association. (2017b). Mul-
ticultural guidelines: An Ecological approach to
context, identity, and intersectionality. http://www.
apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

Anderson, A. (2018). Attitudes towards poverty of
human service practitioners who provide direct
service to families (Publication No. 10979759)
[Doctoral dissertation, Capella University]. Pro-
Quest Dissertations and Theses Global.

Appelbaum, L. D., Lennon, M. C., & Lawrence Aber,
J. (2006). When effort is threatening: The influ-
ence of the belief in a just world on Americans’
attitudes toward antipoverty policy. Political Psy-
chology, 27, 387– 402. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

Association for Play Therapy. (2019). Play therapy
best practices: Clinical, professional, and ethical
issues. https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/
publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

Atherton, C. R., Gemmel, R. J., Haagenstad, S., Holt,
D. J., Jensen, L., O’Hara, D., & Rehner, T. (1993).
Measuring attitudes toward poverty: A new scale.
Social Work Research & Abstracts, 29(4), 28–30.
https://doi.org/10.1093/swra/29.4.28

Baggerly, J. (2006). Service learning with children
affected by poverty: Facilitating multicultural
competence in counseling education students.
Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Develop-
ment, 34(4), 244–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/j
.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

Beeghley, L. (1988). Individual and structural expla-
nations of poverty. Population Research and Pol-
icy Review, 7(3), 201–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF02456102

Brady, D. (2019). Theories of the causes of poverty.
Annual Review of Sociology, 45(1), 155–175.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-
022550

Bray, S. S., & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2015). School
counselors’ ways of knowing and social orienta-
tion in relationship to poverty beliefs. Journal of
Counseling & Development, 93, 312–320. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

Brooks, C. (2008). A legacy of leadership: Gover-
nors and American history. University of Pennsyl-
vania Press.

Capell, J., Veenstra, G., & Dean, E. (2007). Cultural
competence in healthcare: Critical analysis of the
construct, its assessment and implications. Journal
of Theory Construction & Testing, 11(1), 30–37.

Ceballos, P., & Bratton, S. (2010). Empowering La-
tino families: Effects of a culturally responsive
intervention for low-income immigrant Latino par-
ents on children’s behaviors and parental stress.
Psychology in the Schools, 47(8), 761–775. https://
doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

Ciment, J. (2013). Poverty. In C. G. Bates & J.
Ciment (Eds.), Global social issues: An encyclo-
pedia (pp. 846–855). Routledge.

Clark, M., Moe, J., & Hays, D. (2017). The relation-
ship between counselors’ multicultural counseling
competence and poverty beliefs. Counselor Edu-
cation and Supervision, 56(4), 259–273. https://doi
.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.

Cooper, K., & Stewart, K. (2013). Does money affect
children’s outcomes? Joseph Rowntree Founda-
tion. https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-
affect-children’s-outcomes

Crethar, H., Rivera, E., & Nash, S. (2008). In search
of common threads: Linking multicultural, femi-
nist, and social justice counseling paradigms. Jour-
nal of Counseling and Development, 86(3), 269–
278. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008
.tb00509.x

Crethar, H., & Winterowd, C. (2012). Values and
social justice in counseling. Counseling and Val-
ues, 57(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-
007X.2012.00001.x

Cummins, I. (2018). Poverty, inequality and social
work: The impact of neoliberalism and austerity
politics on welfare provision. Policy Press.

Davids, Y., & Gouws, A. (2013). Monitoring percep-
tions of the causes of poverty in South Africa.
Social Indicators Research, 110(3), 1201–1220.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9980-9

Farrigan, T. (2020, 12 February). Rural Poverty and
Well-being. USDA. https://www.ers.usda.gov/top
ics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-
being/

Farthing, R. (2014). Family poverty. In J. Treas, J.
Scott, & M. Richards (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell
companion to the sociology of families (pp. 132–
154). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/97811183
74085.ch7

Feagin, J. R. (1975). Subordinating the poor: Welfare
and American beliefs. Prentice Hall.

Garrett, P. M. (2018). Welfare words: Critical social
work and social policy. Sage. https://doi.org/10
.4135/9781526418661

Gil, E. (2005). From sensitivity to competence in
working across cultures. In A. A. Drewes & E. Gil
(Eds.), Cultural issues in play therapy (pp. 3–25).
The Guilford Press.

Gilbert, J., Goode, T. D., & Dunne, C. (2007). Cur-
ricula enhancement module series: Cultural aware-
ness. National Center for Cultural Competence.

58 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://www.counseling.org/knowledge-center

https://www.counseling.org/knowledge-center

https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/

http://www.apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

http://www.apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

https://doi.org/10.1093/swra/29.4.28

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02456102

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02456102

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-022550

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-022550

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-affect-children%26#x2019;s-outcomes

https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-affect-children%26#x2019;s-outcomes

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008.tb00509.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008.tb00509.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-007X.2012.00001.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-007X.2012.00001.x

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9980-9

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118374085.ch7

https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118374085.ch7

https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526418661

https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526418661

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/
awareness

Goldsmith, W. W., & Blakely, E. J. (2010). Separate
societies: Poverty and inequality in U.S. cities
(2nd ed.). Temple University Press.

Gordon, D., & Townsend, P. (Eds.). (2000). Bread-
line Europe: The measurement of poverty. Polity
Press.

Haughton, J., & Khandker, S. R. (2009). Handbook
on poverty and inequality. World Bank. http://
documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/4880814681
57174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

Howard, C., Freeman, A., Wilson, A., & Brown, E.
(2017). Poverty. Public Opinion Quarterly, 81(3),
769–789. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfx022

Hunt, M. O., & Bullock, H. E. (2016). Ideologies and
beliefs about poverty. In D. Brady & L. M. Burton
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social science of
poverty (pp. 93–116). Oxford University Press.

Jordan, G. (2004). The causes of poverty cultural vs.
structural: Can these be a synthesis? Perspectives
in Public Affairs, 1, 18–34.

Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of
stereotyping in system-justification and production
of false consciousness. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 33(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

Jung, S., Cho, S., & Roberts, R. (2015). The impact
of government funding of poverty reduction pro-
grams: Government funding and poverty reduction
in the Southern U.S. Papers in Regional Science,
94(3), 653– 675. https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs
.12089

Koball, H., & Jiang, Y. (2018). Basic facts about
low-income children: Children under 18 years,
2016. National Center for Children in Poverty.
https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10
.7916/D8JS9Q92

Kraus, S. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of
behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical litera-
ture. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
21, 58–75. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616
7295211007

Krumer-Nevo, M., & Lev-Wiesel, R. (2005). Atti-
tudes of social work students towards clients with
basic needs. Journal of Social Work Education,
41(3), 545–556. https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE
.2005.200303137

Levin, L., & Schwartz-Tayri, T. (2017). Attitudes
towards poverty, organizations, ethics and morals:
Israeli social workers’ shared decision making.
Health Expectations, 20(3), 448–458. https://doi
.org/10.1111/hex.12472

Major, B., & O’Brien, L. (2005). The social psychol-
ogy of stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56,
393– 421. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych
.56.091103.070137

Mead, L. M. (2011). From prophecy to charity: How
to help the poor. The AEI Press.

Moore, K. A., Redd, Z., Burkhauser, M., Mbwana,
K., & Collins, A. (2009). Children in poverty:
Trends, consequences, and policy options (Re-
search Brief No. 2009–11). Child Trends. https://
www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/
2009-11ChildreninPoverty

Moore, R. (2018). Poverty statistics for Southern
States. Southern League Conference. https://www
.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub�580

Morrow, K. A., & Deidan, C. T. (1992). Bias in the
counseling process: How to recognize and avoid it.
Journal of Counseling & Development, 70(5),
571–577. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676
.1992.tb01663.x

National Association of Social Workers. (2015).
Standards and indicators for cultural competence
in social work practice. https://www.socialworkers
.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket�PonPTDEBrn
4%3D&portalid�0

National Scientific Council on the Developing Child.
(2015). Supportive relationships and active skill-
building strengthen the foundations of resilience.
Working Paper 13. https://developingchild.harvard
.edu/resources/supportive-relationships-and-act
ive-skill-building-strengthen-the-foundations-of-
resilience/

New Freedom Commission on Mental Health.
(2003). Achieving the promise: Transforming
Mental health care in America: Final report (Re-
port No. SMA-03–3832). Department of Health
and Human Services.

Noone, J., Sideras, S., Gubrud-Howe, P., Voss, H., &
Mathews, L. (2012). Influence of a poverty simu-
lation on nursing student attitudes toward poverty.
The Journal of Nursing Education, 51(11), 617–
622. https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-201
20914-01

O’Connor, K. (2005). Addressing diversity issues in
play therapy. Professional Psychology, Research
and Practice, 36(5), 566–573. https://doi.org/10
.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

Poverty Programs. (2017). Poverty statistics: USA
poverty. http://www.povertyprogram.com/usa.php

Ratts, M. J., Singh, A. A., Nassar-McMillan, S.,
Butler, S. K., & McCullough, J. R. (2016). Multi-
cultural and social justice counseling competen-
cies. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and De-
velopment, 44(1), 28–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/
jmcd.12035

Ricks, L. A. (2014). Attributes, attitudes, and per-
ceived self-efficacy levels of school counselors to-
ward poverty [Unpublished doctoral dissertation].
Auburn University. https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/
handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School
%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence�2

59CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/awareness

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/awareness

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfx022

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs.12089

https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs.12089

https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10.7916/D8JS9Q92

https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10.7916/D8JS9Q92

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295211007

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295211007

https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE.2005.200303137

https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE.2005.200303137

https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.12472

https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.12472

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub=580

https://www.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub=580

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1992.tb01663.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1992.tb01663.x

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120914-01

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120914-01

https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

http://www.povertyprogram.com/usa.php

https://doi.org/10.1002/jmcd.12035

https://doi.org/10.1002/jmcd.12035

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

Royce, E. (2018). Poverty & power: The problem of
structural inequality (3rd ed.). Rowman & Little-
field Publishers, Inc.

Shapiro, S. M. (2004). The relationship among men-
tal health clinicians’ beliefs in a just world, atti-
tudes toward the poor, and beliefs about helping
the poor [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Wal-
den University.

Sheely-Moore, A., & Bratton, S. (2010). A strengths-
based parenting intervention with low-income Af-
rican American families. Professional School
Counseling, 13(3), 175–183. https://doi.org/10
.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

Siegel, C., Haugland, G., & Schore, R. (2005). The
interface of cultural competence and evidence-
based practices. In R. E. Drake, M. R. Merrens, &
D. W. Lynde (Eds.), Evidence-based mental health
practice: A textbook (pp. 273–299). Norton.

Siu, A. F. Y. (2010). Play therapy in Hong Kong:
Opportunities and challenges. International Jour-
nal of Play Therapy, 19(4), 235–243. https://doi
.org/10.1037/a0020641

Sturm, D. C. (2008). The impact of client level of
poverty on counselor attitudes and attributions
about the client [Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion]. University of North Carolina at Charlotte.

Sue, D. W., Arredondo, P., & McDavis, R. J. (1992).
Multicultural counseling competencies and stan-
dards: A call to the profession. Journal of Multi-
cultural Counseling and Development, 20(2), 64–
88. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992
.tb00563.x

Sue, D. W., Ivey, A. E., & Pedersen, P. B. (1996). A
theory of multicultural counseling and therapy.
Brooks/Cole.

Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). Counseling the cul-
turally diverse: Theory and practice (6th ed.). Wi-
ley.

Sue, S. (2006). Cultural competency: From philoso-
phy to research and practice. Journal of Commu-
nity Psychology, 34(2), 237–245. https://doi.org/10
.1002/jcop.20095

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using
multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Pearson Educa-
tion.

Turner, M. A., & Rawlings, L. A. (2005). Ten lessons
for policy and practice. The Urban Institute. http://
webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_
Poverty_Brief

Turner, N. (2011). How poverty hurts our children.
Nursing New Zealand, 17(6), 33.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2018). How the Census Bureau
measures poverty. United States Census Bureau.
https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/
poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

Van Allen, K., & Sterling, Y. (2011). Pediatric nurses
address children and the economy: Part 1. The
impact of poverty on children and families. Jour-
nal of Pediatric Nursing, 26(4), 369–372. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

van Heerde, J., & Hudson, D. (2010). “The righteous
consider the cause of the poor”? Public attitudes
towards poverty in developing countries. Political
Studies, 58(3), 389–409. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

Watts, D. (2010). Baby boomers. Dictionary of
American government and politics. Edinburgh
University Press.

Weiss-Gal, I., Benyamini, Y., Ginzburg, K., Savaya,
R., & Peled, E. (2009). Social workers’ and service
users’ causal attributions for poverty. Social Work,
54(2), 125–133. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2
.125

Wickham, S., Anwar, E., Barr, B., Law, C., & Tay-
lor-Robinson, D. (2016). Poverty and child health
in the U. K.: Using evidence for action. Archives of
Disease in Childhood, 101 759–766. https://doi
.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

Wittenauer, J., Ludwick, R., Baughman, K., & Fish-
bein, R. (2015). Surveying the hidden attitudes of
hospital nurses’ towards poverty. Journal of Clin-
ical Nursing, 24(15–16):2184–2191. https://doi
.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

Wolff, J. (2019). Poverty. Philosophy Compass,
14(12). Advance online publication. https://doi
.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

World Bank Institute. (2005). Poverty manual. http://
siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/
PovertyManual

Yun, S., & Weaver, R. (2010). Development and
validation of a short form of the Attitude Toward
Poverty Scale. Advances in Social Work, 11(2),
174–187. https://doaj.org/article/00914568df
9245f8841fcc3a769281e4. https://doi.org/10
.18060/437

Received February 4, 2020
Revision received June 26, 2020

Accepted October 5, 2020 �

60 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
Ps
yc
ho
lo
gi
ca
l
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
on
e
of
its
al
lie
d
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://doi.org/10.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

https://doi.org/10.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020641

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020641

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992.tb00563.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992.tb00563.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20095

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20095

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2.125

https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2.125

https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

https://doaj.org/article/00914568df9245f8841fcc3a769281e4

https://doaj.org/article/00914568df9245f8841fcc3a769281e4

https://doi.org/10.18060/437

https://doi.org/10.18060/437

  • Cultural Competence and Poverty: Exploring Play Therapists’ Attitudes
  • Cultural Competence
    Poverty
    Poverty Attitudes
    Play Therapy
    Method
    Procedure
    Participants
    Instrument
    Data Analysis
    Results
    Differences by Region and Age
    Discussion
    Limitations and Future Directions
    Conclusion
    References

Cultural Competence and Poverty:
Exploring Play Therapists’ Attitudes

Lauren Chase and Kristie Opiola
Department of Counseling, University of North Carolina at Charlotte

This article reports the findings of a survey that investigated attitudes toward poverty
among play therapists (N � 390) and its relation to demographic information. Multi-
variate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were used to measure the relationship
between play therapists’ demographics and their attitudes toward poverty, specifically
their structural, personal deficiency, and stigma scores. Results indicated that both
region and age resulted in differing views on povert

y.

Participants living in the
Northeast held stronger structural views of poverty than participants in the South.
Similarly, participants in the

50

–59 and 60 plus age groups disagreed to strongly
disagree with a personal explanation toward poverty than participants in the 30 –39 age
group. The importance of play therapists’ examining their attitudes toward poverty and
the direct impact on their work is discussed. Finally, implications of the results,
including overall findings, are explained.

Keywords: play therapy, attitudes of poverty, cultural competence

Culturally competent training is an element
of credentialing requirements that ensures men-
tal health providers offer adequate and respon-
sive care to diverse populations. Although the
mental health field has embedded cultural com-
petence in their standards and guidelines, there
are discrepancies in the way the profession as-
sesses and measures competence (Sue et al.,
1996). Researchers have investigated attitudes
toward poverty in the helping professions
(Levin & Schwartz-Tayri, 2017; Noone et al.,
2012; van Heerde & Hudson, 2010; & Wit-
tenauer et al., 2015), but no study has focused
on play therapists’ attitudes toward povert

y.

The purpose of this study is to fill a gap in the
literature regarding play therapist’s attitudes to-
ward poverty because awareness and knowl-
edge are key elements to implement culturally
responsive services and skills with diverse chil-
dren in a variety of settings.

Cultural Competence

Cultural competence is an important compo-
nent of professional practice, and practitioners
are expected to develop skills and understand-
ing pertaining to diverse clientele. Researchers
define cultural competence as the set of beliefs,
knowledge, and skills mental health providers
possess in order to deliver effective interven-
tions and services to members of various cul-
tures (Gilbert et al., 2007; Sue, 2006). The New
Freedom Commission on Mental Health (2003)
recognized disparities in mental health delivery
and viewed the lack of cultural competence for
minority populations as a persistent problem.
Culturally competent health care is essential to
providing effective care to all populations. To
aid practitioners in their ability to increase their
cultural competence, leading professional men-
tal health associations have published profes-
sional cultural competency standards and ethi-
cal codes (American Counseling Association
[ACA], 2014; American Psychological Associ-
ation [APA], 2017a, 2017b; National Associa-
tion of Social Workers [NASW], 2015; Ratts et
al., 2015) to promote and guide clinical prac-
tice.

Lauren Chase X https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9811-0520
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-

dressed to Lauren Chase, Department of Counseling, Uni-
versity of North Carolina at Charlotte, 9201 University City
Boulevard, Charlotte, NC 28223, United States. Email:
lchase6@uncc.edu

T
hi

s
do

cu
m

en
t

is
co

py
ri

gh
te

d
by

th
e

A
m

er
ic

an
P

sy
ch

ol
og

ic
al

A
ss

oc
ia

ti
on

or
on

e
of

it
s

al
li

ed
pu

bl
is

he
rs

.
T

hi
s

ar
ti

cl
e

is
in

te
nd

ed
so

le
ly

fo
r

th
e

pe
rs

on
al

us
e

of
th

e
in

di
vi

du
al

us
er

an
d

is
no

t
to

be
di

ss
em

in
at

ed
br

oa
dl

y.

International Journal of

Play Therapy

© 2021 Association for Play Therapy 2021, Vol. 30, No. 1, 50 – 60
ISSN: 1555-6824 https://doi.org/10.1037/pla0000144

50

https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9811-0520

mailto:lchase6@uncc.edu

https://doi.org/10.1037/pla0000144

Cultural competence is fundamental to effec-
tively building relationships with clients and
helping them achieve their mental health goals
(Capell et al., 2007). Sue et al. (1996) identified
three key components of cultural competence:
cultural awareness, cultural knowledge, and
cultural skill. Cultural awareness is founda-
tional and necessary for individuals to develop
culturally competent attitudes, knowledge, and
skills (Gilbert et al., 2007). Furthermore, cultur-
ally aware clinicians are individuals who are
sensitive to their personal values and biases and
acknowledge how their values and biases may
influence their view of the client, client’s prob-
lem, and counseling relationship (Sue, 2006;
Sue & Sue, 2016). Sue and Sue (2016) further
urged culturally competent mental health to
know beliefs, attitudes, principles, and world-
views common to the particular culture of peo-
ple they serve. Additionally, culturally skilled
clinicians benefit from reading relevant diverse
cultural research, pursue educational opportuni-
ties on diverse cultural skills, and involve them-
selves with diverse cultures outside of counsel-
ing relationships (Sue et al., 1992; Sue & Sue,
2016).

Sue and Sue (2016) advocated mental health
providers to assess their own cultural beliefs,
cultural values, and behaviors to increase their
self-awareness and cultural understanding to
decrease barriers in client-provider relation-
ships and increase comfort in working with
diverse clients. Mental health professionals gain
personal awareness when they are curious, re-
flective, vulnerable, and observant (Gilbert et
al., 2007). A lack of cultural awareness and
competence can have negative consequences
for the profession, as well as the clients. Siegel
et al. (2005) cautioned professionals that a lack
of cultural competence on their part may lead to
incorrect assessments and diagnoses and can
cause minority and marginalized populations,
such as clients with low socioeconomic status
and living in poverty, to underutilize services
and prematurely terminate care.

Poverty

Cultural competence includes working with
clients of diverse backgrounds, such as clients
in poverty. The culture of poverty can be a
controversial topic because some practitioners
believe people in poverty play a role in perpet-

uating causes of poverty and sustain the cycle
across generations (Cummins, 2018; Garrett,
2018). The culture of poverty is important in
understanding the concept of poverty because
human action is constrained and enabled by
how people define their actions. As the root of
poverty culture is a multifaceted concept and is
challenging to define, poverty culture dynamics
help researchers examine the cycle of poverty
and social inequality. Researchers and public
agencies often highlight personal and environ-
mental factors that contribute to the definition of
poverty, with a lack of income and resources
being the most defining factors (Ciment, 2013;
U.S. Census Bureau, 2018; World Bank Insti-
tute, 2005). People living in poverty often lack
material essentials, such as shelter, water, cloth-
ing, and food, which can impact one’s overall
well-being (Haughton & Khandker, 2009;
Wolff, 2019). The lack of essential material and
inability to establish daily living patterns and
activities lead individuals in poverty to feel
confined by their financial situation (Gordon &
Townsend, 2000). When society values finan-
cial wealth, people who meet the standard of
poverty may feel lesser than those with financial
stability; therefore, a lack of autonomy, educa-
tion, and self-worth are obstacles keeping many
in poverty.

Poverty rates can be surprisingly high in de-
veloped countries. The United States (US) is
one of the wealthiest nations in the world but
has one of the highest rates of poverty, with one
in eight Americans living in poverty (Poverty
Programs, 2017). While poverty affects people
of all ages and demographics, minority individ-
uals disproportionately exceed the national av-
erage (Koball & Jiang, 2018). Those at greatest
risk of living in poverty are families headed by
single mothers with children (U.S. Census Bu-
reau, 2018).

Growing up in poverty is a contributing fac-
tor to illness, disability, and adverse mental
health outcomes, which can have devastating
effects on children that can extend into adult-
hood (Moore et al., 2009). It appears the longer
a child lives in poverty, the more devastating
the impact (Wickham et al., 2014). Cooper and
Stewart (2013) completed a systemic review of
the literature on household income and health
outcomes and found family income makes a
significant impact on all areas of children’s de-
velopment and their level of academic achieve-

51CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

ment. They posited children living in poverty
have lower cognitive ability, lower social en-
gagement, greater behavioral problems, and
poorer health outcomes. Furthermore, poverty
impacts children’s participation in common
childhood activities, such as access to extracur-
ricular activities and peer social activities
(Turner, 2011). In addition, family poverty im-
pacts living and community environments, so-
cial supports, and parental mental health, which
can impact the level of stress children experi-
ence (Farthing, 2014; National Scientific Coun-
cil on the Developing Child, 2015; Turner &
Rawlings, 2005). High levels of stress can have
detrimental impacts on children’s holistic de-
velopment (National Scientific Council on the
Developing Child, 2015) and increases the risk
of children developing mental health and be-
havioral problems (Van Allen & Sterling,
2011).

Poverty Attitudes

Attitude is an important predictor of how a
mental health professional behaves toward cli-
ents and effectively treat clients (Sturm, 2008).
Researchers have concluded that attitudes are
related to behavior, especially when the atti-
tudes are held with a high degree of assurity, are
constant, available, and formed by direct expe-
riences (Appelbaum et al., 2006; Kraus, 1995;
Morrow & Deidan, 1992; Shapiro, 2004). There
are competing explanations for the rationality
for one’s attitudes toward poverty (Brady, 2019;
Hunt & Bullock, 2016; Jordan, 2004) with two
overarching themes: structural cause of poverty
or personal causes of poverty (Beeghley, 1988;
Feagin, 1975; Howard et al., 2017).

Structural causes of poverty include the be-
lief that poverty is caused by low government
investment in education, health, and employ-
ment opportunities (Beeghley, 1988; Davids &
Gouws, 2013; Feagin, 1975; Goldsmith &
Blakely, 2010; Royce, 2018; Weiss-Gal et al.,
2009). Structural causes focus on how society
limits one’s opportunities through low wages,
lack of education, and lack of affordable health
care (Wittenauer et al., 2015). Society is blamed
as a cause in the structural view (Noone et al.,
2012), and a person is not responsible or in
control of the cause of poverty. In contrast,
personal causes of poverty are viewed as exhib-
iting low motivation, possessing passive or lazy

characteristics, or growing up in a “poverty
culture” (Levin & Schwartz-Tayri, 2017; Mead,
2011; Royce, 2018). Personal causes focus on
the person’s inability to pull themselves out of
poverty and attribute causes to negative money
management or health behaviors. According to
Bray and Schommer-Aikins (2015), personal
causes of poverty imply that people are respon-
sible and have sufficient chances to succeed if
they work hard; therefore, people are viewed as
being in control of their own destiny.

Mental health providers’ attitudes toward
poverty have not been thoroughly examined in
the research. Previous research has focused on a
single region of the country or a specific popu-
lation. Three specific studies explored helping
professionals’ view of poverty: human service
practitioners (Anderson, 2018), school counsel-
ors (Ricks, 2014), and social workers (Weiss-
Gal et al., 2009). Weiss-Gal et al. (2009) studied
the views of social workers and service users in
the midwestern United States, and they con-
cluded participants held comparable levels of
understanding for motivational and psycholog-
ical causes. Furthermore, they found service
users credited more significance to social/
structural causes and fatalistic causes compared
to the social workers in the study. Additionally,
Ricks (2014) examined school counselors’ atti-
tudes toward poverty and found school counsel-
ors living in the southeastern United States held
individualistic attitudes toward poverty and at-
tributed poverty to fatalistic causes. Further-
more, Anderson (2018) interviewed human ser-
vice practitioners from the southern United
States on their view of poverty, and he found
human service practitioners held both structural
and individualistic views of poverty. Overall,
the exploration of poverty attitudes in mental
health providers is sparse and more studies are
needed to improve cultural competence.

Play Therapy

Play therapists represent diverse professional
licensures, including counseling, social work,
psychology, and marriage and family therapy
(Siu, 2010). The Association for Play Therapy
(APT) addresses the importance of diversity in
their mission statement, credentialing guide-
lines and documents but lacks specific cultural
competency guidelines. Currently APT (2019)
refers to multicultural competence as the pro-

52 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

cess of play therapists becoming aware of one’s
own culture and biases. For play therapists,
instruction occurs in a variety of settings, such
as university classrooms, professional confer-
ences, and regional trainings. The variety of
educational backgrounds, clinical training, and
supervised experience acquired by Registered
Play Therapist/Supervisors (RPT/S) and School
Based-Registered Play Therapists (SB-RPT)
generates unique perspectives to play therapy
and creates service delivery options to clients
from diverse backgrounds, such as poverty.

Play therapists striving to increase multicultural
competence can work with children in poverty
more effectively by becoming aware of their own
biases toward poverty and learning culturally re-
sponsive skills to respond to needs of children in
poverty. By exploring biases and beliefs about
clients from low socioeconomic status and pov-
erty, play therapists can obtain knowledge to de-
velop active cultural competence (Gil, 2005). Play
therapists obtain knowledge when they gain infor-
mation about their attitudes toward poverty to
provide more responsive services. Therefore, to
provide high quality services, play therapists need
to explore their beliefs to best attend to the needs
of children and their families who are experienc-
ing poverty. In addition, the field of play therapy
can benefit from exploring play therapists’ atti-
tudes on poverty as a means of identifying areas
where further instruction and exposure are needed.

Specifically, the researchers were interested in
exploring participant’s overall attitudes toward
poverty and the relationship between participant’s
demographic information and their scores on the
Attitudes Toward Poverty-Short Form (ATP-SF).
The demographic characteristics were age, gen-
der, race, region, highest educational degree, pro-
fessional discipline, and years of experience. The
research questions were (a) What are play thera-
pists’ attitudes toward poverty? (b) Are there dif-
ferences in ATP subscales (personal deficiency,
stigma, structural perspective, and total) based on
play therapist demographic characteristics?

Method

Procedure

Participants were recruited from the APT mail-
ing list, Counselor Education and Supervision
Network (CESNET) listserv, and invited mem-
bers of four play therapy-related Facebook groups.

Participants in the APT mailing list were recruited
via email invitation to the online SurveyShare
questionnaire. Participants in the play therapy-
related Facebook groups and CESNET Listserv
were invited to participate from the first author
posting in respective groups and Listserv. Upon
entry to the survey, respondents first read the
informed consent form approved by the universi-
ty’s institutional review board. Participants either
accepted or rejected participation in the study. If
respondents accepted, they were directed to the
survey items. Participants completed the ATP-SF
and a demographic survey, and they could access
the survey only by using the unique email invita-
tion sent to them. Participants utilized email ad-
dresses to sign into SurveyShare, which allowed
the researchers to control for one code per person.

Participants

Initially, over 5,000 invitations to participate
were sent over one month, and 391 survey pack-
ets were received. After removing one incom-
plete survey, the final sample size for data anal-
ysis was 390. The sample size for the present
study was deemed sufficient, based on exceed-
ing Cohen’s (1988) and Tabachnick and Fi-
dell’s (2013) recommendations for the minimal
sample sizes needed to achieve adequate power
(.80) at a significance level of p � .05 (i.e., 120
and 113, respectively). Specific to cultural de-
mographics, participants’ ages ranged from 23
to 78 years (M � 46.13, SD � 11.94). In
addition, 367 participants identified as female
(94.1%), 18 as male (4.6%), and five identified
as other (1.3%). Ethnoculturally, 321 partici-
pants identified as White (82.3%), 18 as African
American (4.6%), 25 as Hispanic/Latinx
(6.4%), and 26 as other cultures (6.7%).

Participants identified as play therapists or
play therapy trainees from multiple disciplines:
mental health (n � 181, 46.4%), social workers
(n � 95, 24.4%), marriage and family therapists
(n � 46, 11.8%), and other (e.g., psychologist,
international professionals; n � 68, 17.5%). In
terms of regions of the country, the profession-
als lived in the following regions: Northeast
(n � 42, 10.8%), Midwest (n � 85, 21.8%),
South (n � 154, 39.5%), West (n � 88, 22.6%),
or internationally (n � 21, 5.4%). Play thera-
pists’ experience levels included: 0 –2 years
(n � 48, 12.3%), 3–5 years (n � 79, 20.3%),

53CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

6 –9 years (n � 80, 20.5%), 10 –13 years (n �
51, 13.1%), and 14 plus years (n � 132, 33.8%).

Instrument

The Attitudes Toward Poverty Scale-Short
Form (ATP-SF; Yun & Weaver, 2010) is a
self-report instrument based on the Attitudes
Toward Poverty Scale (ATP; Atherton et al.,
1993). The ATP-SF is a multidimensional scale
measuring attitudes toward poverty, leaning to-
ward a structural cause or individual deficit
cause. Each item is based on a 5-point Likert
scale (1 � strongly disagree to 5 � strongly
agree). An example of a structural cause-item
on the ATP-SF is, “People are poor due to
circumstances beyond their control.” An indi-
vidual deficit-item is, “Poor people are dishon-
est,” and a stigma-item is “Welfare makes peo-
ple lazy.” High scores indicate a belief that
structural determinants are the primary causes
of poverty while low scores indicate a personal
explanation of poverty (Atherton et al., 1993).
The Total score can range from 21 to 105.

The ATP-SF includes three subfactors: Struc-
tural Perspective, Personal Deficiency, and
Stigma. Structural causes of poverty include the
belief that poverty is caused by low government
investment in education, health, and employ-
ment opportunities (Beeghley, 1988; Feagin,
1975; Weiss-Gal et al., 2009). The Structural
Perspective subscale ranges from 6 to 30. Per-
sonal causes focus on the person’s inability to
pull themselves out of poverty by managing
money poorly or poor health behaviors. The
Personal Deficiency subscale ranges from 7 to
35. Stereotyping people in poverty adds to the
stigma of poverty. The subscale of Stigma is
important because members of marginalized
groups, such as those in poverty, are at a higher
likelihood of being stigmatized (Major &
O’Brien, 2005). Members of groups with less
power often evoke negative responses from
those with more power for those in power to
keep their position (Jost & Banaji, 1994). The
Stigma subscale ranges from 8 to 40. The
ATP-SF has a high level of internal consistency
with � � .87. All of the subscales of the
ATP-SF exceeded the minimum acceptable
level for internal consistency with all scales
being between .50 and .70.

Data Analysis

To examine the impact of play therapists’
demographics on their attitudes toward poverty,
a series of multivariate analyses of variances
(MANOVAs) were conducted. The independent
variables were (a) race, (b) age, (c) marital
status, (d) gender, (e) region, (f) setting, (g)
occupation, (h) years practicing play therapy,
and (i) RPT status, and the dependent variables
were (a) Personal Deficiency, (b) Stigma, (c)
Structural Perspective, and (d) Total Score. Be-
fore conducting the MANOVAs, the data were
screened for missing data, outliers, noncol-
linearity, equality of variance/covariance matri-
ces, and normality. Additionally, to examine
play therapist’s overall attitudes toward pov-
erty, researchers used average scores on the
ATP-SF Total, and Structural Perspective, Per-
sonal Deficiency, and Stigma subscales.

Results

The researchers strived to explore diverse
demographics. Due to the lack of diversity in
the sample, specifically regarding gender, race,
marital status, setting, occupation, RPT status,
and socioeconomic status, the researchers were
unable to explore potential differences. A large
majority of participants were white masters’
level, middle-class women. The average of the
Total score was 86.40 (SD � 10.63). The aver-
age Structural Perspective scale was 23.52
(SD � 3.94). The average of the Personal De-
ficiency scale was 30.32 (SD � 3.55). The
average of the Stigma scale found was 32.44
(SD � 5.53). Table 1 presents the mean scores
and standard deviations for the demographic
information for the ATP-SF Deficiency,
Stigma, Structural Perspective, and Total
scores.

Differences by Region and Age

A MANOVA was conducted to explore the
impact of geographic regions on attitudes to-
ward poverty. Participants were divided into
five regions (Northwest, Midwest, South, West,
and International). Using Pillai’s criterion, the
combined dependent variables were statistically
affected by the region, F(12, 1155) � 2.71, p �
.001. There was a statistically significant differ-
ence at the p � .05 level in Structural score

54 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

based on region (F(4, 385) � 2.184, p � .026),
but not in discipline and years of experience.
Post hoc comparison using Scheffé test indi-
cated that the South was significantly lower
(M � 23.21, SD � 3.87) than the Northeast
(M � 25.33, SD � 3.43) in regard to Structural
Perspective score (p � .046). Researchers did
not have statistically significant findings among
participants’ scores in the Midwest, West, and
International.

A MANOVA was conducted to explore the
impact of age on Total score and subscales of
Stigma, Structural Perspective, and Deficiency.
Using the Pillai’s criterion, the combined de-
pendent variables were statistically affected by
age (F(12, 1143) � 2.419, p � .004). There was a
statistically significant difference at the Defi-
ciency score based on age [F(4, 60.24) � 4.995,
p � .001]. Post hoc comparison using the
Scheffé test indicated that the 30 –39 group
(M � 29.42, SD � 3.60) was statistically sig-
nificant from the 60 � group (M � 31.48, SD �
3.19) in regard to Deficiency (p � .011). The
30 –39 group is statistically significant from the
50 –59 group (M � 31.02, SD � 31.9) in regard
to Deficiency (p � .045). Participants in the

30 –39 group scored significantly lower on the
Deficiency subscale than the 50 –59 and 60 �
groups. Researchers did not have statistically
significant findings among participants who
were 20 –29 and 40 – 49. Table 2 presents par-
ticipants’ differences in their attitudes toward
poverty by region and age.

Discussion

This study aims to help play therapists explore
their attitudes toward poverty and the conscious
and unconscious patterns of beliefs, or stereo-
types, toward clients who are poor. The findings
from this study begin the exploration of play ther-
apists’ attitudes toward poverty and open the door
for discussion about the impact attitudes have on
mental health care delivery. Aiming at an explo-
ration of these attitudes in our scholarship, we
found, on average, play therapists disagreed or
strongly disagreed with individualistic explana-
tions for poverty, as demonstrated by their mean
ATP-SF Total score (M � 86.40, SD � 10.63),
but more telling are the average subscale scores.

The subscale scores range from 23.64 to 32.44.
The average of the Stigma scale was the highest,

Table 1
Attitude Toward Poverty Total and Subscale Scores for
Demographic Information

Average deficiency
score (SD)

Average stigma
score (SD)

Average structural
score (SD)

Total
score (SD)

Age
20–29 29.05 (3.30) 30.73 (6.47) 24.37 (3.43) 84.18 (11.51)
30–39 29.42 (3.60) 32.28 (5.59) 23.37 (4.16) 85.20 (10.84)
40–49 30.31 (3.70) 32.49 (5.49) 23.33 (3.74) 86.21 (10.43)
50–59 31.02 (3.19) 32.88 (5.39) 23.74 (4.03) 87.78 (10.54)
60 � 31.48 (3.19) 32.83 (5.21) 23.57 (4.04) 88.03 (10.00)

Region
Northeast 30.67 (3.97) 32.95 (5.78) 25.33 (3.43) 88.98 (11.30)
Midwest 30.25 (3.10) 33.71 (4.17) 23.65 (3.28) 87.71 (8.30)
South 30.38 (3.58) 31.62 (5.84) 23.21 (8.87) 85.28 (11.14)
West 30.18 (3.65) 32.72 (5.75) 23.10 (4.74) 86.23 (11.21)
International 30.10 (4.10) 31.14 (5.87) 23.52 (3.44) 84.90 (10.86)

Table 2
MANOVA Differences in Attitudes Toward Poverty-SF of Demographic Variables

Pillai’s trace F Hypothesis df Error df Sig. Partial eta squared

Region 0.082 2.72 12 1155 0.001� 0.027
Age 0.074 2.42 12 1143 0.004� 0.025

� significant � .005.

55CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

32.44 (SD � 5.53), and indicates play therapists
tend not to believe in stereotypes or myths about
individuals in poverty. Examples of stigma state-
ments were “poor people think they deserve to be
supported” and “Welfare mothers have babies to
get more money” (Yun & Weaver, 2010). The
average score for the Personal Deficiency scale
was 30.32 (SD � 3.55) and indicates participants
did not agree or strongly disagreed with state-
ments blaming individuals for being poor. Partic-
ipants did not feel a person’s individual deficit is
the primary cause of poverty and did not hold
individuals personally responsible for their pov-
erty. Personal deficiency statements include “poor
people are dishonest” and “poor people act differ-
ently.” The average score for Structural Perspec-
tive scale was the lowest, 23.64 (SD � 3.71), and
indicates play therapy participants more often dis-
agreed with statements that individuals are re-
sponsible for their financial problems and believe
policies or resources are needed to help those who
are poor. Examples of structural attitudes include
“poor people are discriminated against” and “peo-
ple are poor due to circumstances beyond their
control.” In comparison to other helping profes-
sionals, play therapists appear to have a more
favorable attitudes toward poverty than nurses
(e.g., Wittenauer et al., 2015) or school counselors
(e.g., Ricks, 2014) on the Total, Stigma, Personal
Deficiency, and Structural Perspective scales.

As a profession, participants’ scores in relation
to region and age indicated that there were varying
views on the cause of poverty. When exploring
regions in the United States, there were differ-
ences in how strongly participants believed struc-
tural causes influenced their attitudes and expla-
nations toward poverty. Participants living in the
Northeast leaned more strongly toward a struc-
tural determinant of poverty than participants in
the South. This finding may be due to variances in
poverty rates between the South and Northeast.
The South has the highest poverty rates in the
United States, 13.6%, while the Northeast ranks
the lowest, 10.3%. Nine of the 10 states with the
highest poverty rates in 2017 were Mississippi,
Louisiana, West Virginia, Kentucky, Alabama,
Arkansas, Oklahoma, South Carolina, and Ten-
nessee (Moore, 2018). Additionally, poverty rates
in the South have been continuously high for
decades (Jung et al., 2015), and the metro-
nonmetro poverty rate gap in the South has his-
torically been the largest (Farrigan, 2020). From
2014 –2018, the South’s nonmetro poverty rate

was 20.5%, nearly 6% higher than in the region’s
metro areas. In contrast, the Northeast has some of
the lowest poverty rates, under 10.5%, in New
Hampshire, Massachusetts, Connecticut, Rhode
Island, and New Jersey (U.S. Census, 2018). Ad-
ditionally, regional poverty rates for nonmetro and
metro areas in the Northeast were more alike from
2014 to 2018 (Farrigan, 2020). Results might in-
dicate that play therapists’ poverty attitudes are
potentially impacted by poverty rates in their re-
gion of the country. Play therapists may benefit
from understanding the impact of socioeconomic
and regional differences to work best with eco-
nomically disadvantaged clients.

Age was the second factor that resulted in vary-
ing views on poverty. Specifically, participants
who in the 50 –59 and 60 plus age groups more
strongly disagreed with a personal explanation
toward poverty than participants in the 30 –39 age
group. Life experience and exposure to varying
environmental influences may play an important
factor in poverty attitudes between the 30 –39
years old and 50 –59 and 60 plus groups. Partici-
pants who are 50 –59 and 60 plus were born in the
“Baby Boomers” generation. This period was a
time of prosperity and optimism potentially lead-
ing participants to hold a more positive view of
people experiencing poverty (Watts, 2010). Peo-
ple in the Baby Boomer generation may believe
that individuals who are poor are not the cause of
their poverty and need assistance to work their
way out of poverty. Baby Boomers’ experiences
are different than participants in their 30s, “Mil-
lennials.” Millennials experienced the largest eco-
nomic decline since the great depression, the end
of the Cold War, and deindustrialization which
encouraged people to become reliant on higher
education (Brooks, 2008). Millennials reliance on
higher education and the belief that education will
offer them greater prosperity has caused many to
accrue debt with reduced job opportunities and
Millennials living at or below the poverty line.
Their strong belief in education as a path out of
poverty may explain their slightly lower personal
explanation of poverty. Generational patterns and
exposures may help explain the differences in play
therapists’ attitudes toward poverty. As we gain a
better understanding of play therapist’s attitudes
toward poverty, this may allow us to better pin-
point opportunities to empower and partner with
individuals and communities to seek solutions that
will improve the health of those living in poverty.

56 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

Looking at poverty attitudes is important be-
cause the way play therapists and mental health
professionals view poverty potentially affects the
way they work with people in poverty (Anderson,
2018; Clark et al., 2017; Krumer-Nevo & Lev-
Wiesel, 2005). Play therapists who view poverty
from a systemic perspective may treat clients with
more respect and compassion. For instance, a play
therapist who holds a structural view of poverty
may provide traveling services to a homeless shel-
ter to support the potentially transient nature of
some families in poverty. In addition, play thera-
pists with a positive understanding of poverty may
create structures and policies that support the basic
and expanded needs of clients in poverty, such as
offering snacks during playtime or providing mul-
tidisciplinary resources in the community. Addi-
tionally, play therapists who hold a systemic view
of poverty may feel comfortable advocating for
their clients through social justice advocacy. Play
therapists can systemically advocate for their cli-
ents by offering availability of resources, increas-
ing involvement in governmental policies and
laws, and promoting positive relationships among
clients and resources (Crethar et al., 2008; Crethar
& Winterowd, 2012). Furthermore, play therapists
can be active through social justice advocacy by
going to client’s environments, such as their
schools and neighborhoods (Ceballos & Bratton,
2010; Sheely-Moore & Bratton, 2010), increasing
their self-awareness of their work with oppressed
children (Baggerly, 2006), and being aware of
oppression when looking at their client’s emotions
(O’Connor, 2005). The reasons discussed above
are just a few examples of how varied views of
poverty may impact the depth and extent play
therapists’ support clients in poverty. Play thera-
pists may benefit from professional development
that explores and challenges their attitudes toward
poverty on an ongoing basis.

Limitations and Future Directions

The limitations found in this study help inform
and shape the implications for future studies. A
limitation of the study was the use of a single
measure to evaluate the play therapists’ attitudes
toward poverty. Attitudes and poverty are com-
plex concepts that might benefit from being
looked at through multiple lenses. A future study
including multiple measures of attitudes toward
poverty and people who are poor would be bene-
ficial and give more breadth and depth to under-

standing play therapist’s attitudes toward poverty.
Additionally, qualitative data would allow respon-
dents to elaborate on their ideas and experiences.

The authors recruited from two consortiums
(APT and CESNET) and several play therapy-
related Facebook groups, which may have limited
the participation pool. We sought APT members,
as APT is the flagship professional organization in
the US for play therapists. Unfortunately, not all
play therapists are members of APT. Although we
expanded the search to sites that may reach other
potential participants (CESNET and Facebook),
recruitment through other mental health organiza-
tions, such as ACA, NASW, and APA, may
bridge gaps of play therapists who are not mem-
bers of APT. A narrow pool of applicants may
limit participation and bias the results. Future re-
searchers could recruit through multiple profes-
sional organizations to increase the diversity of
potential participants.

A few participants in this study had a strong
reaction to the ATP-SF. Participants voluntarily
emailed the researchers their thoughts and ex-
periences taking the survey. Some participants
had a negative reaction to the ATP-SF, such as
a dislike for the wording of the measure, and
they reported that the ATP-SF made false as-
sumptions and generalizations. The purpose of
this research was to evaluate the quantitative
responses to the survey, and participants’ strong
reactions indicate a qualitative study may be
needed as well.

Conclusion

The current study is the first study exploring
play therapists’ attitudes toward poverty. Overall,
the authors found that region and age play a factor
in how play therapists view poverty. The findings
affirm the importance of continued professional
development around cultural competence when
working with diverse populations. Play therapist’s
awareness and knowledge impacts the way they
work with diverse clients, particularly clients in
poverty. Based on findings from this study, we
believe it is important for play therapists to exam-
ine their own views and explore factors that im-
pact their views of others to ensure they are pro-
viding culturally informed practices and
ultimately respond with more sensitivity for those
struggling in poverty.

57CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

References

American Counseling Association. (2014). 2014
ACA code of ethics. https://www.counseling.org/
knowledge-center

American Psychological Association. (2017a). Ethi-
cal principles of psychologists and code of con-
duct. https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/

American Psychological Association. (2017b). Mul-
ticultural guidelines: An Ecological approach to
context, identity, and intersectionality. http://www.
apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

Anderson, A. (2018). Attitudes towards poverty of
human service practitioners who provide direct
service to families (Publication No. 10979759)
[Doctoral dissertation, Capella University]. Pro-
Quest Dissertations and Theses Global.

Appelbaum, L. D., Lennon, M. C., & Lawrence Aber,
J. (2006). When effort is threatening: The influ-
ence of the belief in a just world on Americans’
attitudes toward antipoverty policy. Political Psy-
chology, 27, 387– 402. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

Association for Play Therapy. (2019). Play therapy
best practices: Clinical, professional, and ethical
issues. https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/
publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

Atherton, C. R., Gemmel, R. J., Haagenstad, S., Holt,
D. J., Jensen, L., O’Hara, D., & Rehner, T. (1993).
Measuring attitudes toward poverty: A new scale.
Social Work Research & Abstracts, 29(4), 28–30.
https://doi.org/10.1093/swra/29.4.28

Baggerly, J. (2006). Service learning with children
affected by poverty: Facilitating multicultural
competence in counseling education students.
Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Develop-
ment, 34(4), 244–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/j
.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

Beeghley, L. (1988). Individual and structural expla-
nations of poverty. Population Research and Pol-
icy Review, 7(3), 201–222. https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF02456102

Brady, D. (2019). Theories of the causes of poverty.
Annual Review of Sociology, 45(1), 155–175.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-
022550

Bray, S. S., & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2015). School
counselors’ ways of knowing and social orienta-
tion in relationship to poverty beliefs. Journal of
Counseling & Development, 93, 312–320. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

Brooks, C. (2008). A legacy of leadership: Gover-
nors and American history. University of Pennsyl-
vania Press.

Capell, J., Veenstra, G., & Dean, E. (2007). Cultural
competence in healthcare: Critical analysis of the
construct, its assessment and implications. Journal
of Theory Construction & Testing, 11(1), 30–37.

Ceballos, P., & Bratton, S. (2010). Empowering La-
tino families: Effects of a culturally responsive
intervention for low-income immigrant Latino par-
ents on children’s behaviors and parental stress.
Psychology in the Schools, 47(8), 761–775. https://
doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

Ciment, J. (2013). Poverty. In C. G. Bates & J.
Ciment (Eds.), Global social issues: An encyclo-
pedia (pp. 846– 855). Routledge.

Clark, M., Moe, J., & Hays, D. (2017). The relation-
ship between counselors’ multicultural counseling
competence and poverty beliefs. Counselor Edu-
cation and Supervision, 56(4), 259–273. https://doi
.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.

Cooper, K., & Stewart, K. (2013). Does money affect
children’s outcomes? Joseph Rowntree Founda-
tion. https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-
affect-children’s-outcomes

Crethar, H., Rivera, E., & Nash, S. (2008). In search
of common threads: Linking multicultural, femi-
nist, and social justice counseling paradigms. Jour-
nal of Counseling and Development, 86(3), 269–
278. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008
.tb00509.x

Crethar, H., & Winterowd, C. (2012). Values and
social justice in counseling. Counseling and Val-
ues, 57(1), 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-
007X.2012.00001.x

Cummins, I. (2018). Poverty, inequality and social
work: The impact of neoliberalism and austerity
politics on welfare provision. Policy Press.

Davids, Y., & Gouws, A. (2013). Monitoring percep-
tions of the causes of poverty in South Africa.
Social Indicators Research, 110(3), 1201–1220.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9980-9

Farrigan, T. (2020, 12 February). Rural Poverty and
Well-being. USDA. https://www.ers.usda.gov/top
ics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-
being/

Farthing, R. (2014). Family poverty. In J. Treas, J.
Scott, & M. Richards (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell
companion to the sociology of families (pp. 132–
154). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/97811183
74085.ch7

Feagin, J. R. (1975). Subordinating the poor: Welfare
and American beliefs. Prentice Hall.

Garrett, P. M. (2018). Welfare words: Critical social
work and social policy. Sage. https://doi.org/10
.4135/9781526418661

Gil, E. (2005). From sensitivity to competence in
working across cultures. In A. A. Drewes & E. Gil
(Eds.), Cultural issues in play therapy (pp. 3–25).
The Guilford Press.

Gilbert, J., Goode, T. D., & Dunne, C. (2007). Cur-
ricula enhancement module series: Cultural aware-
ness. National Center for Cultural Competence.

58 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://www.counseling.org/knowledge-center

https://www.counseling.org/knowledge-center

https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/

http://www.apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

http://www.apa.org/about/policy/multicultural-guidelines

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2006.00506.x

https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

https://www.a4pt.org/resource/resmgr/publications/best_practices_-_sept_2019

https://doi.org/10.1093/swra/29.4.28

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.2006.tb00043.x

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02456102

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02456102

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-022550

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-073018-022550

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12029

https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20502

https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12084

https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-affect-children%26#x2019;s-outcomes

https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/does-money-affect-children%26#x2019;s-outcomes

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008.tb00509.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2008.tb00509.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-007X.2012.00001.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-007X.2012.00001.x

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-011-9980-9

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://www.ers.usda.gov/topics/rural-economy-population/rural-poverty-well-being/

https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118374085.ch7

https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118374085.ch7

https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526418661

https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526418661

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/
awareness

Goldsmith, W. W., & Blakely, E. J. (2010). Separate
societies: Poverty and inequality in U.S. cities
(2nd ed.). Temple University Press.

Gordon, D., & Townsend, P. (Eds.). (2000). Bread-
line Europe: The measurement of poverty. Polity
Press.

Haughton, J., & Khandker, S. R. (2009). Handbook
on poverty and inequality. World Bank. http://
documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/4880814681
57174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

Howard, C., Freeman, A., Wilson, A., & Brown, E.
(2017). Poverty. Public Opinion Quarterly, 81(3),
769–789. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfx022

Hunt, M. O., & Bullock, H. E. (2016). Ideologies and
beliefs about poverty. In D. Brady & L. M. Burton
(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social science of
poverty (pp. 93–116). Oxford University Press.

Jordan, G. (2004). The causes of poverty cultural vs.
structural: Can these be a synthesis? Perspectives
in Public Affairs, 1, 18–34.

Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of
stereotyping in system-justification and production
of false consciousness. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 33(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

Jung, S., Cho, S., & Roberts, R. (2015). The impact
of government funding of poverty reduction pro-
grams: Government funding and poverty reduction
in the Southern U.S. Papers in Regional Science,
94(3), 653– 675. https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs
.12089

Koball, H., & Jiang, Y. (2018). Basic facts about
low-income children: Children under 18 years,
2016. National Center for Children in Poverty.
https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10
.7916/D8JS9Q92

Kraus, S. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of
behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical litera-
ture. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
21, 58–75. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616
7295211007

Krumer-Nevo, M., & Lev-Wiesel, R. (2005). Atti-
tudes of social work students towards clients with
basic needs. Journal of Social Work Education,
41(3), 545–556. https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE
.2005.200303137

Levin, L., & Schwartz-Tayri, T. (2017). Attitudes
towards poverty, organizations, ethics and morals:
Israeli social workers’ shared decision making.
Health Expectations, 20(3), 448– 458. https://doi
.org/10.1111/hex.12472

Major, B., & O’Brien, L. (2005). The social psychol-
ogy of stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56,
393– 421. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych
.56.091103.070137

Mead, L. M. (2011). From prophecy to charity: How
to help the poor. The AEI Press.

Moore, K. A., Redd, Z., Burkhauser, M., Mbwana,
K., & Collins, A. (2009). Children in poverty:
Trends, consequences, and policy options (Re-
search Brief No. 2009 –11). Child Trends. https://
www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/
2009-11ChildreninPoverty

Moore, R. (2018). Poverty statistics for Southern
States. Southern League Conference. https://www
.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub�580

Morrow, K. A., & Deidan, C. T. (1992). Bias in the
counseling process: How to recognize and avoid it.
Journal of Counseling & Development, 70(5),
571–577. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676
.1992.tb01663.x

National Association of Social Workers. (2015).
Standards and indicators for cultural competence
in social work practice. https://www.socialworkers
.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket�PonPTDEBrn
4%3D&portalid�0

National Scientific Council on the Developing Child.
(2015). Supportive relationships and active skill-
building strengthen the foundations of resilience.
Working Paper 13. https://developingchild.harvard
.edu/resources/supportive-relationships-and-act
ive-skill-building-strengthen-the-foundations-of-
resilience/

New Freedom Commission on Mental Health.
(2003). Achieving the promise: Transforming
Mental health care in America: Final report (Re-
port No. SMA-03–3832). Department of Health
and Human Services.

Noone, J., Sideras, S., Gubrud-Howe, P., Voss, H., &
Mathews, L. (2012). Influence of a poverty simu-
lation on nursing student attitudes toward poverty.
The Journal of Nursing Education, 51(11), 617–
622. https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-201
20914-01

O’Connor, K. (2005). Addressing diversity issues in
play therapy. Professional Psychology, Research
and Practice, 36(5), 566–573. https://doi.org/10
.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

Poverty Programs. (2017). Poverty statistics: USA
poverty. http://www.povertyprogram.com/usa.php

Ratts, M. J., Singh, A. A., Nassar-McMillan, S.,
Butler, S. K., & McCullough, J. R. (2016). Multi-
cultural and social justice counseling competen-
cies. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and De-
velopment, 44(1), 28– 48. https://doi.org/10.1002/
jmcd.12035

Ricks, L. A. (2014). Attributes, attitudes, and per-
ceived self-efficacy levels of school counselors to-
ward poverty [Unpublished doctoral dissertation].
Auburn University. https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/
handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School
%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence�2

59CULTURAL COMPETENCE AND POVERTY

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/awareness

https://nccc.georgetown.edu/curricula/documents/awareness

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/488081468157174849/Handbook-on-poverty-and-inequality

https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfx022

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs.12089

https://doi.org/10.1111/pirs.12089

https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10.7916/D8JS9Q92

https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10.7916/D8JS9Q92

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295211007

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295211007

https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE.2005.200303137

https://doi.org/10.5175/JSWE.2005.200303137

https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.12472

https://doi.org/10.1111/hex.12472

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/2009-11ChildreninPoverty

https://www.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub=580

https://www.slcatlanta.org/research/index.php?pub=580

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1992.tb01663.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1992.tb01663.x

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

https://www.socialworkers.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=PonPTDEBrn4%3D&portalid=0

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

Supportive Relationships and Active Skill-Building Strengthen the Foundations of Resilience

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120914-01

https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120914-01

https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.36.5.566

http://www.povertyprogram.com/usa.php

https://doi.org/10.1002/jmcd.12035

https://doi.org/10.1002/jmcd.12035

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

https://etd.auburn.edu/bitstream/handle/10415/4207/Poverty%20and%20School%20Counseling_L_Ricks ;sequence=2

Royce, E. (2018). Poverty & power: The problem of
structural inequality (3rd ed.). Rowman & Little-
field Publishers, Inc.

Shapiro, S. M. (2004). The relationship among men-
tal health clinicians’ beliefs in a just world, atti-
tudes toward the poor, and beliefs about helping
the poor [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Wal-
den University.

Sheely-Moore, A., & Bratton, S. (2010). A strengths-
based parenting intervention with low-income Af-
rican American families. Professional School
Counseling, 13(3), 175–183. https://doi.org/10
.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

Siegel, C., Haugland, G., & Schore, R. (2005). The
interface of cultural competence and evidence-
based practices. In R. E. Drake, M. R. Merrens, &
D. W. Lynde (Eds.), Evidence-based mental health
practice: A textbook (pp. 273–299). Norton.

Siu, A. F. Y. (2010). Play therapy in Hong Kong:
Opportunities and challenges. International Jour-
nal of Play Therapy, 19(4), 235–243. https://doi
.org/10.1037/a0020641

Sturm, D. C. (2008). The impact of client level of
poverty on counselor attitudes and attributions
about the client [Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion]. University of North Carolina at Charlotte.

Sue, D. W., Arredondo, P., & McDavis, R. J. (1992).
Multicultural counseling competencies and stan-
dards: A call to the profession. Journal of Multi-
cultural Counseling and Development, 20(2), 64–
88. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992
.tb00563.x

Sue, D. W., Ivey, A. E., & Pedersen, P. B. (1996). A
theory of multicultural counseling and therapy.
Brooks/Cole.

Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). Counseling the cul-
turally diverse: Theory and practice (6th ed.). Wi-
ley.

Sue, S. (2006). Cultural competency: From philoso-
phy to research and practice. Journal of Commu-
nity Psychology, 34(2), 237–245. https://doi.org/10
.1002/jcop.20095

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using
multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Pearson Educa-
tion.

Turner, M. A., & Rawlings, L. A. (2005). Ten lessons
for policy and practice. The Urban Institute. http://
webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_
Poverty_Brief

Turner, N. (2011). How poverty hurts our children.
Nursing New Zealand, 17(6), 33.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2018). How the Census Bureau
measures poverty. United States Census Bureau.
https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/
poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

Van Allen, K., & Sterling, Y. (2011). Pediatric nurses
address children and the economy: Part 1. The
impact of poverty on children and families. Jour-
nal of Pediatric Nursing, 26(4), 369–372. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

van Heerde, J., & Hudson, D. (2010). “The righteous
consider the cause of the poor”? Public attitudes
towards poverty in developing countries. Political
Studies, 58(3), 389– 409. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

Watts, D. (2010). Baby boomers. Dictionary of
American government and politics. Edinburgh
University Press.

Weiss-Gal, I., Benyamini, Y., Ginzburg, K., Savaya,
R., & Peled, E. (2009). Social workers’ and service
users’ causal attributions for poverty. Social Work,
54(2), 125–133. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2
.125

Wickham, S., Anwar, E., Barr, B., Law, C., & Tay-
lor-Robinson, D. (2016). Poverty and child health
in the U. K.: Using evidence for action. Archives of
Disease in Childhood, 101 759–766. https://doi
.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

Wittenauer, J., Ludwick, R., Baughman, K., & Fish-
bein, R. (2015). Surveying the hidden attitudes of
hospital nurses’ towards poverty. Journal of Clin-
ical Nursing, 24(15–16):2184–2191. https://doi
.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

Wolff, J. (2019). Poverty. Philosophy Compass,
14(12). Advance online publication. https://doi
.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

World Bank Institute. (2005). Poverty manual. http://
siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/
PovertyManual

Yun, S., & Weaver, R. (2010). Development and
validation of a short form of the Attitude Toward
Poverty Scale. Advances in Social Work, 11(2),
174–187. https://doaj.org/article/00914568df
9245f8841fcc3a769281e4. https://doi.org/10
.18060/437

Received February 4, 2020
Revision received June 26, 2020

Accepted October 5, 2020 �

60 CHASE AND OPIOLA

T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.

https://doi.org/10.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

https://doi.org/10.5330/PSC.n.2010-13.175

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020641

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020641

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992.tb00563.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1912.1992.tb00563.x

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20095

https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20095

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

http://webarchive.urban.org/UploadedPDF/311204_Poverty_Brief

https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

https://www.census.gov/topics/income-poverty/poverty/guidance/poverty-measures.html

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2011.04.026

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2009.00800.x

https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2.125

https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/54.2.125

https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2014-306746

https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

https://doi.org/10.1111/jocn.12794

https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12635

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/PGLP/Resources/PovertyManual

https://doaj.org/article/00914568df9245f8841fcc3a769281e4

https://doaj.org/article/00914568df9245f8841fcc3a769281e4

https://doi.org/10.18060/437

https://doi.org/10.18060/437

  • Cultural Competence and Poverty: Exploring Play Therapists’ Attitudes
  • Cultural Competence
    Poverty
    Poverty Attitudes
    Play Therapy
    Method
    Procedure
    Participants
    Instrument
    Data Analysis
    Results
    Differences by Region and Age
    Discussion
    Limitations and Future Directions
    Conclusion
    References

What Will You Get?

We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

Premium Quality

Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

Experienced Writers

Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

On-Time Delivery

Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

24/7 Customer Support

Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

Complete Confidentiality

Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

Authentic Sources

We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

Moneyback Guarantee

Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

Order Tracking

You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

image

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

image

Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

Preferred Writer

Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

Grammar Check Report

Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

One Page Summary

You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

Plagiarism Report

You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

Free Features $66FREE

  • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
  • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
  • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
  • Paper Formatting $05FREE
  • Cover Page $05FREE
  • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
  • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
  • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
  • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
image

Our Services

Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

  • On-time Delivery
  • 24/7 Order Tracking
  • Access to Authentic Sources
Academic Writing

We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

Professional Editing

We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

Thorough Proofreading

We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

image

Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

Check Out Our Sample Work

Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

Categories
All samples
Essay (any type)
Essay (any type)
The Value of a Nursing Degree
Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
Nursing
2
View this sample

It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

0+

Happy Clients

0+

Words Written This Week

0+

Ongoing Orders

0%

Customer Satisfaction Rate
image

Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

image

We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

  • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
  • Customized writing as per your needs.

We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

  • Proactive analysis of your writing.
  • Active communication to understand requirements.
image
image

We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

  • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
  • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
Place an Order Start Chat Now
image

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy