I need a 3 page research paper on
Title: Addictive being young and older on Social Media, why activities outdoors can prevent addiction
In the attached zip file, I have provided 10 journals that you need to use for this research paper.
In the word doc, I have shared the topic and sub-topics that you have to use. And it also has guidelines from the teacher for this paper.
Due on Saturday, 13th March 4PM PST
REQUIREMENT: Write a RESEARCH PAPER
Title:
Addictive being young and older on Social Media, why activities outdoors can prevent addiction.
Hypothesis:
I predict younger people use social media more than older people.
3 pages total.
1-2 paragraphs intro
And then elaborate on 3 topics, using the bullet points and corresponding journals.
What’s your feeling about the title and why. Use 10 journals, but please don’t copy authors, only (last names and dates) and While reading they might mention a author you get click that author to see what they’re talking about talk about that to, this is a research paper.
I have sub topics I need you to focus on.
Please find things to quote on topics and on bullet points. please stay on the topics and my title
Take the information out that you would like to write about on the bullet points I added below and particularly look at the abstract, method, discussion and results in the journal so you focus on the title and the bullet point. Its a research survey paper
3 Topics (and corresponding sub-topics in each section) to be used in the writing:
1. Being on Social Media and addiction to on social media
Interaction online and or activities
Addiction with social media
Using social media more and more
2. Social Media and a Mental use
Being unhappy online while often scrolling (McNally et al)
Having to be liked on social media ( Liang et al)
Mental unstable and using social media ( Aschbrenner et al)
Being on social media, being sad, happy, or angry ( McNally et al)
3. Addictive and Social Media isolation or addiction
· Addictive to being on social media (Connolly et al)
· Social media more than interaction (Ulusoy et al)
· Being alone and coping with social media (Bungert et al)
GUIDELINES from Teacher
Introduce the reader to the general problem and provide any necessary definitions. And clearly state the purpose of the paper (i.e. I want to see a sentence that looks something like this: “The purpose of this paper is…”)
Provide enough background. You want the reader to understand the subject and some of the general background and past research on the topic. You need at least 10 sources for the whole paper, and most of them will be in the literature review (so that’s about 3-4 per sub-section). Focus on peer-reviewed journals.
Citations in APA style. (Only last names and the year, and it differs by the number of authors on the paper). Use online resources for help.
Then build support for the hypothesis. Take everything you’ve already written (definitions, background, problem), and tie it all together to explain why and how your hypothesis answers the research question, or addresses the problem or the purpose of the study.
Clearly state the hypothesis at the end (in statement form, with clear directional relationships between variables). This should not be a question, and you have to propose a direction (not something generic or going in both directions for example)
Introduction/Literature Review
Format of the introduction and literature review:
1. First 1-2 paragraphs
The first sentence should engage the reader (basically this is why they should read your paper)
Then define your variables and important terms
Finally, clearly state the purpose of your paper
2. The next 3-5 pages
This is where you go into the past research (this is the literature review)
Subheadings (level 2 in APA format) are helpful to help guide your lit review. Think of 3-4 sub-topics that are related to your variables.
Example: Let’s say that I think perceptions of sexual assault victims will be different depending on how the research is presented (from the victim’s perspective vs from the perpetrator’s perspective) and I think this difference will be further influenced by gender. So I can turn these three things into sub-topics:
Perceptions of sexual assault victims (but doesn’t have to be specifically about perspective taking or based on gender)
Perspective taking and feelings of empathy (doesn’t have to relate specifically to sexual assault or gender)
Differences of perception by gender (doesn’t have to be specifically about sexual assault)
The point of the literature review is to present the past research on the different topic, then you tie them all together at the end of your intro/lit review
3. Last 1-2 paragraphs
This is where you tie together all the research and state your hypothesis
Example: I presented on perceptions, perspective taking, and gender, and here is how they are all connected…
State your hypothesis as your expected findings in how the variables will relate
Example: “I believe that perceptions of victims of sexual assault will be more negative when the information is presented from the perpetrator’s perspective, but that this difference will be only be true for men”
Other things that should be included:
Title page in APA style with running head different first page and page numbers in the top right
You can begin an abstract (you will add a sentence or two with each section that you write, Intro, Methods, Results, Discussion) and I won’t actually grade it until the final submission.
Journals/Joural 1 x
A Closer Look at Appearance and Social Media: Measuring Activity, Self-Presentation, and Social Comparison and Their Associations With Emotional Adjustment
Keywords:
depression, body image, social media, body dysmorphic symptoms, social comparison
Abstract (English):
Social media use links with 2 major concerns of adolescents, namely, appearance and comparing favorably with others. Founded on theory, our purpose was to develop a reliable and valid measure of appearance preoccupation online, the Social Media Appearance Preoccupation Scale (SMAPS). In Study 1 (N = 283 Grade 9–12 students), Australian adolescents completed surveys containing 21 SMAPS items. After psychometric analyses, 18 retained items loaded highly on factors tapping (a) online self-presentation, (b) appearance-related activity online, or (c) appearance comparison. The items loading on each factor had high interitem correlations, and girls had higher SMAPS scores than boys. In Study 2 (N = 327 Australian university students <26 years), the SMAPS was confirmed and validated with a range of measures of social media use, emotional adjustment, appearance concerns, and social behaviors. Factor loadings were invariant by gender, SMAPS subscale scores had very small correlations with age, and incremental validity was tested and supported. Additionally, SMAPS subscale scores interacted with general social media use, adding to the explanation of appearance anxiety. The SMAPS will be a useful resource for the study of appearance-related social media use and interactions with friends and others online. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)
Population:
Human
Male
Female
Location:
Australia
Age Group:
Adolescence (13-17 yrs)
Adulthood (18 yrs & older)
Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs)
Tests & Measures:
Social Media Appearance Preoccupation Scale DOI: 10.1037/t76315-000
Method
Participants and procedure
The participants were 283 high school students ages 13 to 18 years (M = 16.62, SD = .95; 47% male and 53% female) drawn from three schools in an urban area of Australia. The schools were moderate in size and contained Grades 7 to 12. The schools reported that their students were generally from low-middle to high-middle socioeconomic backgrounds. To measure sociocultural background, participants were asked to endorse as many options as applied, with most (84%) of the participants endorsing white Australian, 18% instead or in addition endorsing Asian, two endorsing Australian first peoples/Torres Strait Islander/Pacific Islander, and 11 also describing a diverse range of other backgrounds. Two participants did not respond to any of the social media items and were removed from the dataset, leaving a final sample size of 281.
Approval for this study was obtained from the Griffith University Human Research Ethics Committee. Attempts were made to recontact 335 students from three schools that had participated in an earlier study (not focused on social media use) to obtain consent from parents and adolescents to participate in this study. In the initial consent process, students who returned parent and personal consent forms (regardless of participation) were included in a draw to win 5 $100 gift vouchers to a store of their choice. Overall, 309 families were able to be contacted and 26 parents or students declined to participate, resulting in a 92% response rate. Students from two schools completed the 45-min survey either by mail or online. One school opted to have surveys completed during school time under research assistant supervision. Each participant in this study received a $20 gift card when the survey was returned.
Measures
To investigate appearance-related preoccupation in adolescents’ use of social media, participants responded to 21 items that were developed to tap into the five aforementioned themes identified in the literature including image activities (e.g., “I prefer to upload photos of myself to social media where I look fit and healthy”), investment and self-presentation (e.g., “When I upload photos of myself I usually use filters or alter/change them to make myself look better”), social comparison (e.g., “I feel like I want to change my diet after viewing other people’s pictures online”), active versus passive (e.g., “I approve photos of myself before anyone can tag them” vs. “I am often dissatisfied with my weight or looks in my social media pictures”), and negative responses (e.g., “Seeing pictures of others makes me feel down on myself”). To the creation and wording of such items, both literature on social media use and existing measures related to sociocultural and body image theories of social grooming (Dunbar, 1996; Tufekci, 2008), social comparison (Festinger, 1954; Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), self-presentation (Goffman, 1959; Leary, 1996; Manning, 1992), and self-objectification (Frederickson & Roberts, 1997; Noll & Fredrickson, 1998) were considered. Additionally, attention was also given to represent a mix of body image concerns such as weight and diet, fitness, health and muscularity, as well as general appearance in the wording of the 21 items. Furthermore, 10 additional items were included as filler items that asked about general social media use (seven items), personal trolling behavior (one item), and interactions with friends online (two items). Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated the tendency to participate in these online activities and to experience stronger feelings associated with these online interactions.
Results
Item reduction and factor analyses
The 21 items were evaluated to assess whether they met two assumptions of exploratory factor analysis (Hair, Black, Babin, Anderson, & Tatham, 2006). First, Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, χ2(210) = 4,594.3, p < .001, indicating an acceptable number of significant correlations among variables. Second, the Kaiser Meyer Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy for the overall sample was good (.93).
The exploratory factor analysis was conducted using principle axis factoring (PAF) with oblique rotation. PAF was used given that this method is recommended for psychological data because it allows for measurement error (Xia, Green, Xu, & Thompson, 2019). In line with best practices, the number of factors to extract was based on Velicer’s minimum average parcel (MAP) test and parallel analysis (Hayton, Allen, & Scarpello, 2004; O’Connor, 2000). When we conducted a PAF, three factors were extracted with eigenvalues over 1 (10.63, 2.02, and 1.46) and Velicer’s MAP test and parallel analysis supported the extraction of three factors, with all three eigenvalues greater than the first three eigenvalues calculated using parallel analysis (O’Connor, 2000). The variance accounted for in the items was 67.20%. As shown in Table 1, the pattern matrix showed loadings above .5 for all but two items and no high cross-loadings for all items, with the exception of one item. Thus, we removed this cross-loading item and removed the two items with loadings below .50 and the PAF was repeated.
appearance teasing experienced and witnessed, and general social media use.
Method
Participants and procedure
The participants were 327 university students aged 17 to 25 years (M = 20.1, SD = 1.1; 68% female; note: Two participants were just under age 17). To measure sociocultural background, participants were allowed to endorse as many options as applied, with most (84%) of the participants endorsing white Australian, 12% instead or in addition endorsing Asian, 3% endorsing Australian first peoples/Torres Strait Islander/Pacific Islander, and 9% describing a diverse range of other backgrounds. Participants also reported on their mother and father educational levels: Mother’s education: 21% did not graduate high school, 15% graduated high school, 22% completed vocational training, 39% undertook some university study. Father’s education: 16% did not graduate high school, 24% graduated high school, 22% completed vocational training, 38% undertook some university study. Overall 344 students attempted the questionnaire, but 15 participants were removed for missing data on all measures (i.e., they only completed one or two pages of the questionnaire) and two were removed because they were over age 25. The upper age limit was 25, given that most university students are below this age and because social media use and patterns can change as individuals get older (Olson, O’Brien, Rogers, & Charness, 2012).
Measures
Social media appearance preoccupation
The final 18-item SMAPS from Study 1 was used to measure social appearance online self-presentation (seven items), appearance-related activity (five items), and appearance comparison (six items). Responses options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). See the results section for further details.
Depressive symptoms
Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire—Short Version (Angold & Costello, 1987), consisting of a series of 13 descriptive phrases about how the adolescent has been feeling or behaving recently (e.g., “I felt miserable or unhappy”). Items are rated from 1 (not true) to 5 (very true), and the total score was calculated by averaging all items. Higher scores indicate more depressive symptoms, Cronbach’s α = .95.
Social anxiety
The Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (La Greca & Lopez, 1998) assessed symptoms of social anxiety. Eighteen descriptive items (e.g., “I worry about doing something new in front of others”) were rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (not true) to 5 (very true). A total score was calculated by averaging all items, with higher scores indicating higher social anxiety symptoms, Cronbach’s α = .95.
Appearance anxiety symptoms
The Appearance Anxiety Inventory (Roberts et al., 2018; Veale et al., 2014) was used to measure symptoms of body image anxiety. The Appearance Anxiety Inventory is a 10-item scale (e.g., “I try to camouflage or alter aspects of my appearance”). Participants indicated on a 5-point scale the frequency with which they experienced symptoms 0 (never) to 4 (always or almost always). The total score was formed by summing all items, where higher scores reflected greater appearance symptoms, Cronbach’s α = .93.
Disordered eating and related behaviors
Six items from the Eating Attitudes Test-26 (Garner, Olmsted, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982) were used to measure disordered eating and related behaviors in adolescents. Questions related to eating binges, vomiting, laxative and diet pills, excessive exercise, use of pills or powders to control muscle mass and skipping meals (e.g., “In the past 6 months have you. . . . Gone on eating binges where you feel that you may not be able to stop eating?”). Participants responded on a scale measuring the frequency of these behaviors that ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (once a day or more). A composite score was formed by averaging the item responses. Higher scores indicate more disordered eating, Cronbach’s α = .74.
General interpersonal stress
Using items from the Responses to Stress Questionnaire (Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, & Saltzman, 2000), participants reported their personal experience with 10 interpersonal stressors in the past 6 months (e.g., “Being around people who are rude or mean; Having problems with a friend”). Responses ranged from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). The items were averaged to form a composite interpersonal stress score, Cronbach’s α was. .84.
Coping flexibility
One six-item scale from Self-Perceived Flexible Coping with Stress (Zimmer-Gembeck et al., 2018) was used to measure the perceived capacity to use multiple coping strategies when facing stressful events (e.g., “I can come up with lots of ways to make myself feel better if I am stressed”). Response options ranged from 1 (not at all true) to 7 (totally true). A composite was formed by averaging the items, Cronbach’s α was. .84.
Sexual harassment
Sexual harassment was measured using five items similar to the 14-item American Association of University Women (AAUW) Education Foundation questionnaire (AAUW, 2001). To produce five items, AAUW items were condensed so that much of the content of the 14 items was contained in the five items. However, one item deemed too sensitive (e.g., sexual assault) was removed. Four items gathered reports of verbal and/or nonphysical sexual harassment experiences (“made sexual comments, jokes, gestures or looks at you”; “showed, given, or left you sexual pictures, photos, notes or messages”; “written sexual graffiti about you on a wall or other place”; “spread sexual rumours about you”) and one item related to physical sexual harassment (“flashed you some sexual part of their body”; “touched, grabbed, or pinched you in a sexual way”; or “grabbed you or pulled at your clothing in a sexual way”). Response options ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Items were averaged so that a higher score indicated greater variety and frequency of sexual harassment, Cronbach’s α was .81.
Social media appearance teasing experienced and witnessed
One item, with two parts, based on the Perceptions of Teasing Scale (Thompson, Cattarin, Fowler, & Fisher, 1995), assessed frequency of appearance-related teasing from the same- and other-sex (“In the past year, how often have you been teased about the way you look on social media?”). The two parts involved reporting about teasing by the same-sex separately from other-sex peers. A second item with two parts asked about witnessing online appearance-related teasing by the same- and the other-sex (“In the past year, how often have you witnessed but not taken part in teasing on social media?”) Responses ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Items were averaged to form a composite to indicate online appearance-related teasing separate from a composite of witnessing online appearance-related teasing, Cronbach’s α was .85 for online appearance-related teasing and .93 for witnessing online appearance-related victimization.
Appearance-related support from others
Support about appearance from important others was measured with a four-item scale adapted from the Important Other Climate Questionnaire (Williams et al., 2006) and the Body Acceptance by Others Scale (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) and was used to assess participants’ perceptions of the support they receive from important others in their lives when they feel bad about their appearance (e.g., “Important people in my life make me feel important regardless of how I look”). Participants responded on a scale ranging from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5 (very often or always true), where the total score was formed using the average of the items. Higher scores indicate greater perceived support from significant others, Cronbach’s α = .92.
Social media use
Four items from the Facebook Intensity Scale (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007) were modified (to focus on social media rather than Facebook specifically) to measure emotional connectedness and integration of social media use in daily life (e.g., “Using social media is part of my everyday activity”). Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), and items were averaged to produce a total use score, Cronbach’s α = .87.
Results
Confirmatory factor analyses
The confirmatory factor analyses of the 18-item SMAPS was conducted using MPlus with robust maximum likelihood estimation. The three latent factors of Online Self-Presentation, Appearance-Related Activity, and Appearance Comparison were free to covary with each other. The results
supported the three-factor structure. As shown in Table 2, all loadings were .60 or above, with one exception. After freeing four covariances between errors, the model had an adequate fit to the data, χ2(128) = 323.60, p < .001, comparative fit index (CFI) = .96, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .068 (90% confidence interval [CI; .059, .078], p = .001).
The factor structure was invariant by gender. More specifically, when the loadings of all items on the three factors were freed to differ (as well as the covariances between factors) for young women and men, the model had an adequate fit to the data, χ2(260) = 511.51, p < .001, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .055 (90% CI [.048, .062], p = .139). However, when all loadings were fixed to gender equality and the fit of these two models were compared, there was no significant difference in the model fits, χdiff2(132) = 187.91, p > .05. Thus, there was no indication that the loadings or the covariances between factors differed significantly for young women and men. For example, the correlation between online self-presentation and appearance comparison was r = .78 for young women and r = .76 for young men. The correlation between online self-presentation and appearance-related activity was r = .41 for young women and r = .45 for young men, and the correlation between appearance comparison and appearance-related activity was r = .39 for young women and r = .43 for young men.
Finally, given the high correlation between online self-presentation and appearance comparison, we also fit a two-factor model with all online self-presentation and appearance comparison items freed to load only on one factor, whereas other items were freed to load only on a second factor of appearance-related online activity. The fit of this model was poor, χ2(130) = 624.23, p < .001, CFI = .89, RMSEA = .108 (90% CI [.100, .117], p < .001).
Top of Form
Discussion
Many adolescents and young adults report a great deal of concern about their appearance, including being judged by others or not conforming to societal ideals (Fardouly & Vartanian, 2015; Holland & Tiggemann, 2016; Ricciardelli & Yager, 2016; Webb & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2014). Such concerns can result in excessive appearance anxiety, body dysmorphia, body dissatisfaction, or disordered eating, all of which can interfere with tasks of daily living, health, and happiness (Roberts et al., 2018; Veale et al., 2014; Zimmer-Gembeck, Webb, Farrell, & Waters, 2018). This preoccupation with appearance may be fuelled even more today by the excessive social comparison and image displays that are a basic part of using social media. Despite this apparent appearance preoccupation and the evidence that it is tied to historical change in social media platforms and their use (Saunders & Eaton, 2018; Seabrook et al., 2016), we could locate no standard measure to assess appearance-related social media use and preoccupation, which could be applied across a range of contemporary social media platforms while also being founded in social theories. Such a measure was needed, as it may be appearance-related use and preoccupation with social media self-presentation and comparisons that will help to identify adolescents and young adults at the highest risk of appearance-related clinical disorders and other related problems. Thus, in this study, a new measure, the SMAPS, was developed and tested in high school (Study 1) and university (Study 2) students. Once developed and confirmed, the SMAPS subscales were validated against a range of measures of social media use and appearance sensitivities, and the subscales were also examined as correlates of emotional maladjustment. Attention was also given to gender differences and invariance, age associations, and the possible of interactions of SMAPs subscales with other social media measures when predicting emotional and appearance-related adjustment problems.
Three SMAPS subscales were found in exploratory analyses with high school students and were confirmed in a second study with university students. The SMAPS subscales included Online Self-Presentation, Appearance-Related Online Activity, and Appearance Comparison. These subscales, which were assessed with only 18 items, will allow for consideration of general appearance-related social media use separate from preoccupation in the forms of spending time engaged in self-presentational activities and comparing oneself to others online. As would be expected, appearance-related online activity had moderate correlations with self-presentation and appearance comparison, whereas self-presentation and appearance comparison were more highly correlated with each other. Nevertheless, analyses supported three rather than two factors. Moreover, providing evidence of the validity of all three SMAPS subscales, appearance-related activity was associated with appearance-related problems including disordered eating, but had only small associations with general emotional adjustment (social anxiety and depression). The other two SMAPS subscales tapping preoccupation seemed to generate more risk across a range of problems including appearance-related anxiety, disordered eating, engaging in and witnessing social media teasing about appearance, as well as having moderate positive associations with both general social anxiety and depressive symptoms. In addition, multivariate models showed that SMAPS appearance-related activity and appearance comparison had incremental validity when the outcomes were social anxiety, depression, appearance anxiety, and disordered eating; they had unique associations with all of these outcomes even after considering a range of other social media and appearance-related behaviors, as well as stress, coping, and sexual harassment.
Our analyses of gender shows that the SMAPS can be successfully used with both young men and women, with invariance found for the three-subscale factor structure in Study 2. In addition, age tended to show nonsignificant associations with the three SMAPS subscales, across the age range of 13 to 18 years in Study 1 and age 17 to 25 years in Study 2. Yet, as is usually found in studies of body image and appearance (Ricciardelli & Yager, 2016; Webb et al., 2017; Webb & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2014), young women (both high school students and university students) did endorse more appearance-related social media activity and more preoccupation with online self-presentation relative to young men. Young women also engaged in more appearance comparison than young men. In fact, young women scored about 0.5 points higher on activity and about 1 point higher (on a 7-point scale) than young men for self-presentation and appearance comparison. However, rarely were the SMAPS subscale scores more strongly associated with emotional adjustment in one gender relative to the other. In fact, we found only two interaction effects, with both supporting the possibility that greater preoccupation with self-presentation is more strongly associated with emotional maladjustment in young men relative to young women. Given that some research has identified that females are more likely than males to receive emotional support online (from their same-sex friends; Joiner et al., 2014, 2016), it may be that preoccupation with self-presentation is more problematic for young men because they do not get the same level of support received by young women online, which could serve to balance or temper the negative effect of preoccupation.
Overall, the findings support the use of the three SMAPS subscales as separate aspects of appearance-related social media use and preoccupation to examine their unique correlations with other behaviors, attitudes, or disorders in youth. Yet, we also showed in the current analyses that they may interact with other existing measures, focusing here on interactions with general level of social media use, to better explain variation in adjustment among youth. In particular, we found that the general level of social media use interacted with all three SMAPS subscales to better explain why some youth reported more elevated appearance anxiety symptoms than others. The consideration of other interaction effects (i.e., between existing measures and the SMAPS subscales) will be a useful addition to future research. For example, future research could examine risk factors for appearance-related preoccupation, to identify whether preexisting vulnerabilities might explain both social media preoccupation and symptom development. Also important, developmental research is needed focusing on social media use and appearance-related preoccupation, ideally beginning in late childhood and continuing all the way throughout adolescence and into young adulthood to capture data within the age periods when body image and eating disorders first onset for both women and men (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004; Menzel et al., 2010; Pearson et al., 2017; Sharpe et al., 2018).
There is one primary limitation of the present research to note here. The participants in the two studies were Australian young people with the majority endorsing White or Asian race/ethnicity. This may limit the generalizability of the findings. It will be important to test the SMAPS with groups of adolescents and young adults from other countries and from a diversity of other background. Despite the possibility of limited generalizability, the findings provide access to a new measure of appearance-related use and preoccupation, the SMAPS. The SMAPS has three subscales relevant to understanding emotional adjustment and social experiences of young men and young women across the ages of 12 to 25 years. Also, scores derived from the SMAPS were found to be both reliable and valid measure. Future research could build on the findings here to focus on development of appearance-related concerns and disordered eating as potentially related to social media use.
References
American Association of University Women. (2001). Hostile hallways: Bullying, teasing, and sexual harassment in school. American Journal of Health Education, 32, 307–309. 10.1080/19325037.2001.10603488
Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (1987). Mood and Feelings Questionnaire. Durham, NC: Duke University.
Australian Media Communication Authority. (2013). Like, post, share: Young Australians’ experience of social media. Retrieved from [https://www.acma.gov.au/~/media/mediacomms/Report/pdf/Like%20post%20share%20Young%20Australians%20experience%20of%20social%20media%20Quantitative%20research%20report ]
Avalos, L. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). Exploring a model of intuitive eating with college women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 486–497. 10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.486
Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning through imitation. In M. R.Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 211–274). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Brown, Z., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). Attractive celebrity and peer images on Instagram: Effect on women’s mood and body image. Body Image, 19, 37–43. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.08.007
Burke, M., Marlow, C., & Lento, T. (2010). Social network activity and social well-being. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 85, 455–459.
Choukas-Bradley, S., Nesi, J., Widman, L., & Higgins, M. K. (2019). Camera-ready: Young women’s appearance-related social media consciousness. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8, 473–481. 10.1037/ppm0000196
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2017). The relationship between Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activities and body image concerns in young women. Body Image, 23, 183–187. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.002
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2018). ‘Selfie’-objectification: The role of selfies in self-objectification and disordered eating in young women. Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 68–74. 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.027
Connor-Smith, J. K., Compas, B. E., Wadsworth, M. E., Thomsen, A. H., & Saltzman, H. (2000). Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 976–992. 10.1037/0022-006X.68.6.976
Dijkstra, J. K., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Borch, C. (2013). Popularity and adolescent friendship networks: Selection and influence dynamics. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1242–1252. 10.1037/a0030098
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends”: social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. 10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & O’Connor, M. (2008). Eating disorder examination. In C. G.Fairburn (Ed.), Cognitive behaviour therapy and eating disorders (pp. 265–308). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fardouly, J., Magson, N. R., Johnco, C. J., Oar, E. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2018). Parental control of the time preadolescents spend on social media: Links with preadolescents’ social media appearance comparisons and mental health. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47, 1456–1468. 10.1007/s10964-018-0870-1
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Negative comparisons about one’s appearance mediate the relationship between Facebook usage and body image concerns. Body Image, 12, 82–88. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.004
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2016). Social media and body image concerns: Current research and current directions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 9, 1–5. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.005
Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J. A., Meuwly, N., & Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook and depressive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2, 161–170. 10.1037/a0033111
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140. 10.1177/001872675400700202
Frederickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
Garner, D. M. (2004). Eating Disorder Inventory-3. Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The eating attitudes test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12, 871–878. 10.1017/S0033291700049163
Gerson, J., Plagnol, A. C., & Corr, P. J. (2017). Passive and Active Facebook Use measure (PAUM): Validation and relationship to reinforcement sensitivity theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 81–90. 10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.034
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Haferkamp, N., Eimler, S. C., Papadakis, A. M., & Kruck, J. V. (2012). Men are from Mars, women are from Venus? Examining gender differences in self-presentation on social networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15, 91–98. 10.1089/cyber.2011.0151
Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., Anderson, R., & Tatham, R. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Hayton, J. C., Allen, D. G., & Scarpello, V. (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 191–205. 10.1177/1094428104263675
Hogue, J. V., & Mills, J. S. (2019). The effects of active social media engagement with peers on body image in young women. Body Image, 28, 1–5. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.11.002
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100–110. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2017). “Strong beats skinny every time”: Disordered eating and compulsive exercise in women who post fitspiration on Instagram. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50, 76–79. 10.1002/eat.22559
Joiner, R., Brosnan, M., Duffield, J., Gavin, J., & Maras, P. (2007). The relationship between Internet identification, Internet anxiety and Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1408–1420. 10.1016/j.chb.2005.03.002
Joiner, R., Cuprinskaite, J., Dapkeviciute, L., Johnson, H., Gavin, J., & Brosnan, M. (2016). Gender differences in response to Facebook status updates from same and opposite gender friends. Computers in Human Behavior, 58, 407–412. 10.1016/j.chb.2016.01.008
Joiner, R., Stewart, C., Beaney, C., Moon, A., Maras, P., Guiller, J., . . .Brosnan, M. (2014). Publically different, privately the same: Gender differences and similarities in response to Facebook status updates. Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 165–169. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.004
Kim, J. W., & Chock, T. M. (2015). Body image 2.0: Associations between social grooming on Facebook and body image concerns. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 331–339. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.009
Kim, M., & Park, W. (2016). Who is at risk on Facebook? The effects of Facebook news feed photographs on female college students’ appearance satisfaction. The Social Science Journal, 53, 427–434. 10.1016/j.soscij.2016.08.007
Kleemans, M., Daalmans, S., Carbaat, I., & Anschütz, D. (2018). Picture perfect: The direct effect of manipulated Instagram photos on body image in adolescent girls. Media Psychology, 21, 93–110. 10.1080/15213269.2016.1257392
La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–94. 10.1023/A:1022684520514
Leary, M. R. (1996). Self-presentation: Impression management and interpersonal behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Lonergan, A. R., Bussey, K., Mond, J., Brown, O., Griffiths, S., Murray, S. B., & Mitchison, D. (2019). Me, my selfie, and I: The relationship between editing and posting selfies and body dissatisfaction in men and women. Body Image, 28, 39–43. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.12.001
Lup, K., Trub, L., & Rosenthal, L. (2015). Instagram #instasad?: Exploring associations among instagram use, depressive symptoms, negative social comparison, and strangers followed. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18, 247–252. 10.1089/cyber.2014.0560
Manning, P. (1992). Erving Goffman and modern sociology. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Polity Press.
Marengo, D., Longobardi, C., Fabris, M. A., & Settanni, M. (2018). Highly-visual social media and internalizing symptoms in adolescence: The mediating role of body image concerns. Computers in Human Behavior, 82, 63–69. 10.1016/j.chb.2018.01.003
McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004). Weight and shape concerns of boys and men. In J. K.Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of eating disorders and obesity (pp. 606–634). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
McLean, S. A., Paxton, S. J., Wertheim, E. H., & Masters, J. (2015). Photoshopping the selfie: Self photo editing and photo investment are associated with body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48, 1132–1140. 10.1002/eat.22449
Meier, E. P., & Gray, J. (2014). Facebook photo activity associated with body image disturbance in adolescent girls. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 199–206. 10.1089/cyber.2013.0305
Melioli, T., Rodgers, R. F., Rodrigues, M., & Chabrol, H. (2015). The role of body image in the relationship between internet use and bulimic symptoms: Three theoretical frameworks. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18, 682–686. 10.1089/cyber.2015.0154
Menzel, J. E., Schaefer, L. M., Burke, N. L., Mayhew, L. L., Brannick, M. T., & Thompson, J. K. (2010). Appearance-related teasing, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating: A meta-analysis. Body Image, 7, 261–270. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.05.004
Moreno, M. A., Kota, R., Schoohs, S., & Whitehill, J. M. (2013). The Facebook influence model: A concept mapping approach. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 504–511. 10.1089/cyber.2013.0025
Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 623–636. 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00181.x
O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 32, 396–402. 10.3758/BF03200807
Olson, K. E., O’Brien, M. A., Rogers, W. A., & Charness, N. (2012). Diffusion of technology: Frequency of use for younger and older adults. Ageing International, 36, 13–145. 10.1007/s12126-010-9077-9
Pearson, C. M., Miller, J., Ackard, D. M., Loth, K. A., Wall, M. M., Haynos, A. F., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2017). Stability and change in patterns of eating disorder symptoms from adolescence to young adulthood. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 50, 748–757. 10.1002/eat.22692
Pew Research Center. (2016). Social media update 2016. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from https://www.pewinternet.org/2016/11/11/social-media-update-2016/
Rajan, B. (2018). A picture is worth a thousand words: A study of Millenial women’s curation of self on Facebook profile pictures. Comunicación y Genéro, 2, 173–191. 10.5209/CGEN.62674
Ricciardelli, L. A., & Yager, Z. (2016). Adolescence and body image: From developmental to preventing dissatisfaction. New York, NY: Routledge.
Roberts, C., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Lavell, C., Gregertsen, E. C., Miyamoto, T., & Farrell, L. J. (2018). The Appearance Anxiety Inventory: Factor structure and associations with appearance-based rejection sensitivity and social anxiety. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 19, 124–130. 10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.10.004
Rodgers, R. F., Faure, K., & Chabrol, H. (2009). Gender differences in parental influences on adolescent body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Sex Roles, 61, 837–849. 10.1007/s11199-009-9690-9
Rodgers, R. F., & Melioli, T. (2016). The relationship between body image concerns, eating disorders and Internet use, Part I: A review of empirical support. Adolescent Research Review, 1, 95–119. 10.1007/s40894-015-0016-6
Rutledge, C. M., Gillmor, K. L., & Gillen, M. M. (2013). Does this profile picture make me look fat? Facebook and body image in college students. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2, 251–258. 10.1037/ppm0000011
Saunders, J. F., & Eaton, A. A. (2018). Snaps, selfies, and shares: How three popular social media platforms contribute to the sociocultural model of disordered eating among young women. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 21, 343–354. 10.1089/cyber.2017.0713
Seabrook, E. M., Kern, M. L., & Rickard, N. S. (2016). Social networking sites, depression, and anxiety: A systematic review. JMIR Mental Health, 3, e50. 10.2196/mental.5842
Sensis. (2017). Sensis social media report. Retrieved from https://www.sensis.com.au/socialmediareport
Sharpe, H., Griffiths, S., Choo, T. H., Eisenberg, M. E., Mitchison, D., Wall, M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2018). The relative importance of dissatisfaction, overvaluation and preoccupation with weight and shape for predicting onset of disordered eating behaviors and depressive symptoms over 15 years. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 51, 1168–1175. 10.1002/eat.22936
Smith, A. R., Hames, J. L., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2013). Status update: Maladaptive Facebook usage predicts increases in body dissatisfaction and bulimic symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 149, 235–240. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.032
Steers, M. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone else’s highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33, 701–731. 10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39. 10.1146/annurev.ps.33.020182.000245
Talbot, C. V., Gavin, J., van Steen, T., & Morey, Y. (2017). A content analysis of thinspiration, fitspiration, and bonespiration imagery on social media. Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 40. 10.1186/s40337-017-0170-2
Thompson, J. K., Cattarin, J., Fowler, B., & Fisher, E. (1995). The Perception of Teasing Scale (POTS): A revision and extension of the physical appearance related teasing scale (PARTS). Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 146–157. 10.1207/s15327752jpa6501_11
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). The scope of body image disturbance: The big picture. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Tiggemann, M., & Miller, J. (2010). The Internet and adolescent girls’ weight satisfaction and drive for thinness. Sex Roles, 63, 79–90. 10.1007/s11199-010-9789-z
Tufekci, Z. (2008). Grooming, Gossip, Facebook and MySpace: What can we learn about these sites from those who won’t assimilate?Information Communication and Society, 11, 544–564. 10.1080/13691180801999050
Uhlmann, L. R., Donovan, C. L., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Bell, H. S., & Ramme, R. A. (2018). The fit beauty ideal: A healthy alternative to thinness or a wolf in sheep’s clothing?Body Image, 25, 23–30. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.01.005
Veale, D., Eshkevari, E., Kanakam, N., Ellison, N., Costa, A., & Werner, T. (2014). The Appearance Anxiety Inventory: Validation of a process measure in the treatment of body dysmorphic disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42, 605–616. 10.1017/S1352465813000556
Webb, H., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2014). The role of friends and peers in adolescent body dissatisfaction: A review and critique of 15 years of research. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 24, 564–590. 10.1111/jora.12084
Webb, H. J., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Farrell, L., Waters, A., Nesdale, D., & Downey, G. (2017). “Pretty pressure” from peers, parents, and the media: A longitudinal study of appearance-based rejection sensitivity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27, 718–735. 10.1111/jora.12310
Webb, H. J., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Mastro, S. (2016). Stress exposure and generation: A conjoint longitudinal model of body dysmorphic symptoms, peer acceptance, popularity, and victimization. Body Image, 18, 14–18. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.04.010
Wheeler, L., & Miyake, K. (1992). Social comparison in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 760–773. 10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.760
Williams, G. C., Lynch, M. F., McGregor, H. A., Ryan, R. M., Sharp, D., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Validation of the “Important Other” Climate Questionnaire: Assessing autonomy support for health-related change. Families, Systems and Health, 24, 179–194. 10.1037/1091-7527.24.2.179
Xia, Y., Green, S. B., Xu, Y., & Thompson, M. S. (2019). Proportion of indicator common variance due to a factor as an effect size statistic in revised parallel analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 79, 85–107. 10.1177/0013164418754611
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Skinner, E. A., Modecki, K., Webb, H. J., Rapee, R. M., Hawes, T., & Gardner, A. (2018). The self-perception of flexible coping with stress: A new measure and relations with emotional adjustment. Cogent Psychology. Advance online publication. 10.1080/23311908.2018.1537908
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Webb, H. J. (2017). Body image and peer relationships: Unique associations of adolescents’ social status and competence with peer- and self-reported appearance victimization. Journal of Adolescence, 61, 131–140. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.10.002
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Webb, H. J., Farrell, L. J., & Waters, A. M. (2018). Girls’ and boys’ trajectories of appearance anxiety from age 10 to 15 years are associated with earlier maturation and appearance-related teasing. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 337–350. 10.1017/S0954579417000657
Bottom of Form
Journals/journal 10
Self-Construal Moderates Age Differences in Social Network
Characteristics
Dannii Y. Yeung and Helene H. Fung
Chinese University of Hong Kong
Frieder R. Lang
Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-Nuremberg
In this study, the authors examined age differences in social network characteristics (SNC) among Hong
Kong Chinese. The sample consisted of 596 Chinese adults, ranging from 18 to 91 years old. Age was
positively associated with close social partners and negatively associated with peripheral social partners.
For individuals who were more likely to define the self as interconnected with others (i.e., interdependent
self-construal), increasing age was associated with a greater number of close social partners. The negative
association between age and the number of peripheral social partners, well-documented in the Western
literature, was found only among Chinese adults with lower interdependence but not among those with
higher interdependence. These findings highlight the importance of examining the underlying mechanism
rather than a particular pattern of SNC across cultures.
Keywords: social network, socioemotional selectivity theory, interdependence, adulthood, culture
Age differences in social network characteristics (SNC) have
been widely reported in the Western literature (e.g., Ajrouch,
Antonucci, & Janevic, 2001; Carstensen, 1992; Fung, Carstensen,
& Lang, 2001; Lang & Carstensen, 1994, 2002). Increasing age
was associated with fewer peripheral social partners (peripheral
partners), whereas the number of emotionally close social partners
(close partners) remained relatively stable across age. Socioemo-
tional selectivity theory (Carstensen, Issacowitz, & Charles, 1999)
explains these age differences in SNC in terms of older people’s
greater desire to seek emotionally meaningful social interactions
when they perceive future time as being increasingly limited (Fung
& Carstensen, 2004; Lang & Carstensen, 2002). In this brief
report, we describe a study in which we examined whether indi-
vidual differences in interdependent self-construal may make peo-
ple hold different perceptions about what is regarded as emotion-
ally meaningful social interactions and thus exhibit different
patterns of age-related SNC.
Age Differences in SNC
Age-related differences in SNC have been widely documented
in both cross-sectional (Ajrouch et al., 2001; Fung et al., 2001;
Lang & Carstensen, 1994; 2002; Lang, Staudinger, & Carstensen,
1998) and longitudinal studies (Carstensen, 1992; Field & Min-
kler, 1988; Lang, 2000; van Tilburg, 1998) conducted among
Americans and Europeans. Compared with younger people, older
people have fewer peripheral partners but maintain a similar num-
ber of close partners in their social networks. Older people’s social
networks thus consist of a greater proportion of close partners than
do younger people’s.
Socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen et al., 1999) explains
these age differences in SNC in motivational terms. It argues that
goals for social interaction change as a function of future time per-
ception. Younger people perceive their future time as being relatively
more expansive and they prefer interacting with social partners of
greater diversity to fulfill their future-oriented goals. Their social
networks thus comprise largely peripheral partners. When individuals
grow older, however, they perceive their future as being increasingly
limited. They shift their attention from long-term future-oriented goals
to short-term emotional goals. As a result, they tend to interact with
social partners who can best provide them with emotionally mean-
ingful experiences. Their social networks thus mainly consist of close
partners—such as family members and close friends— but fewer
peripheral partners (Carstensen, Gross, & Fung, 1997; Lang &
Carstensen, 1994). Longitudinal studies conducted among Germans
(Lang, 2000) and Americans (Carstensen, 1992) further demonstrated
that changes in network size were mainly due to the reduction in the
number of peripheral partners. In particular, when older adults per-
ceived their life was coming to an end, they deliberately discontinued
interactions with peripheral partners while maintaining the interac-
tions with close partners such as family members, relatives, and close
friends.
Self-Construal
To the extent that what people seek under future time limitation is
emotionally meaningful social relationships (Fung & Carstensen,
2004), then individual differences in what is considered to be emo-
tionally meaningful may lead to different patterns of social relation-
ships across adulthood. Self-construal, defined as how one perceives
oneself in relation to other people, may be a good determinant of what
is considered to be emotionally meaningful in social situations. Ac-
Dannii Y. Yeung and Helene H. Fung, Department of Psychology,
Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China; Frieder R. Lang,
Institute of Psychogerontology, Friedrich-Alexander-University Erlangen-
Nuremberg, Germany.
The study was supported by Hong Kong Research Grants Council
Earmarked Research Grant CUHK4256/03H and a Chinese University of
Hong Kong direct grant awarded to Helene Fung.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Helene
H. Fung, Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong,
Room 328 Sino Building, Chung Chi College, Shatin, New Territories,
Hong Kong. E-mail: hhlfung@psy.cuhk.edu.hk
Psychology and Aging Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 23, No. 1, 222–226 0882-7974/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.222
222
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
cording to the self-construal theory (Markus & Kitayama, 1991),
people with high levels of interdependent self-construal define the self
as embedded in ingroups and interconnected with others. Such defi-
nitions of self can affect social judgments (Kemmelmeier & Oyser-
man, 2001) and guides behaviors (Gudykunst & Lee, 2003; Markus &
Kitayama, 1991), including social behaviors (Malikiosi-Loizos &
Anderson, 1999; Takahashi, Ohara, Antonucci, & Akiyama, 2002).
They may also affect age-related SNC.
In particular, those with a higher level of interdependent self-
construal may be more likely to maintain interactions with social
partners of greater diversity even when they grow older. These
individuals may attach great importance to taking care of family
members and relatives (i.e., greater familism; Szalay, Strohl, Fu, &
Lao, 1994; Yang, 1988) and to maintaining reciprocal relation-
ships with all social partners, even peripheral ones (i.e., Renqing or
relationship orientation; Cheung et al., 2001; Yeung, Fung, &
Lang, 2007; Zhang & Yang, 1998). They may thus be more likely
to maintain or even increase the number of close social partners
and be less likely to reduce the number of peripheral social
partners as they grow older.
Present Study
Most of the studies on age-related SNC reviewed above were
conducted in Western cultures such as North America and Germany.
People in these Western cultures have reliably been found to be less
interdependent (Hofstede, 1980; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Oyser-
man, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002; Triandis, 1995) than are East
Asians. Such differences tend to intensify with age (Fung & Ng,
2006). In this study, we examined the moderating role of interdepen-
dent self-construal1 in age-related SNC among Hong Kong Chinese,
an East Asian culture with great diversity in self-construal. Hong
Kong people, under the influence of their Chinese heritage, emphasize
interdependence to a greater degree than do North Americans and
Europeans (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Oyserman et al., 2000).
Yet, much individual difference in self-construal exists within the
culture (Gudykunst & Lee, 2003), owing to Hong Kong’s status as
one of the top international financial centers and its 150-year history
as a British colony (Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, 2000).
This variability in interdependent self-construal allowed us to test
whether individual differences in self-construal would moderate age
differences in SNC. We predicted that Hong Kong Chinese with
lower levels of interdependent self-construal would show a pattern of
age-related SNC similar to that found among Westerners in the prior
literature, that is, increasing age was associated with fewer peripheral
partners but a similar number of close partners. Yet, among those with
higher levels of interdependent self-construal, a different pattern of
age-related SNC would be observed: Increasing age would be asso-
ciated with more close partners and a weaker reduction in the number
of peripheral partners.
Method
Participants
The sample included 596 participants who were between 18 and
91 years old (M � 42.26 years, SD � 19.33), including 219 men
and 377 women. Twelve percent of participants had no formal
education, 15.1% had primary education, 30% attained secondary
education, and over 40% attained tertiary education or above.
Almost half of the participants were married, and the remaining
participants were single (38.6%), divorced (2.4%), or widowed
(12.6%).
Procedure
Participants were recruited by convenience sampling from uni-
versities, community centers, and other public places and stratified
by age group (young, middle age, and old) and gender. Although
such a sampling procedure might have yielded a sample that was
not as representative as those in prior studies that used probability
sampling (e.g., Ajrouch et al., 2001; Lang & Carstensen, 2002), the
procedure is consistent with that used in an American study that
sampled across the entire adulthood (e.g., Fung et al., 2001). When
we compared the demographic characteristics of this convenience
sample with those of the general population (United Nations
Statistics Division, 2007a, 2007b), we found that this sample was
generally representative of the population except that participants
were better educated and more likely to be widowed.
Consent for participation was first obtained, followed by the
Wechsler Digit Symbol Test (Wechsler, 1983). Those who dem-
onstrated cognitive difficulties in completing the test did not
continue the study. The rest completed the following measures.
Measures
SNC. The Social Convey Questionnaire (Kahn & Antonucci,
1980) was used to measure SNC. Participants nominated social
partners to one of three circles that surrounded the word I. The
inner circle indicated very close social partners that “one cannot
imagine life without.” The middle and outer circles indicated
rather close social partners and less close social partners, respec-
tively. The number of partners nominated to the inner circle
indicates the number of close partners, whereas the number nom-
inated to the other two circles indicates the number of peripheral
partners.
Self-construal. Interdependent self-construal was measured by
the subscale of Self Construal Scales (Gudykunst et al., 1996). It
consists of 14 items assessing the level of interdependence. Sample
items include being willing to stay in a group if one’s participation
is needed even if one is not happy about it and consulting others
before making important decisions. Participants rated these items
on a 7-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of
interdependent self-construal. The reliability coefficient for this
scale was .83.
1 We originally measured both interdependence and independence be-
cause the two types of self-construal were believed to be independent
constructs (i.e., orthogonal factors; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Singelis,
1994). But we eventually excluded independence from all the analyses
reported in this article because (a) interdependence and independence were
found to be moderately correlated (r � .40) in this sample; (b) preliminary
analyses revealed that there was no interaction effect between interdepen-
dence and independence on the number of close or peripheral social
partners; and (c) independence did not moderate age differences in the
number of either close or peripheral social partners, and this lack of
moderating effect is consistent with findings from prior studies (Kem-
melmeier & Oyserman, 2001; Poon & Fung, in press) in which the
moderating role of independence in social psychological phenomena was
examined.
223BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
Demographic, health, and cognitive variables. Age, gender,
education level, and marital status were recorded. Perceived health
was measured by the Wahler Physical Symptoms Inventory
(Wahler, 1983). Participants reported whether they had any of 42
physical symptoms on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (none) to 6
(almost every day). A mean score was computed, with a higher
score indicating more perceived health symptoms. The mean num-
ber of perceived health symptoms was 1.94 (SD � 0.53). In
addition, as mentioned above, all participants completed the
Wechsler Digit Symbol Test (Wechsler, 1983), both as a screening
tool and as a measure of cognitive ability. In the test, participants
matched symbols with digits in a 90-s period. The total number of
matches successfully made during the time period was taken as a
rough estimate of nonverbal cognitive ability.
Results
The mean network size was 13.31 (SD � 7.20), the mean
number of close partners was 4.83 (SD � 3.06), and the mean
number of peripheral partners was 8.49 (SD � 5.82). Statistically
controlling for gender, education level, perceived health, and cog-
nitive ability did not affect the results we report below.
Moderated regression analyses were conducted to examine the
moderating effects of interdependent self-construal on age differ-
ences in the number of close social partners and the number of
peripheral social partners. Age and interdependence were entered
(as continuous variables) in Block 1, and the Age � Interdepen-
dence interaction was entered in Block 2. The interaction term was
computed after standardizing each variable.
Results from Block 1 revealed that, similar to prior Western
findings (e.g., Germans, Lang & Carstensen, 1994; Americans,
Fung et al., 2001), age was negatively associated with the number
of peripheral social partners, B � �0.098, SE � 0.012, � �
�.327, p � .001. However, although these Western findings did
not reveal any age differences in the number of close social
partners, our results showed that age was positively associated
with the number of close social partners, B � 0.030, SE � 0.006,
� � .189, p � .001. Results from Block 2 revealed a significant
Age � Interdependence interaction on close partners, B � 0.379,
SE � 0.137, � � .110, �R2 � .012, �F(1, 592) � 7.638, p � .01,
and on peripheral partners, B � 0.926, SE � 0.256, � � .141,
�R2 � .019, �F(1, 592) � 13.110, p � .001 (see Table 1).
To illustrate the significant Age � Interdependence interaction
effects on SNC, interdependence was divided into the high, me-
dium, and low levels by a tercile split.2 Age was positively
associated with the number of close partners among participants
with medium and high levels of interdependence, B � 0.034, p �
.01, and B � 0.051, p � .001, respectively, but not among those
with a low level of interdependence, B � �0.008, ns. Moreover,
age was negatively associated with the number of peripheral
partners among participants with low and medium levels of inter-
dependence, Bs � �0.170 and �0.119, respectively, ps � .001,
but not among those with a high level of interdependence, B �
�0.046, ns (see Figure 1).
Discussion
In this study, we tested whether interdependent self-construal
moderated age differences in SNC among Hong Kong Chinese.
We predicted that those with lower interdependent self-construal
would show a pattern of age-related SNC similar to that found
among Germans (e.g., Lang & Carstensen, 1994; Lang et al., 1998)
and Americans (e.g., Fung et al., 2001) in the prior literature, that
is, increasing age would be associated with fewer peripheral part-
ners but the same number of close partners. But those with higher
interdependent self-construal might attach greater importance to
emotionally close social partners as these social partners are the
ingroup (Szalay et al., 1994; Yang, 1988). They might thus be
more likely to maintain or even increase their number of close
social partners with age. Moreover, these individuals might also
feel more obligated to maintain reciprocal relationships with all
2 The patterns of results were similar regardless of whether interdepen-
dent self-construal (a continuous variable) was split up by a tercile split, by
a median split, or by the 1 standard deviation above the mean, mean, 1
standard deviation below the mean division as recommended by Aiken and
West (1991). We chose to use a tercile split because such a split was less
likely to result in misclassification than was a median split (MacCallum,
Zhang, Preacher, & Rucker, 2002), and it has been successfully used in a
prior study on a similar topic (Lang & Carstensen, 2002).
Table 1
Moderated Regression Analyses of the Number of Close and Peripheral Social Partners
Number of close social partners Number of peripheral social partners
Unstandardized
coefficients
Standardized
coefficients
R2 �R2
Unstandardized
coefficients
Standardized
coefficients
R2 �R2B SE �s B SE �
Block 1 .095 .095*** .122 .122***
Age 0.030 0.006 .189*** �0.098 0.012 �.327***
Interdependence 0.808 0.169 .195*** 1.955 0.317 .248***
Block 2 .106 .012** .141 .019***
Age 0.028 0.006 .178*** �0.103 0.012 �.341***
Interdependence 0.743 0.170 .179*** 1.796 0.317 .228***
Age � Interdependence 0.379 0.137 .110** 0.926 0.256 .141***
Note. ** p � .01. ***p � .001.
224 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
social partners, even peripheral ones (Cheung at al., 2001; Fung &
Ng, 2006; Yeung et al., 2007; Zhang & Yang, 1998) and thus find
it harder to drop peripheral social partners from their social net-
works with age.
Findings confirmed our predictions regarding the moderating role
of interdependent self-construal. The stability of the number of emo-
tionally close social partners across age, typically found in Western
studies (e.g., Fung et al., 2001; Lang & Carstensen, 1994), was
replicated only among Hong Kong Chinese with a low level of
interdependent self-construal. In contrast, those with medium and
high levels of interdependent self-construal exhibited a positive asso-
ciation between age and the number of emotionally close social
partners.
A consistent pattern was also found for the association between
age and the number of peripheral social partners. Although a
negative association between age and the number of peripheral
social partners was observed for the entire sample, the association
was significant only among those with low and medium levels of
interdependent self-construal. The association was much weaker
and, in fact, no longer significant among those with a high inter-
dependent self-construal.
Taken together, these findings suggest that the pattern of age
differences in SNC typically found in prior literature (e.g., Fung et
al., 2001; Lang & Carstensen, 1994; Lang et al., 1998) may not be
universal. Increasing age may be associated with stability in the
number of emotionally close social partners but a reduction in the
number of peripheral social partners only among individuals who
are less likely to see themselves as embedded in social groups. For
those who do, aging is associated with an increase in the number
of emotionally close social partners and a much smaller reduction
in the number of peripheral social partners. The basic premise of
socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen et al., 1999) is that
people seek emotionally meaningful social relationships when they
perceive future time as being increasingly limited (Fung &
Carstensen, 2004). Our findings extend the theory by pointing out
that to the extent that cultures or individuals differ in what they
consider to be emotionally meaningful, the actual patterns of age
differences in social relationships may vary.
A few limitations should be considered when interpreting our
findings. First, the findings are cross-sectional and the sample
included participants from a heterogeneous age range of 18 to 91
years. We could not rule out possible cohort effects on self-
construal or SNC, nor could our findings address research ques-
tions such as potential developmental transformation from periph-
eral relationships into close relationships over time. Second, the
convenience sampling limits the generalizability of our findings. In
future studies, researchers should investigate the phenomena in
longitudinal studies with more representative samples from diverse
backgrounds. Such longitudinal studies would also allow closer
examinations of developmental changes in perceived closeness
over time. Finally, in this study, we only examined age-related
patterns of SNC among Hong Kong Chinese, without a direct
comparison with Western samples. It is possible that people from
different cultures may hold different definitions of close and
peripheral social partners, which might have contributed to the
observed differences in age-related SNC between Hong Kong
Chinese and Western samples. In future studies, researchers should
directly compare both the patterns and the definitions of age-
related social relationships between East Asian and Western cul-
tures to address these issues.
Despite such limitations, our findings contribute to the literature by
showing that the socioemotional selectivity phenomenon may not be
defined by a particular pattern of SNC. Those who place greater
emphasis on interdependence may regard all types of social partners
as emotionally meaningful and thus are more likely to increase the
number of emotionally close social partners and are less likely to
reduce the number of peripheral social partners with age. These
findings highlight the importance of studying the mechanism rather
than a particular pattern of socioemotional aging across cultures.
10080604020
Age
10.00
8.00
6.00
4.00
2.00
0.00
N
o
.
o
f
E
m
o
ti
o
n
a
ll
y
C
lo
s
e
S
o
c
ia
l
P
a
rt
n
e
rs
High Interdependence
Medium Interdependence
Low Interdependence
B = -.008
B = .034*
B = .051***
10080604020
Age
18.00
15.00
12.00
9.00
6.00
3.00
0.00
N
o
.
o
f
P
e
ri
p
h
e
ra
l
S
o
c
ia
l
P
a
rt
n
e
rs
High Interdependence
Medium Interdependence
Low Interdependence
B = -.170***
B = -.119***
B = -.046
Figure 1. The relationship between age and close social partners and between age and peripheral social
partners as a function of interdependence. B � unstandardized regression coefficient. * p � .05. *** p � .001.
225BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Ajrouch, K. J., Antonucci, T. C., & Janevic, M. R. (2001). Social networks
among Blacks and Whites: The interaction between race and age. Jour-
nals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 56B,
S112–S118.
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood:
Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7,
331–338.
Carstensen, L. L., Gross, J. J., & Fung, H. H. (1997). The social context of
emotional experience. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual
review of gerontology and geriatrics: Vol. 17. Focus on emotion and
adult development (pp. 325–352). New York: Springer.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time
seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psycholo-
gist, 54, 165–181.
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Zhang, J. X., Sun, H. F., Gan, Y. I., Song, W. Z.,
& Xie, D. (2001). Indigenous Chinese personality constructs: Is the
five-factor model complete? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32,
407– 433.
Field, D., & Minkler, M. (1988). Continuity and change in social support
between young-old, old-old, and very-old adults. Journal of Gerontol-
ogy, 43, P100 –P106.
Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Motivational changes in response
to blocked goals and foreshortened time: Testing alternatives for socio-
emotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 19, 68 –78.
Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lang, F. R. (2001). Age-related patterns
in social networks among European Americans and African Americans:
Implications for socioemotional selectivity across the life span. Interna-
tional Journal of Aging and Human Development, 52, 185–206.
Fung, H. H., & Ng, S. K. (2006). Age differences in personality among
Canadians and Hong Kong Chinese: Personality maturation may differ
across cultures. Psychology and Aging, 21, 810 – 814.
Gudykunst, W. B., & Lee, C. M. (2003). Assessing the validity of self
construal scales: A response to Levine et al. Human Communication
Research, 29, 253–274.
Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K.,
& Heyman, S. (1996). The influence of cultural individualism–
collectivism, self construals, and individual values on communication
styles across cultures. Human Communication Research, 22, 510 –543.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture consequences: International differences in
work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000).
Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and
cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709 –720.
Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convoys over the life course:
Attachment, roles and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim
(Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 254 –283). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Kemmelmeier, M., & Oyserman, D. (2001). The ups and downs of thinking
about a successful other: Self-construals and the consequences of social
comparisons. European Journal of Psychology, 31, 311–320.
Lang, F. R. (2000). Endings and continuity of social relationships: Maxi-
mizing intrinsic benefits within personal networks when feeling near to
death. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 155–182.
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (1994). Close emotional relationships in
late life: Further support for proactive aging in the social domain.
Psychology and Aging, 9, 315–324.
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time per-
spective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17,
125–139.
Lang, F. R., Staudinger, U. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1998). Perspectives on
socioemotional selectivity in late life: How personality and social con-
text do (and do not) make a difference. Journals of Gerontology:
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53B, P21–P30.
MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On
the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological
Methods, 7, 19 – 40.
Malikiosi-Loizos, M., & Anderson, L. R. (1999). Accessible friendships,
inclusive friendships, reciprocated friendships as related to social and
emotional loneliness in Greece and the USA. European Psychologist, 4,
165–178.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 –253.
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking
individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions
and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.
Poon, C. Y. M., & Fung, H. H. (in press). Physical activity and psycho-
logical well-being among Hong Kong Chinese older adults: Exploring
the moderating role of self-construals. International Journal of Aging
and Human Development.
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdepen-
dent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20,
580 –591.
Szalay, L. B., Strohl, J. B., Fu, L., & Lao, P. S. (1994). American and
Chinese perceptions and belief systems: A People’s Republic of China–
Taiwanese comparison. New York: Plenum Press.
Takahashi, K., Ohara, N., Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (2002). Common-
alities and differences in close relationships among the Americans and
Japanese: A comparison by the individualism/collectivism concept. Inter-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 453– 465.
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
United Nations Statistics Division. (2007a). Education enrolment by level.
Retrieved June 29, 2007, from http://unstats.un.org/unsd/cdb/
cdb_series_xrxx.asp?series_code�25540
United Nations Statistics Division. (2007b). Marital status of population by
sex, age group, urban and rural. Retrieved June 29, 2007, from http://
unstats.un.org/unsd/cdb/cdb_series_xrxx.asp?series_code�14880
van Tilburg, T. (1998). Losing and gaining in old age: Changes in personal
network size and social support in a four-year longitudinal study. Jour-
nals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 53B,
S313–S323.
Wahler, H. J. (1983). Wahler Physical Symptoms Inventory manual. Los
Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Wechsler, D. (1983). The psychometric tradition: Developing the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 6,
82– 85.
Yang, C. F. (1988). Familism and development: An examination of the role
of family in contemporary China mainland, Hong Kong and Taiwan. In
D. Sinha & H. S. R. Kao (Eds.), Social values and development: Asian
perspectives (pp. 93–123). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Yeung, D. Y., Fung, H. H., & Lang, F. R. (2007). Gender differences in
social network characteristics and psychological well-being among
Hong Kong Chinese: The role of future time perspective and adherence
to renqing. Aging & Mental Health, 11, 45–56.
Zhang, Z., & Yang, C.-F. (1998). Beyond distributive justice: The reason-
ableness norm in Chinese reward allocation. Asian Journal of Social
Psychology, 1, 253–269.
Received June 14, 2007
Revision received October 24, 2007
Accepted November 1, 2007 �
226 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
Journals/Journal 2
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology
When Every Day Is a High School Reunion: Social Media
Comparisons and Self-Esteem
Claire Midgley, Sabrina Thai, Penelope Lockwood, Chloe Kovacheff, and Elizabeth Page-Gould
Online First Publication, August 13, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336
CITATION
Midgley, C., Thai, S., Lockwood, P., Kovacheff, C., & Page-Gould, E. (2020, August 13). When Every
Day Is a High School Reunion: Social Media Comparisons and Self-Esteem. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336
When Every Day Is a High School Reunion: Social Media Comparisons
and Self-Esteem
Claire Midgley
University of Toronto
Sabrina Thai
Brock University
Penelope Lockwood, Chloe Kovacheff, and Elizabeth Page-Gould
University of Toronto
Although past research has shown that social comparisons made through social media contribute to negative
outcomes, little is known about the nature of these comparisons (domains, direction, and extremity), variables that
determine comparison outcomes (post valence, perceiver’s self-esteem), and how these comparisons differ from
those made in other contexts (e.g., text messages, face-to-face interactions). In 4 studies (N � 798), we provide the
first comprehensive analysis of how individuals make and respond to social comparisons on social media, using
comparisons made in real-time while browsing news feeds (Study 1), experimenter-generated comparisons (Study
2), and comparisons made on social media versus in other contexts (Studies 3 and 4). More frequent and more
extreme upward comparisons resulted in immediate declines in self-evaluations as well as cumulative negative
effects on individuals’ state self-esteem, mood, and life satisfaction after a social media browsing session. Moreover,
downward and lateral comparisons occurred less frequently and did little to mitigate upward comparisons’ negative
effects. Furthermore, low self-esteem individuals were particularly vulnerable to making more frequent and more
extreme upward comparisons on social media, which in turn threatened their already-lower self-evaluations. Finally,
social media comparisons resulted in greater declines in self-evaluations than those made in other contexts.
Together, these studies provide the first insights into the cumulative impact of multiple comparisons, clarify the role
of self-esteem in online comparison processes, and demonstrate how the characteristics and impact of comparisons
on social media differ from those made in other contexts.
Keywords: social comparisons, self-esteem, social media, Facebook, Instagram
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336.supp
In just over a decade, social media use has skyrocketed. In 2005,
only 5% of Americans reported using one or more social media
platforms; by 2019, this number had risen to 72% (Pew Research
Center, 2019). Furthermore, the majority of Facebook, Instagram,
Snapchat, and YouTube users visit these sites at least once per day,
contributing to a global average of over 2 hr per day spent on
social media per person (Clement, 2020). Although social media
can enhance social connection (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe,
2007; Liu, Ainsworth, & Baumeister, 2016) and provide opportu-
nities for self-disclosure and perceived social support (Davis,
2012; Ko & Kuo, 2009), the preponderance of research indicates
that social media use is associated with negative outcomes, such as
envy, romantic jealousy, decreased self-esteem and subjective
well-being, increased loneliness and social isolation, and depres-
sion (Burke, Marlow, & Lento, 2010; Hwang, Cheong, & Feeley,
2009; Kalpidou, Costin, & Morris, 2011; Krasnova, Wenninger,
Widjaja, & Buxmann, 2013; Kross et al., 2013; Muise,
Christofides, & Desmarais, 2009; Tandoc, Ferrucci, & Duffy,
Editor’s Note. David Dunning served as the action editor for this arti-
cle.—KK
X Claire Midgley, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto;
Sabrina Thai, Department of Psychology, Brock University; Penelope
Lockwood, Chloe Kovacheff, and Elizabeth Page-Gould, Department of
Psychology, University of Toronto.
Chloe Kovacheff is now at the Rotman School of Management, Univer-
sity of Toronto.
This research was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research
Council of Canada (SSHRC) Postdoctoral Fellowship to Sabrina Thai and an
SSHRC Insight Grant to Penelope Lockwood. Portions of the present research
were presented at the 18th Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality and
Social Psychology, San Antonio, Texas and the Psychology of Media &
Technology Preconference at the 20th Annual Meeting of the Society for
Personality and Social Psychology, Portland, Oregon. We thank Laksmiina
Balasubramaniam and Ariana Youm for their assistance with data collection.
We also thank Joanne Wood, Brett Q. Ford, Geoff MacDonald, and Emily
Impett for their helpful comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this
article. Data are available by emailing the authors. Study materials, syntax for
analyses, and supplementary materials are available on Open Science Frame-
work: https://osf.io/kn49t/.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Claire
Midgley, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 George
Street, Toronto, ON M5S 3G3, Canada. E-mail: claire.midgley@mail
.utoronto.ca
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology:
Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000
ISSN: 0022-3514 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336
1
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6680-2944
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336.supp
https://osf.io/kn49t/
mailto:claire.midgley@mail.utoronto.ca
mailto:claire.midgley@mail.utoronto.ca
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336
2015; Verduyn et al., 2015; Valkenburg, Peter, & Schouten, 2006;
Vogel, Rose, Roberts, & Eckles, 2014; Woods & Scott, 2016; for
reviews, see Best, Manktelow, & Taylor, 2014 and Verduyn,
Ybarra, Résibois, Jonides, & Kross, 2017). Despite these negative
associations, however, social media use continues to grow (Clem-
ent, 2020; Pew Research Center, 2019); thus, it is important to
understand when and how social media will result in negative
outcomes, and for whom these negative outcomes will be most
significant.
Social Media Is Associated With Threatening
Social Comparisons
A growing body of research suggests that social media exerts a
negative impact on users through social comparison processes:
Individuals see that others on social media appear to be experi-
encing more positive outcomes, and consequently feel worse about
themselves. Indeed, a number of studies point to associations
between Facebook use, upward comparisons, and negative out-
comes. Heavy users, in contrast to infrequent users, are more likely
to agree that others are happier, have better lives, and are doing
better (Chou & Edge, 2012; de Vries & Kühne, 2015). Further-
more, making more upward Facebook comparisons has been as-
sociated with negative self-perceptions of one’s own social com-
petence and attractiveness, increased depressive symptoms, and
lower overall well-being (Appel, Crusius, & Gerlach, 2015; de
Vries & Kühne, 2015; Fardouly & Vartanian, 2015; Feinstein et
al., 2013; Gerson, Plagnol, & Corr, 2016; Liu et al., 2017; Steers,
Wickham, & Acitelli, 2014; Tandoc et al., 2015; Vogel et al.,
2014; Wang, Wang, Gaskin, & Hawk, 2017). These negative
effects, moreover, seem especially pronounced for low self-esteem
individuals (Cramer, Song, & Drent, 2016; Jang, Park, & Song,
2016). This past research, however, does not provide clear evi-
dence that social comparison is responsible for negative social
media outcomes (Appel, Gerlach, & Crusius, 2016). Because these
studies relied primarily on retrospective reports, it may be that
individuals who are experiencing negative outcomes in these do-
mains are simply more likely to recall or report on comparisons
with superior others.
It is unclear, moreover, which characteristics of the social media
context lead to particularly negative social comparison outcomes.
Because past studies showing the connection between social media
and social comparison have focused exclusively on social media
contexts (e.g., Appel et al., 2015), it remains unclear how these
processes may differ from other, non-social-media contexts. Pre-
sumably, the cognitive mechanisms underlying social comparison
will be similar regardless of whether one is exposed to a superior
other on social media, another online context, or in real life, with
upward comparisons typically leading to threats to self-esteem and
diminished mood and life satisfaction (Gerber, Wheeler, & Suls,
2018). One may feel worse if one learns about a friend’s superior
academic performance regardless of whether one hears about the
friend’s success through social media or face-to-face interaction.
Why then, are social media comparisons associated with especially
negative outcomes?
In the present studies, we identify specific attributes of social
media comparisons that are especially damaging to the self.
Through examining comparisons in real-time in lab studies, as well
as in studies using both experimental and experience-sampling
designs, we make three key contributions to the literature: First, we
show that social media provides more opportunities for individuals
to make comparisons, in particular to superior others; individuals
make more frequent upward comparisons when using social media
than in other contexts. Second, because social media posts tend to
be highly positive, individuals make comparisons that are more
extreme in their “upwardness” than in other contexts. This greater
frequency and extremity of upward comparisons results in a par-
ticularly negative impact of social media use on the self. Third, we
advance the literature on social comparison and self-esteem by
showing that low self-esteem individuals are especially likely to
make more frequent and extreme upward comparisons, which in
turn leads to a more negative impact on their self-evaluations; we
further examine whether social media amplifies these negative
outcomes relative to other contexts.
Social Media and Upward Comparison Frequency
Social media provides a continuous stream of information about
other people’s accomplishments. Past research suggests that social
comparisons occur automatically (Chatard, Bocage-Barthélémy,
Selimbegović, & Guimond, 2017; Gilbert, Giesler, & Morris,
1995; Mussweiler, Rüter, & Epstude, 2004); when individuals
encounter information about another person, their own self-
perceptions will be affected. The sheer number of posts in a news
feed, each offering a thumbnail sketch about another person,
would seem likely to yield numerous comparison opportunities.
Furthermore, to the extent that social media posts are positive,
they are most likely to yield upward comparisons, resulting in
negative outcomes for the self (Gerber et al., 2018). Indeed,
evidence suggests that news feed content is predominantly about
positive experiences. Although people do not typically post false
information about themselves online (Back et al., 2010), they do
engage in selectively positive self-presentation (Walther, 2007;
Wilson, Gosling, & Graham, 2012) and are more likely to post
positive rather than negative content (e.g., Dorethy, Fiebert, &
Warren, 2014; Qiu, Lin, Leung, & Tov, 2012; Seidman, 2013). As
a result, individuals browsing their news feeds are more likely to
see posts about friends’ exciting social activities than dull days at
the office, affording numerous opportunities for comparisons to
seemingly better-off others.
To date, research has not directly tested whether social media
exerts a negative impact on the self by eliciting more frequent
upward comparisons than in other, non-social-media contexts. A
number of studies suggest that individuals who are especially
prone to making comparisons experience negative outcomes when
using social media (Alfasi, 2019; de Vries, Möller, Wieringa,
Eigenraam, & Hamelink, 2018; Hanna et al., 2017; Stapleton,
Luiz, & Chatwin, 2017; Vogel, Rose, Okdie, Eckles, & Franz,
2015; Wang et al., 2017). These studies, however, did not explic-
itly measure comparison frequency, instead assessing whether an
orientation to make comparisons more generally (Gibbons &
Buunk, 1999) or comparison tendency on a particular platform
(e.g., “Today, when I was on Facebook, I felt less confident about
what I have achieved compared with other people”; Steers et al.,
2014) might be related to social media use outcomes. Such global
reports may not accurately reflect the degree to which individuals
were engaging in actual comparison activities (Gerber et al., 2018)
and may instead reflect individuals’ lay theories regarding social
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
comparisons on social media. That is, individuals’ perceptions of
how often comparisons are occurring may not match how often
they actually make comparisons on social media (Cramer et al.,
2016).
Moreover, although recent research suggests that individuals
with a greater orientation toward making social comparisons ex-
perience worse outcomes after using social media (Lee, 2014;
Vogel et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2017), research conducted before
the advent of social media suggests that these individuals experi-
ence similarly negative outcomes, such as increased depressive
symptoms and social anxiety, in nonsocial-media contexts (Gib-
bons & Buunk, 1999). Consequently, it is unclear whether frequent
social media comparisons are associated with more negative out-
comes, or whether individuals with a propensity to compare them-
selves experience more negative outcomes in any context. One
experience-sampling study that did measure comparison frequency
demonstrated that physical appearance comparisons made on so-
cial media were actually less frequent than comparisons resulting
from in-person encounters (Fardouly, Pinkus, & Vartanian, 2017).
Given this study’s focus on physical appearance, however, it is
unclear how social media comparison frequency predicts compar-
ison outcomes more broadly. In the present research, we examined
whether upward comparisons would be more frequent on social
media than in other contexts, and whether this greater frequency in
turn would be associated with more negative social media use
outcomes.
Social Media and Upward Comparison Extremity
We propose that social media will exert a negative impact not
only by eliciting a greater number of comparisons to superior
others, but also by prompting individuals to make comparisons that
are especially “upward.” According to the selective accessibility
model (Mussweiler, 2003; Mussweiler & Strack, 1999), individu-
als first assess whether they are similar or dissimilar to a superior
other at a holistic level; they then go on to test their specific
hypothesis either that they are similar or dissimilar to the target.
Social media posts are especially likely to highlight large discrep-
ancies between the comparer and the poster, leading to a holistic
assessment of dissimilarity, and a subsequent test for evidence of
dissimilarity. The characteristics of social media will yield partic-
ularly compelling evidence that other people are very superior to
the self. Posters tend to focus on particularly positive events,
showcasing a “newsreel highlight” of their lives (Steers et al.,
2014; Zhao, Grasmuck, & Martin, 2008); for example, individuals
do not merely post albums of vacation photos, but rather carefully
choose, and often digitally enhance, a few select photos that
indicate that their vacation was spectacular (Lo & McKercher,
2015). Moreover, such posts are likely to present a strong contrast
with the immediate experiences of post viewers, who are, by
definition, staring at an electronic device, often while engaged in
mundane activities (Tien & Aynsley, 2019). Thus, we argue that
comparisons on social media are often to superior others who are
not simply better off than the self, but rather who appear to be
much better off than the self. These more extreme comparisons
may in turn be particularly threatening: To the extent that individ-
uals perceive these highly positive outcomes to be unattainable,
they will feel worse about themselves (Lockwood & Kunda,
1997). Consistent with this argument, one study on physical ap-
pearance comparisons found that women made more extreme
upward comparisons with social media targets than to in-person
targets, and subsequently evaluated their attractiveness more neg-
atively (Fardouly et al., 2017). It is unclear, however, whether this
effect extends to domains beyond physical appearance. In sum-
mary, the present studies are the first to assess whether individuals
make more extreme upward comparisons when using social media
than in other contexts, with a negative impact on self-evaluations,
mood, and life satisfaction.
Self-Esteem and Social Media Comparisons
The frequency and extremity of upward comparisons may also
explain why social media exerts an especially negative impact on
individuals with low self-esteem. To the extent that low self-
esteem individuals chronically view themselves as of lower worth
than other individuals, it seems likely that they will be especially
prone to view successful others as upward comparison targets.
Consistent with this possibility, a number of studies indicate that
lower self-esteem is indeed associated with more frequent upward
comparisons (Locke & Nekich, 2000; Vohs & Heatherton, 2004;
Wayment & Taylor, 1995; for a review, see Wood & Lockwood,
1999). Other studies suggest, however, that low self-esteem indi-
viduals suffer not because they find many instances in which they
are inferior to others but because they find few instances in which
they are superior to others (Locke, 2005; Wheeler & Miyake,
1992). Thus, the literature on self-esteem and upward comparison
frequency in offline contexts is mixed. Research focused specifi-
cally on social media contexts has found that individuals lower in
self-esteem report a greater tendency to make Facebook compar-
isons in general (Cramer et al., 2016; Jang et al., 2016); however,
these studies did not directly examine the role of self-esteem in
predicting the actual frequency of upward relative to downward
social media comparisons, nor did they compare frequency of
social comparisons across different contexts.
For low self-esteem individuals, social media offers especially
fertile ground for making upward comparisons. Given their nega-
tive self-perceptions, they may find it difficult to construe them-
selves as being “in the same league” as someone experiencing
success (Collins, 1996); as a result, social media posts about even
modestly positive achievements may be perceived as upward com-
parisons. Thus, although low self-esteem individuals will make
more upward comparisons than will higher self-esteem individuals
in general, this difference in frequency may be amplified in social
media contexts, where opportunities for comparisons abound, re-
sulting in especially negative consequences for the self.
In addition, to the extent that low self-esteem individuals chron-
ically view themselves as having lower self-worth than others,
social media posts may yield especially extreme comparisons. Low
self-esteem individuals may be habitually likely to make a holistic
evaluation that they are dissimilar from successful others. Consis-
tent with the selective accessibility model (Mussweiler, 2003), this
initial judgment will trigger a search for evidence that they are
inferior to successful others. The exaggerated positivity of social
media posts (Walther, 2007; Wilson et al., 2012), in combination
with the already impoverished self-evaluations of low self-esteem
individuals, should lead to perceptions of an especially large
discrepancy between the self and the poster—and a more extreme
upward comparison. Consistent with this possibility, one study
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
3SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
found that participants with more contingent self-esteem were
more likely to make more extreme upward comparisons and sub-
sequently experience worse mental health outcomes; however, this
research examined only physical appearance comparisons in non-
social-media contexts, and focused on contingent rather than low
self-esteem (Patrick, Neighbors, & Knee, 2004). We argue that
although individuals in general will make more extreme social
comparisons when using social media relative to other contexts,
this will be especially true of low self-esteem individuals; conse-
quently, they will experience the greatest reduction in their self-
evaluations, mood, and life satisfaction after social media use.
The Present Research
In summary, the present research provides the first evidence
identifying the characteristics of social comparisons on social
media— heightened frequency and extremity—that differ from
social comparisons in other contexts. We also provide the first
evidence that these particular characteristics in turn contribute to
social media’s harmful effects on the self, particularly for individ-
uals lower in self-esteem. In Study 1, we examined low and high
self-esteem individuals’ actual comparison behavior in real time
by assessing their reactions to posts in their own social media news
feeds. This enabled us to assess the direction, extremity, and
impact of each comparison made, as well as the cumulative effects
of these comparisons at the end of the browsing session. In Study
2, we experimentally manipulated post content; this enabled us to
assess whether self-esteem would determine the perceived extrem-
ity and impact of comparisons, while holding post content con-
stant. In Study 3, we manipulated context by asking participants to
use social media or engage in other online activities on their
smartphone, and then assessed social comparison activity and
outcomes. In Study 4, we used an experience sampling methodol-
ogy to examine, in a naturalistic setting, whether the frequency,
direction, and impact of comparisons on social media would differ
from those in other contexts.
Across studies, we predicted that participants would make more
frequent upward than downward social media comparisons, which
in turn would negatively impact state self-esteem, mood, and life
satisfaction (Studies 1– 4). We also predicted that these upward
comparisons would be to more extremely superior others, and that,
for each comparison, extremity would be associated with a more
negative impact on self-evaluations (Studies 1– 4). Further, we
predicted that the frequency and extremity of upward comparisons
would be greater in social media than in other contexts, and so
would have more negative outcomes (Studies 3 and 4). In addition,
we predicted that low self-esteem individuals would make more
frequent and extreme upward social comparisons than high self-
esteem individuals, and consequently would experience the great-
est comparison threat (Studies 1–3); we predicted that these self-
esteem effects would be especially potent among individuals using
social media relative to other contexts (Study 3).
Study 1
In Study 1, we assessed social comparisons among participants
browsing their social media news feeds on either Facebook or Insta-
gram. After viewing each post, participants indicated whether they
had made a social comparison, and if so, the comparison domain,
direction, and impact on their self-evaluations. In addition, to assess
the cumulative effects of the posts viewed, we measured participants’
mood, state self-esteem, and life satisfaction after the browsing ses-
sion. Whereas past studies have used global retrospective self-reports
regarding social comparison activity on social media, participants in
the present study reported on the social comparisons they made while
browsing their news feed in real time in the lab; this enabled us to
more accurately measure the frequency, direction, and extremity of
social media comparisons, while reducing potential bias in recall. We
predicted that the more extreme the upward comparison, the more
negative the immediate impact on self-evaluations. In addition, we
predicted that individuals would make more upward comparisons
than downward or lateral comparisons, and these upward comparisons
would have a cumulative negative impact on their self-evaluations,
mood, and life satisfaction.
Study 1 also allowed us to examine the role of self-esteem in
determining the frequency, extremity, and outcomes of comparisons
occurring during an actual social media session. We predicted that
lower self-esteem participants would make more extreme upward
comparisons than high self-esteem individuals and, thus, report feel-
ing worse about themselves after each comparison. In addition, we
predicted that low self-esteem individuals would make more frequent
upward comparisons, which in turn would result in more negative
mood, self-evaluations, and life satisfaction at the end of the session.
Participants browsed either their Facebook or Instagram news feed
on their smartphones and answered questions about the first 20 posts
on a desktop computer. We examined comparisons on two popular
social media platforms (Greenwood, Perrin, & Duggan, 2016) to
confirm that our results would generalize to more than one site.
Method
Participants. We recruited 251 introductory psychology stu-
dents for a study on social media use. Thirty-eight were excluded
from our analyses: Nine participants experienced technical problems
with the survey, 18 participants did not complete the survey within the
allotted time, four participants indicated at the end of the session that
they did not understand the instructions, four participants behaved in
a manner indicating that they did not take the study seriously (e.g.,
stated that they gave random answers to finish the study faster), and
three were unable to use their smartphones to browse their social
media feeds. Our analyses included 213 introductory psychology
students (157 women and 56 men; Mage � 18.98 years, SD � 1.64
years) who participated for course credit. We collected sufficient data
(i.e., at least 85 observations; Cohen, 1992) to detect a small effect at
both levels of our multilevel models (NL1 � 1796; NL2 � 206). Post
hoc power analyses revealed that we had at least 81.06% power for
our primary multilevel results. For our other analyses, sensitivity
analysis revealed that we had sufficient power to detect a small-to-
medium effect (r � .19).1
Procedure. Participants who use both Facebook and Insta-
gram were invited to take part in a study on social media use. Upon
arrival at the lab, participants first completed the 10-item Rosen-
berg Self-Esteem Scale (e.g., “I take a positive attitude toward
myself”; � � .87; Rosenberg, 1965). Participants rated themselves
on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 1 (strongly dis-
1 Across all studies, we had relatively small samples of males and, thus,
limited statistical power to detect significant gender effects.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
4 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
agree) to 7 (strongly agree). The pretest also included questions
about frequency of Facebook and Instagram use; all participants
indicated using both Facebook and Instagram at least once per
week. Finally, to ensure that participants understood what a social
comparison was and could complete the task successfully, they
each completed a brief training session in which they were pro-
vided with a detailed description of what does (and does not)
constitute a social comparison, and practiced identifying social
comparisons in sample scenarios.2
After the training session, participants were randomly assigned
to either the Facebook or Instagram condition and were asked to
open the corresponding app on their smartphone. To ensure that
participants using both platforms were following a similar proce-
dure, we asked them to answer questions about the first 20 posts in
their news feed. Participants were asked to complete the question-
naire without navigating away from their news feed. For each post,
participants indicated the extent to which they had made a social
comparison while viewing the post (1 � not at all, 7 � com-
pletely). If participants had made a comparison (i.e., answered 2 or
above), they answered additional questions about the comparison.
Comparison questions. For posts that led to comparisons,
participants first indicated in which domain(s) the comparison
occurred (from a list based on options provided in an earlier study;
Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), with the option of selecting one or
multiple domains, or selecting “other” and providing their own
domain label (see Table 1 for a list of domain options). Participants
then reported the comparison direction, by indicating whether the
comparison was to someone worse- or better-off than themselves
on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from �3 (much worse-
off than me) to 3 (much better-off than me). This item enabled us
to assess not only the direction of the comparison, but also the
extremity. For example, a score of either 1 or 3 would indicate an
upward comparison, but the 3 indicates a more extreme upward
comparison (Patrick et al., 2004). They then rated themselves on
two self-evaluation items (“After making this comparison, I felt
better about myself” and “After making this comparison, I felt
worse about myself” [reverse-scored]) on a 7-point scale with
endpoints ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Responses for the two self-evaluation items, r � �.71, p � .001
were averaged to create a composite score of postcomparison
self-evaluations.
Post-social media questionnaire. After answering questions
about 20 posts, participants were instructed to put down their
smartphones. They then completed measures assessing their affect,
self-esteem, and life satisfaction.
State affect. Participants first completed the 20-item Positive
and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Telle-
gen, 1988). Ratings were made on a 5-point scale (1 � not at all,
5 � extremely; � � .82).
State self-esteem. Participants then completed a state self-
esteem measure (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991), indicating how true
a series of 20 statements were for them “right now” on a 5-point
scale (1 � not at all, 5 � extremely; � � .92).
Life satisfaction. Finally, participants completed the five-item
Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin,
1985), indicating their agreement with each item on a 7-point scale
(1 � strongly disagree, 7 � strongly agree; � � .85).3
Results
Overview of analyses. We first analyzed our data at the
comparison-level, examining the domain, direction, extremity, and
impact of individual comparisons. We next examined our data at
the session-level, to assess whether frequency of upward compar-
isons predicted postsession outcomes. Unless otherwise noted, we
analyzed our data using R 3.6.0.4
Individual comparisons.
Domains. Both Facebook and Instagram comparisons oc-
curred in a variety of domains (see Table 1). For both platforms,
the top three domains of comparison were looks/attractiveness,
popularity/friendship, and vacations/activities/lifestyle.
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Participants made
comparisons that were, on average, upward in direction (M � 1.04,
95% confidence interval, CI [0.97, 1.11], SD � 1.54).5 An
intercept-only multilevel model indicated this was significantly
greater than the scale midpoint of 0, b � 1.04, 95% CI [0.93, 1.15],
SE � 0.06, t(193.13) � 17.99, p � .001. Moreover, a multilevel
model with direction separated into its between- and within-person
components and a random slope of comparison direction revealed
that when individuals made comparisons that were more upward in
direction than usual, they reported lower self-evaluations after the
comparison b � �0.64, 95% CI [�0.72, �0.55], SE � 0.04,
t(175.25) � �14.98, p � .001.6 That is, for each participant, their
more upward comparisons (relative to that individual’s average
comparison direction) resulted in more negative outcomes than
their less upward comparisons.
Self-esteem and self-evaluations. We then tested whether so-
cial media comparisons are especially damaging for individuals
lower in self-esteem because they make more extreme compari-
sons. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a 2–1–1 multilevel
mediation model because the predictor (i.e., self-esteem) varied
only at the level of the person, but the mediator (i.e., comparison
extremity) and outcome variable (i.e., self-evaluations) varied
across all posts, which were nested within individuals. Thus, the
person-level average of comparison extremity was included as a
covariate in the final model and direction was person-centered
(Zhang, Zyphur, & Preacher, 2009). This mediational hypothesis
was tested using a variant of the bootstrap procedure (Preacher &
Hayes, 2008) amended for 2–1–1 multilevel mediation using the
indirect function in R (indirect.mlm; Page-Gould & Sharples,
2016) with 5,000 bootstrapped samples to accurately estimate the
indirect effect and its 95% CI. Furthermore, because the relation-
ship between comparison extremity and self-evaluations could
vary from person-to-person, we modeled this path (i.e., b path) as
2 The full version of the training session, including our comparison
scenarios, is available in our study materials on OSF (https://osf.io/acyzu/).
3 Participants in this and subsequent studies answered additional ques-
tions not analyzed for this set of studies. The complete questionnaires and
datasets for these studies are available by emailing the authors.
4 Syntax for all manuscript analyses are available on OSF (https://osf
.io/2k6vd/).
5 All 95% confidence intervals are calculated using the bootstrap per-
centile method.
6 We compared a full model that included platform, trait self-esteem,
person-mean comparison direction, person-centered comparison direction,
and their interactions to a model that included main effects only. Platform
did not moderate this effect, �2(2)�2.15, p�.34.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
5SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
https://osf.io/acyzu/
https://osf.io/2k6vd/
https://osf.io/2k6vd/
a random slope (see Figure 1). Our analysis revealed that trait
self-esteem was associated with lower self-evaluations as a
function of its relationship with comparison extremity,
abwithin � 0.01, 95% CI [0.006, 0.02], abbetween � 0.01, 95% CI
[0.007, 0.02]: Individuals with lower self-esteem made compari-
sons that were more upward, a � �0.03 [�0.04, �0.02], which in
turn made them feel worse about themselves, bwithin � �0.42,
95% CI [�0.45, �0.38], bbetween � �0.48, 95% CI
[�0.54, �0.39]. Although the effect of trait self-esteem on self-
evaluations was significant, c � 0.06, 95% CI [0.05, 0.07], the
direct effect of self-esteem on self-evaluations was reduced when
the indirect path through comparison extremity was taken into
account, c’ � 0.04, 95% CI [0.03, 0.06]. Thus, compared with
individuals with higher self-esteem, those with lower self-esteem
felt worse after social media comparisons, at least in part because
these comparisons were more extreme in their upwardness.
Overall session.
Comparison frequency and postsession outcomes. Of the 20
posts viewed by participants, an average of 8.42 (Mdn � 8.00,
SD � 5.07) resulted in a social comparison. Furthermore, a one-
way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) corrected
using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates (ε � .78) revealed a signifi-
cant effect of comparison type on comparison frequency, F(1.56,
329.53) � 140.60, p � .001. Participants made more upward (M �
5.32, SD � 3.85) than downward (M � 1.20, SD � 1.55; t(211) �
14.80, p � .001) or lateral (M � 1.90, SD � 2.46; t(211) � 11.05,
p � .001) comparisons. They also made more lateral than down-
ward comparisons, t(211) � 3.78, p � .001.7
We then examined whether comparison behavior over all 20
posts influenced subsequent reports of mood, state self-esteem,
and life satisfaction. To account for correlations between the
outcome measures, we conducted a multivariate regression in
which mood, state self-esteem, and life satisfaction were regressed
simultaneously on the number of upward, downward, and lateral
comparisons made by participants. The total number of upward
comparisons predicted outcomes, F(3, 206) � 8.95, p � .001, but
number of lateral, F(3, 206) � 1.81, p � .15, or downward
comparisons did not, F(3, 206) � 0.87, p � .46. Univariate
analyses indicated that making a greater number of upward com-
parisons was associated with less positive affect, b � �0.02, 95%
CI [�0.04, �0.002], SE � 0.01, t(208) � �2.18, p � .030, r �
.15, lower state self-esteem, b � �1.26, 95% CI [�1.84, �0.70],
SE � 0.25, t(208) � �4.96, p � .001, r � .33, and lower life
satisfaction, b � �0.10, 95% CI [�0.14, �0.05], SE � 0.02,
t(208) � �4.22, p � .001, r � .28. There were no effects of
number of lateral or downward comparisons, ts � 1.87, ps � .06.
Thus, regardless of the number of downward and lateral compar-
isons participants made while viewing their news feeds, making
more upward comparisons predicted worse mood, lower state
self-esteem, and diminished life satisfaction after individuals
viewed the 20 posts.
7 Platforms did not differ in terms of number of comparisons reported,
b � �0.02, SE � 0.02, z � �0.96, p � .34. However, relative to
participants using Facebook, participants using Instagram were especially
likely to make upward comparisons relative to any other type of compar-
ison, b � �0.54, SE � 0.18, z � �3.03, p � .002, Odds Ratio � 1.71:1.
Participants who used Instagram had a 71.39% chance of making an
upward comparison, whereas participants who used Facebook had a
59.36% chance of making an upward comparison.
Table 1
Comparison Domains
Comparison domain
Study 1 (N � 1,795) Study 3 (N � 255) Study 4 (N � 1,211)
Instagram
(n � 941)
Facebook
(n � 854)
Social media
(n � 169)
Other contexts
(n � 86)
Social media
(n � 124)
Other contexts
(n � 1,087)
Looks/attractiveness 263 (27.9%) 163 (19.1%) 23 (13.6%) 10 (11.6%) 41 (33.1%) 167 (15.4%)
Academics/career 54 (5.7%) 89 (10.4%) 14 (8.3%) 5 (5.8%) 11 (8.9%) 254 (23.4%)
Dating/relationships 48 (5.1%) 47 (5.5%) 5 (3.0%) 6 (7.0%) 7 (5.6%) 65 (6.0%)
Popularity/friendships 98 (10.4%) 119 (13.9%) 15 (8.9%) 5 (5.8%) 3 (2.4%) 46 (4.2%)
Vacations/activities/lifestyle 219 (23.3%) 162 (19.0%) 26 (15.4%) 9 (10.5%) 16 (12.9%) 83 (7.6%)
Personality/morality 38 (4.0%) 86 (10.1%) 12 (7.1%) 9 (10.5%) 3 (2.4%) 112 (10.3%)
Skills/abilities 75 (8.0%) 63 (7.4%) 14 (8.3%) 13 (15.1%) 12 (9.7%) 126 (11.6%)
Health/physical fitness 65 (6.9%) 62 (7.3%) 20 (11.8%) 13 (15.1%) 16 (12.9%) 80 (7.4%)
Wealth/finances 31 (3.3%) 22 (2.6%) 9 (5.3%) 4 (4.7%) 1 (0.8%) 19 (1.7%)
Family 21 (2.2%) 19 (2.2%) 10 (5.9%) 6 (7.0%) 0 (0%) 17 (1.6%)
Other 29 (3.1%) 22 (2.6%) 13 (7.7%) 4 (4.7%) 1 (0.8%) 16 (1.5%)
Multiple domains — — 8 (4.7%) 2 (2.3%) 13 (10.5%) 102 (9.4%)
Note. N is the number of comparisons reported across all participants. In Study 1, participants could select multiple domains; thus, the sum of all domains
in Study 1 exceeds the total.
Figure 1. The association between self-esteem and self-evaluations is
mediated by differences in comparison extremity (Study 1). Unstandard-
ized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are
reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses
represent the total effect. Values with the subscript within represent the
within-subject effects, and values with the subscript between represent
between-subjects effects. The direct and total effects have not been parsed
into within- and between-person components.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
6 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
Self-esteem and postsession outcomes. Next, we tested
whether individuals lower in trait self-esteem would have lower
state self-esteem, life satisfaction, and mood after browsing their
news feeds as a result of making more upward comparisons during
the session. First, we regressed number of upward comparisons on
trait self-esteem using a Poisson regression with a log link function
to account for the fact that the number of upward comparisons
represented frequency counts and, thus, violated the normality
assumption required for traditional regression. Consistent with our
hypothesis, low self-esteem individuals made more upward com-
parisons, b � �0.04 [�0.06, �0.02], SE � 0.01, z � �6.70, p �
.001.8
We then conducted three mediation analyses, one for each
outcome, using a bootstrapping procedure (Hayes, 2013) with
5,000 resamples and generating 95% CIs for the indirect effects.9
Number of upward comparisons mediated the positive association
between trait self-esteem and state self-esteem, ab � 0.13, 95% CI
[0.03, 0.26], SE � 0.06. Although the total effect of trait self-
esteem on state self-esteem was significant, c � 2.23, 95% CI
[1.90, 2.54], SE � 0.16, the direct effect of trait self-esteem on
state self-esteem was reduced when the indirect path through
number of upward comparisons was taken into account, c’ � 2.10,
95% CI [1.77, 2.44], SE � 0.16 (see Figure 2 panel A). Number of
upward comparisons also mediated the positive association be-
tween trait self-esteem and life satisfaction, ab � 0.01, 95% CI
[0.001, 0.02], SE � 0.01. Although the total effect of trait self-
esteem on life satisfaction, c � 0.16, 95% CI [0.12, 0.19], SE �
0.02, was significant, the direct effect of self-esteem on life satis-
faction was reduced when the indirect path through number of
upward comparisons was taken into account, c’ � 0.14, 95% CI
[0.11, 0.18], SE � 0.02 (see Figure 2 panel B). Finally, we tested
whether number of comparisons would mediate the positive asso-
ciation between trait self-esteem and affect. Although this indirect
effect was in the predicted direction, it was not reliable, ab �
0.001, 95% CI [�0.003, 0.005], SE � 0.002.10 Thus, when using
either Facebook or Instagram, participants with lower trait self-
esteem reported making a greater number of upward comparisons
and, thus, experienced significantly lower state self-esteem and life
satisfaction after the social media session.
Discussion
Study 1 demonstrates a direct link between comparison behav-
iors and their immediate consequences when browsing social
media. Participants made comparisons that were primarily upward,
and many individuals made multiple comparisons in a single
session, with a median of 8 comparisons in 20 posts. Across both
social media platforms, making more upward comparisons while
viewing posts from others was associated with lower state self-
esteem and life satisfaction after the social media session, regard-
less of the number of downward and lateral comparisons individ-
uals had also made.
Additionally, we found that, compared with higher self-esteem
individuals, those with lower self-esteem reported making more
extreme upward comparisons, which predicted lower self-
evaluations after each comparison. Furthermore, individuals with
lower self-esteem reported a greater number of upward compari-
sons, which predicted more negative state self-esteem and life
satisfaction after the session. This provides evidence that low
self-esteem individuals may be susceptible to making more fre-
quent and more extreme upward comparisons, which are both
associated with more negative outcomes for the self.
Our results indicate that comparisons on social media occurred
in a wide range of domains. Whereas past research examining
offline comparisons found that individuals tended to make the
most comparisons about academics and personality followed by
physical appearance and lifestyle (Wheeler & Miyake, 1992), we
found that individuals made more comparisons about attractive-
ness, popularity, and vacations and leisure activities when using
social media. Indeed, only 10% of comparisons made on social
media were in the domains of personality and academics (despite
the fact that all participants were students). Thus, it appears that,
with the rise of social media, the domains in which individuals
make comparisons may have shifted, with a greater focus on
physical appearance, popularity, and recreation activities. Al-
though comparison domain was not a focus of these studies, it is
nevertheless useful to consider that social media may prompt
comparisons that differ in domain as well as frequency and ex-
tremity. We note, however, that we did not directly compare
comparisons on social media with those in other contexts; we
examine this in Studies 3 and 4.
Study 2
Study 1 provides evidence that self-esteem is associated with
both more frequent and more extreme upward comparisons while
using social media, which in turn are associated with more nega-
tive self-evaluations. We note, however, that this study used a
correlational design. It is possible that low self-esteem individuals
are simply viewing different content than are their higher self-
esteem peers. For example, it may be that low self-esteem indi-
viduals have negative self-perceptions because they have many
superior friends, in which case the posts they view from those
friends on social media may be more positive and threatening,
resulting in more extreme upward comparisons. In Study 2, we
8 There was a main effect of platform, b � �0.10, SE � 0.03,
z � �3.21, p � .001: Participants made more upward comparisons on
Instagram than on Facebook; this effect was qualified by a significant trait
self-esteem by platform interaction, b � 0.02, SE � 0.006, z � 3.12, p �
.002. Although lower self-esteem predicted making more upward compar-
isons on both Facebook, b � �0.02, SE � 0.01, z � �2.42, p � .02, and
Instagram, b � �0.06, SE � 0.008, z � �7.21, p � .001, this effect was
much larger on Instagram than on Facebook. Thus, low self-esteem indi-
viduals’ tendency to make upward comparisons is exacerbated when they
browse Instagram relative to when they browse Facebook.
9 In the reported models, we treated number of upward comparisons as
a continuous variable. However, we tested additional models that treated
number of upward comparisons as a count variable (Geldhof et al., 2018).
These results were consistent with those reported in the manuscript and are
reported in greater detail in online supplemental materials (https://osf.io/
9vbw6/).
10 We tested whether there was a self-esteem by social media platform
effect for any of the postsession outcomes measured. There were no
platform effects for affect or life satisfaction, ts � .66, ps � .51; however,
there was a significant trait self-esteem by platform interaction for state
self-esteem, b � �0.42 [�0.74, �0.12], SE � 0.15, t(208) � �2.70, p �
.01, r � .18: This effect was much larger for participants using Instagram,
b � 2.67 [2.31, 3.05], SE � 0.22, t(208) � 11.90, p � .001, r � .64, than
for those using Facebook, b � 1.84 [1.31, 2.34], SE � 0.21, t(208) � 8.63,
p � .001, r � .51. Thus, low self-esteem individuals feel worse about
themselves after browsing Instagram than after browsing Facebook.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
7SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336.supp
https://osf.io/9vbw6/
https://osf.io/9vbw6/
assessed participants’ responses to a set of Facebook posts we
created for the purpose of the study; thus, holding the valence of
post content constant for low and high self-esteem individuals.
This design is similar to that employed by Vogel et al. (2014) in
which participants viewed a post with more or fewer likes, a
manipulation of popularity. In our study, we instead manipulated
the content of the posts to determine the impact of content valence.
Participants were a community sample recruited via Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk (MTurk); all viewed social media posts in which
individuals described events that varied in valence (positive, neg-
ative, and neutral). Participants were told that posts were real
examples taken from the social media pages of participants who
had taken part in a previous study; in fact, the posts were created
by the experimenters for the purposes of the study. After each post,
participants indicated whether they made a comparison and how
they felt about themselves as a result of the comparison.
As in Study 1, we examined whether self-esteem would predict
comparison extremity. Here, however, by exposing all participants
to the same positive, negative, and neutral posts, we can also rule
out the possibility that low self-esteem individuals report making
more upward social media comparisons simply because they have
friends who post a disproportionate amount of positive content
online. We predicted that, after viewing the same posts as people
with higher self-esteem, participants with lower self-esteem would
report making upward comparisons that were more upward in
direction (i.e., more extreme), which in turn would lead to more
negative self-evaluations.
Method
Participants. Through MTurk, we recruited 103 individuals
who were paid $1.00 USD. Participants were eligible for the study
if they used Facebook at least once per month and passed two
standard attention checks (Maniaci & Rogge, 2014). Nine partic-
ipants failed one or both attention checks, and three participants
indicated they used Facebook less than once per month. Our
analyses included 91 participants (63 women, 27 men, and one
person of other/undisclosed gender; Mage � 32.95, SD � 10.19
years). As in Study 1, we were interested in within- and between-
person effects and, thus, collected sufficient data to detect a small
effect (r � .10) at each level (at least 85 observations; Cohen,
1992); post hoc power analyses revealed that for all analyses
(except one which had .70 power), our final sample sizes (NL1 �
364; NL2 � 91) had at least .81 power.
Procedure. Participants were invited to take part in a study
regarding social perceptions on Facebook. First, participants com-
pleted the same self-esteem scale (� � .93) used in Study 1.
Participants were then presented with four posts, ostensibly written
by past participants. Posts were presented one at a time. After each
one, participants answered a series of questions. All participants
saw two neutral posts that described everyday personal experi-
ences (see Table 2); they viewed these as the first and last of the
four posts. Participants were then randomly assigned to see either
a positive then a negative post or a negative then a positive post.
The positive post described either a personal achievement or a
Figure 2. The association between self-esteem and postbrowsing outcomes are mediated by number of upward
comparisons (Study 1). Panel A depicts the mediational model for state self-esteem, and panel B depicts the
mediational model for life satisfaction. Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence
intervals are reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses represent the total effect.
Table 2
Content of Facebook Posts Viewed by Participants (Study 2)
Post ID Post content
Neutral 1 Any suggestions for good lunch spots? In the mood to try something new :P
Neutral 2 OMGGG to the season finale of Walking Dead! Who else was shocked?!?
Negative 1 got some bad news, I’m unexpectedly out of a job . . . but we guess I’ve got to pick myself up and move forward. would really
appreciate anyone that knows someone who is hiring!
Negative 2 Heartbroken—trust me everyone, never fall in love . . . even when you think you know someone, it’s impossible to actually know what
is in their head—it is SUCH a myth that two people can become one . . .
Positive 1 Finally got my dream job at John Hopkins Hospital!! Thank you to everyone who helped me get here . . . this is PROOF that hard work,
tirelessness, and determination does pay off—you can all reach your dreams :)
Positive 2 So thankful to be celebrating 1 year with my babe today . . . you make me smile like no one else can& make my life so much better.
We can’t wait for what is next for us. love you xxxooo
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
8 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
pleasant outcome (i.e., getting a good job or a positive relationship
experience), and the negative post described a personal negative
experience (i.e., a lay-off or a break-up). In summary, all partici-
pants viewed four posts—two neutral, one negative and one pos-
itive. We used two domains for each condition to ensure that
results were not limited to one domain only.
In line with the study’s cover story, participants read each post
and answered a series of questions about their perception of the
posters’ personality. The last question was the comparison direc-
tion measure (“To what extent do you feel this person is worse-off
or better-off than you?”); participants responded on a 7-point scale
with endpoints ranging from �3 (much worse off than me) to 3
(much better off than me) with a midpoint of 0 (neither worse off
nor better off than me). Participants then completed a two-item
self-evaluation measure (r � .69) similar to the one used in Study
1; they indicated the extent to which they felt worse about them-
selves and better about themselves on a 7-point scale ranging
from �3 (strongly disagree) to 3 (strongly agree).
Results
Overview of analyses. We first examined whether partici-
pants were more likely to make an upward comparison after
viewing a positive (vs. neutral or negative) post and, thus, report
lower self-evaluations. We then examined whether lower self-
esteem participants were more likely than their higher self-esteem
peers to make upward comparisons in response to positive posts.
Finally, across all posts, we examined whether, as in Study 1,
participants with lower self-esteem made comparisons that were
more extremely upward and consequently experienced worse post-
comparison outcomes. Unlike Study 1, all analyses in Study 2
were conducted at the level of the individual posts and compari-
sons, as participants viewed too few posts to assess frequency of
comparisons as an outcome measure.
Order effects. There was a significant effect of order on
comparison extremity, b � 0.24, 95% CI [0.09, 0.39], SE � 0.08,
t(346) � 3.17, p � .001: Participants who saw the positive post
before the negative post (i.e., made an upward comparison first)
rated comparisons to be more upward than participants who saw
the negative post before the positive post (i.e., made a downward
comparison first). Thus, although there was no effect of order on
self-evaluations, b � �0.03, 95% CI [�0.17, 0.10], SE � 0.07,
t(97.88) � �0.50, p � .62, we controlled for the effect of order in
all analyses.
Comparison direction and extremity. We then examined
whether participants made different types of comparisons in re-
sponse to the positive, neutral, and negative posts using a one-way
repeated measures ANOVA corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser
estimates (ε � .76) because post type was nested within person.
Comparisons made in response to the positive posts (M � 0.77,
SD � 1.46) were more upward than those made to the neutral posts
(M � 0.16, SD � 0.77), which were, in turn, more upward than
comparisons made to the negative posts (M � �1.46, SD � 1.19;
F[1.51, 135.94] � 108.80, p � .001, ts � 3.81, ps � .001). This
suggests that our manipulation of post content was indeed effec-
tive, with positive content posts leading to upward comparisons,
and negative content posts leading to downward comparisons.
Self-esteem and comparison extremity. We then examined
whether self-esteem influenced comparison extremity for each
post type. Comparison direction was modeled as a function of
self-esteem (grand-mean centered continuous variable), post va-
lence (two dummy-coded variables), and their interaction while
controlling for the effect of order. There was a main effect of post
valence, �2(2) � 162.41, p � .001: Positive posts resulted in more
upward comparisons (M � 0.79, SE � 0.12) than neutral (M �
0.18, SE � 0.09) and negative posts (M � �1.45, SE � 0.12).
There was also a main effect of self-esteem, b � �0.20, SE �
0.08, t(93.20) � �2.63, p � .01: Individuals lower in self-esteem
made more extreme upward comparisons. The post valence by
self-esteem interaction was not significant, �2(2) � 5.27, p � .07.
Although the overall interaction did not reach significance, we
tested the effect of self-esteem for each post type because of our a
priori hypothesis that individuals with lower self-esteem would
report comparisons that were more upward after viewing a positive
post (Howell, 2013). To test this hypothesis, we recoded the post
valence variables so that each post valence was the reference group
(Aiken & West, 1991), resulting in three 2-level multilevel models
with random intercepts estimated using an unstructured covariance
matrix and the Satterthwaite method of estimating degrees of
freedom. Self-esteem effect sizes for each type of post was esti-
mated using semipartial R2 (Edwards, Muller, Wolfinger, Qaqish,
& Schabenberger, 2008).
For positive posts, there was a significant effect of self-
esteem, b � �0.40, 95% CI [�0.63, �0.16], SE � 0.12,
t(315.30) � �3.28, p � .001, semipartial R2 � 0.03 (see Figure
3). In contrast, there was no effect of self-esteem for either neutral,
b � �0.15, 95% CI [�0.33, 0.04], SE � 0.09, t(186.14) � �1.57,
p � .12, semipartial R2 � 0.01, or negative posts, b � �0.06, 95%
CI [�0.29, 0.19], SE � 0.12, t(315.30) � �0.45, p � .65,
semipartial R2 � 0.001. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis,
when exposed to the same positive posts, individuals lower in
self-esteem tended to make more extreme upward comparisons
than individuals higher in self-esteem; however, lower self-esteem
individuals did not differ from higher self-esteem individuals in
comparison extremity after being exposed to neutral or negative
posts. Because the overall interaction was not significant, we note
that these results must be interpreted with caution.
Self-evaluations. To test whether participants felt worse after
viewing the positive posts, relative to other posts, as a result of
Figure 3. The effect of self-esteem on comparison extremity for each
post type (Study 2) while controlling for order. Errors bars represent
standard errors. Greater scores on the y-axis indicate a more extreme
upward comparison. See the online article for the color version of this
figure.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
9SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
making more extreme upward comparisons, we conducted a boot-
strapped 1–1–1 multilevel mediation using a similar analytic strat-
egy as Study 1. Because we were primarily interested in the
occurrence of upward comparisons and, thus, the effect of positive
posts, we entered post valence as two dummy-coded variables, one
that compared positive to neutral posts (0 � positive, 1 � neutral,
0 � negative), and one that compared positive to negative posts
(0 � positive, 0 � neutral, 1 � negative). The predictor (i.e., post
valence) varied within participants only. Thus, we did not enter the
person-level average of post valence as a covariate in the final
model. Furthermore, the relationships between post valence and
direction as well as direction and self-evaluations could vary from
person-to-person, so we modeled these paths (i.e., a and b paths)
as random slopes. Because the function we used allowed us to
specify only a single independent variable, we included the other
dummy code as a covariate and ran the bootstrap analysis twice,
allowing each dummy code to be the independent variable once
and the covariate once (Hayes & Preacher, 2014). We also spec-
ified the same starting value (i.e., seed) to ensure that the same
bootstrap samples were used for both analyses. Finally, we con-
trolled for the order in which the posts were presented.
Our first analysis revealed that the difference between positive
and neutral posts affected self-evaluations as a function of its
relationship with comparison direction, abwithin � 0.23, 95% CI
[0.09, 0.34], abbetween � 0.20, 95% CI [0.10, 0.35]: When partic-
ipants read a positive post, relative to a neutral one, they were
more likely to make an upward comparison, awithin � �0.60, 95%
CI [�0.85, �0.32], which in turn made them feel worse about
themselves, bwithin �0.38, 95% CI [�0.45, �0.19],
bbetween � �0.33, 95% CI [�0.50, �0.23]. Although the differ-
ence between positive and neutral posts on self-evaluations was
significant, c � 0.30 [0.07, 0.53], the direct effect of post valence
was not significant when the indirect path through comparison
direction was taken into account, c’ � 0.01, 95% CI [�0.21, 0.23]
(see Figure 4 panel A). The population covariance for this model
was estimated to be �ab � 0, 95% CI [�0.03, 0.07]. This implies
that the mediational model was consistent across individuals.
Thus, posts appear to exert their impact on self-evaluations
through social comparisons.
Our second analysis revealed that the difference between posi-
tive and negative posts also affected self-evaluations as a function
of its relationship with comparison direction, abwithin � 0.93, 95%
CI [0.70, 1.32], abbetween � 0.73, 95% CI [0.53, 1.20]: When
participants saw a positive post, relative to a negative one, they
were more likely to make an upward comparison, awithin � �2.24,
95% CI [�2.53, �1.95], which in turn made them feel worse
about themselves, bwithin � �0.42, 95% CI [�0.55, �0.32],
bbetween � �0.33, 95% CI [�0.52, �0.24]. Although the differ-
ence between positive and negative posts on self-evaluations was
significant, c � 0.88, 95% CI [0.63, 1.12], the direct effect of post
type was not significant when the indirect path through compari-
son direction was taken into account, c’ � �0.09, 95% CI [�0.54,
0.11] (see Figure 4 panel B). As in the first model, the population
covariance for this model implies that that the mediational model
was consistent across individuals, �ab � 0.002, 95% CI [�0.02,
0.08]. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis, participants felt worse
about themselves after being exposed to positive posts, relative to
negative or neutral posts, because they made comparisons that
were more upward in direction.
Self-esteem and self-evaluations. Finally, we tested whether,
as in Study 1, self-esteem predicted worse self-evaluations after
viewing individual social media posts as a result of making more
extreme upward comparisons. We conducted a bootstrapped 2–1–1
multilevel mediation using the same analytic strategy described in
Study 1. Furthermore, the relationship between comparison ex-
tremity and self-evaluations could vary from person-to-person;
thus, we modeled this path (i.e., b path) as a random slope (see
Figure 5) as we did in Study 1. We also controlled for order in this
model. Our analysis revealed that trait self-esteem was associated
with lower self-evaluations as a function of its relationship with
comparison extremity, abwithin � 0.11, 95% CI [0.03, 0.20],
abbetween � 0.07, 95% CI [0.02, 0.19]: Individuals with lower
self-esteem made comparisons that were more upward, a � �0.29,
95% CI [�0.53, �0.08], which in turn made them feel worse
about themselves, bwithin � �0.37, 95% CI [�0.43, �0.25],
bbetween � �0.24, 95% CI [�0.46, �0.18]. Although the effect of
trait self-esteem on self-evaluations was significant, c � 0.45, 95%
CI [0.29, 0.62], the direct effect of self-esteem on self-evaluations
was reduced when the indirect path through comparison extremity
was taken into account, c’ � 0.39, 95% CI [0.20, 0.49]. Thus,
consistent with our hypothesis, lower self-esteem participants felt
worse about themselves after viewing social media posts at least in
part because they made upward comparisons that were more
Figure 4. The association between post valence and self-evaluations is mediated by differences in comparison
extremity (Study 2). Panel A depicts the mediational model for the difference between positive and neutral posts,
and panel B depicts the mediational model for the difference between positive and negative posts. Unstandard-
ized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported along the paths they model.
Statistics reported within parentheses represent the total effect. The direct and total effects have not been parsed
into within- and between-person components.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
10 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
extreme in upwardness than those of higher self-esteem partici-
pants.
Discussion
In summary, posts that were positive in valence did lead par-
ticipants to make upward comparisons and, thus, feel worse about
themselves. The more extreme the upward comparison, the more
negative the impact on the self. Furthermore, although all partic-
ipants were likely to make an upward comparison in response to
the positive posts, lower self-esteem participants interpreted these
posts as more upward than did higher self-esteem participants.
Indeed, we replicated our first mediation model from Study 1:
Compared with participants with higher self-esteem, those with
lower self-esteem tended to make more extreme upward compar-
isons and, as a result, experienced greater decreases in their self-
evaluations after being exposed to the same content. Thus, low
self-esteem individuals do not merely have better memory for
upward comparisons than their higher self-esteem peers, or view
posts with more positive content. Moreover, this effect was limited
to positive posts only: Low self-esteem individuals perceived
individuals in negative and neutral posts similarly to higher self-
esteem individuals; they were not less likely to see the worse-off
others as downward comparisons and were not more likely to see
neutral posts as upward comparisons. Therefore, these data pro-
vide initial evidence suggesting that it is not any comparison
behavior in general (Steers et al., 2014), but rather positive posts
resulting in more extreme upward comparisons that are a key
contributor to low self-esteem individuals’ more negative out-
comes after social media use. Because the overall interaction
between post valence and self-esteem was not significant, how-
ever, we note that the findings related to extremity and self-esteem
must be interpreted with caution.
In Studies 1 and 2, participants reported on comparisons
throughout the time they spent viewing social media posts. Al-
though this provided information about participants’ responses to
each post they viewed, we note that this procedure may also have
created demand characteristics, in that participants may have been
especially likely to notice and report on social comparisons. It may
be that participants make fewer actual social comparisons when
they are not prompted to think about them in this way. Accord-
ingly, in Study 3, instead of asking participants to report on each
comparison as it occurred, we instead asked them to report on
comparisons at the end of the session, without alerting them in
advance to the focus on comparison behavior.
Study 3
We argue that social media may be especially likely to elicit
upward comparisons, and that these comparisons will have a
negative impact, particularly among individuals low in self-
esteem, who make more frequent (Study 1) and more extreme
(Studies 1 and 2) upward comparisons. Up to this point, however,
we have not directly compared social media comparisons to those
that occur in other contexts. In Study 3, we experimentally ma-
nipulated context to examine whether social media would indeed
be especially likely to elicit threatening upward comparisons.
Specifically, participants were randomly assigned to use their
smartphone either to access social media or for any other purposes
(e.g., surf the net, text, watch online videos) for 10 min; they then
reported their self-evaluations and information about any compar-
isons they had made. This manipulation allowed us to examine
whether social media comparisons differ from other technology-
based comparisons, and to assess whether people feel worse after
using social media relative to engaging in other technology-based
activities as a result of the type of social comparisons they make.
Finally, we evaluated whether low self-esteem individuals would
be especially likely to make upward comparisons, and conse-
quently be negatively affected, on social media relative to other
online contexts.
We also used this study to examine two variables found in
previous research (Tesser, 1988) to be implicated in comparison
outcomes: closeness of the comparison target and domain rele-
vance. Past research indicates that individuals are more threatened
when outperformed by a close other in a domain that is important
to them (Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988). Many social media
contacts, however, are more distant and may even be past class-
mates or celebrities with whom one has no direct contact. More-
over, individuals often view posts about leisure activities or activ-
ities that may not be directly relevant to the self. This would
suggest that, according to the self-evaluation maintenance (SEM)
model (Tesser, 1988), social media comparisons might be less
threatening than comparisons in other contexts. Accordingly, we
measured both closeness and domain importance. Studies 1 and 2
provided initial evidence that comparisons on social media would
be threatening to the self. Accordingly, we expected that, despite
lower closeness and domain importance, social media comparisons
would nevertheless pose a greater threat to the self than those in
other contexts, due to their greater frequency and extremity.
Method
Participants. In total, 482 participants who indicated in a
prescreen survey that they owned a smartphone and currently used
either Facebook, Instagram, or both completed the study for $1.50
USD. We excluded 67 participants from the analyses: Thirty-five
participants did not follow the instructions for the browsing ses-
sion (i.e., went on Facebook and/or Instagram in the no social
media condition or did not go on Facebook and/or Instagram in the
social media condition), four participants failed our comparison
training session, and 28 participants submitted the survey more
Figure 5. The association between self-esteem and self-evaluations is
mediated by differences in comparison extremity (Study 2). Unstandard-
ized regression coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are
reported along the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses
represent the total effect. The direct and total effects have not been parsed
into within- and between-person components.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
11SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
than once.11 For duplicate responses, we retained the first com-
pleted response and deleted subsequent responses.
Our analyses included 415 MTurk workers (245 women, 168
men, and two persons of other/undisclosed gender; Mage � 37.55
years, SD � 12.79 years). We collected sufficient data (i.e., at least
85 observations; Cohen, 1992) to detect a small effect at both
levels of our multilevel models (NL1 � 255; NL2 � 124). For our
other analyses, sensitivity analysis revealed that we had sufficient
power to detect a small-to-medium effect (r � .14).
Procedure.
Prescreen survey. We invited participants to complete a 5-min
prescreen eligibility survey that included questions about their
technology and social media use. To be eligible for the study,
participants had to indicate they owned a smartphone, which they
would use during the experimental manipulation, and a second
device they could use to complete the actual survey (i.e., laptop or
desktop computer); in addition, they had to indicate that they used
either Facebook, Instagram, or both platforms. As part of this
prescreen survey, participants completed the same self-esteem
measure used in Studies 1 and 2 (� � .93).
Study questionnaire. We asked participants to use their smart-
phones for 10 min, specifying that they either refrain from using
social media (no social media condition) or spend the entire 10 min
using Facebook and/or Instagram (social media condition). At the
end of the browsing session, participants indicated what they did
during the browsing session (see Table 3). To encourage honest
reporting, we told participants that their compensation would not
be affected by whether or not they followed the manipulation
instructions. Next, participants in both groups completed the same
comparison training session used in Study 1 and then reported
whether they had made any comparisons during the 10-min brows-
ing session. If they reported making at least one comparison, they
were asked to enter the first names of each target to whom they
compared themselves. For each target they listed, participants were
asked additional questions about the comparison. As in Studies 1
and 2, we asked in what domain the comparison occurred and the
extent to which the comparison target was doing better or worse
than the self. In Study 3, we also asked participants to indicate how
close they felt to the comparison target on a 7-point scale with
endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (extremely). Participants
also rated how important the domain was to them using a 7-point
scale with endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all important) to 6
(extremely important). Finally, participants reported their state
self-evaluations using a one-item measure (“Right now, how do
you feel about yourself?”) rated on a 7-point scale with endpoints
ranging from �3 (much worse about myself than usual) to 3 (much
better about myself than usual).
Results
Overview of analyses. As in Study 1, we first present our
analyses at the comparison-level, examining whether comparisons
on social media differed from comparisons in other computer-
mediated contexts in terms of domain, direction, and impact. We
also examine whether, as in Studies 1 and 2, comparisons made by
individuals lower in self-esteem were more upward in direction
than those made by individuals with higher self-esteem. We then
present session-level analyses in which we first examine whether
context and self-esteem were associated with frequency of upward
comparisons during the 10-min session. In addition, we examine
whether spending time on social media (vs. not using social media)
predicted greater likelihood of making one or more upward com-
parisons and, thus, lower self-evaluations at the end of the session.
Finally, we assessed whether this session-level effect was stronger
for individuals with lower self-esteem: We assessed whether low
self-esteem individuals using social media were especially likely
to make one or more upward comparisons and, thus, experience
the most negative consequences to the self.
Individual comparisons.
Domains. Consistent with Study 1, we found that social media
comparisons occurred in a variety of domains (see Table 1), and
the most common domains of comparison in this context again
included looks/attractiveness and vacations/activities/lifestyle.
In contrast, the most common comparison domains in other
technology-based contexts were health/physical fitness, personal-
ity/morality, and skills/abilities. A series of logistic multilevel
models, however, indicated that comparisons made on social me-
dia (0) compared with those in the no social media condition (1)
were not significantly more likely to be about looks/attractiveness,
b � �0.42, SE � 0.66, z � �0.63, p � .53, or vacations/activities/
lifestyle, b � �0.43, SE � 0.54, z � �0.79, p � .43, but were less
likely to be about personality/morality, b � 1.12, SE � 0.002, z �
598.98, p � .001.
Domain importance. We next examined whether domain
importance ratings differed by experimental condition using a
multilevel model. We modeled domain importance ratings as a
function of comparison context (effects-coded: �1 � no social
media condition; 1 � social media condition) with a random
11 We considered answering fewer than half of the questions correctly as
failing the comparison training session.
Table 3
Frequencies of Participants’ Online Activities (Study 3)
No social media Social media
Condition Condition
Activity (n � 213) (n � 202)
Made a phone call 20 5
Answered a phone call 12 5
Read a text/message 100 32
Wrote a text/message 67 16
Read an email 96 11
Wrote an email 24 4
Read an article 64 15
Read a book 10 1
Listened to music 26 7
Listened to the radio 7 5
Listened to a podcast 6 3
Watched a video 56 23
Watched TV or a movie 12 6
Used a dating app/website 5 2
Checked Facebook 0 172
Checked Instagram 0 90
Used other social media 36 5
Used Snapchat 7 1
Used Twitter 9 1
Other activity 85 6
Multiple activities 171 109
Note. Frequencies do not total condition sample size because participants
were able to report multiple activities.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
12 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
intercept for each person. Experimental conditions did not
differ in terms of domain importance, b � �0.01, 95% CI
[�0.29, 0.26], SE � 0.14, t(112.79) � �0.10, p � .92. Do-
mains of comparisons made on social media (M � 3.88, SE �
0.16) and in other technology-based contexts (M � 3.91, SE �
0.22) were both rated as moderately important. Thus, we found
no evidence that social media comparisons involve domains
that are more (or less) personally relevant than comparisons in
other technology-based contexts.
Closeness to target. We then examined whether closeness to
the comparison target differed depending on the context in
which a comparison was made; we used the same multilevel
model as we used for domain importance. Experimental condi-
tions did not differ in terms of target closeness, b � �0.04,
95% CI [�0.39, 0.31], SE � 0.18, t(119.37) � �0.24, p � .81.
Targets in social media comparisons (M � 2.10, SE � 0.21) and
in other technology-based contexts (M � 2.18, SE � 0.29) were
rated as relatively low in closeness. In summary, we found no
evidence that comparisons on social media were in domains
higher (or lower) in importance, or to targets with greater (or
lesser) closeness to the self. This suggests that neither closeness
nor importance can account for any differences we observed in
comparison outcomes between social media and other online
contexts.12
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Participants’ self-
evaluations after comparisons were modeled as a function of
comparison context condition with a random intercept for each
person, b � �0.18, 95% CI [�0.41, 0.05], SE � 0.12,
t(130.58) � �1.51, p � .13. Although this effect was not
significant, it was in the expected direction: Compared with
other contexts, comparisons made while using social media
were associated with feeling worse about the self after the
comparison. Next, extremity of comparison direction was mod-
eled as a function of comparison context with a random inter-
cept for each person. This analysis revealed that social media
comparisons were not more upward in direction than those in
other technology-based contexts, b � 0.07, 95% CI [�0.19,
0.33], SE � 0.13, t(127.04) � 0.52, p � .60. Additionally,
unlike in Studies 1 and 2, lower self-esteem did not predict
making more extreme upward comparisons, b � �0.08, 95% CI
[�0.34, 0.18], SE � 0.13, t(132.10) � �0.59, p � .56. We
review possible explanations for these findings below.
Overall session.
Comparison frequency. Next, we conducted a series of
Poisson regressions to examine whether conditions differed in
terms of number of comparisons. Participants assigned to the
social media condition made more comparisons (M � 0.84,
SD � 1.50) than those assigned to the no social media condition
(M � 0.40, SD � 1.20), b � 0.36, 95% CI [0.14, 0.61], SE �
0.07, z � 5.50, p � .001. Furthermore, participants in the social
media condition made a greater number of upward, b � 0.35,
95% CI [0.11, 0.64], SE � 0.08, z � 4.58, p � .001, and lateral
comparisons, b � 0.48, 95% CI [0.08, 0.99], SE � 0.19, z �
2.59, p � .01, than those in the no social media condition. There
was no difference between the two conditions for number of
downward comparisons, b � 0.31, 95% CI [�0.17, 1.04], SE �
0.18, z � 1.69, p � .09. Thus, individuals using social media
made more comparisons than those simply browsing the Inter-
net. Moreover, social media use was associated with an in-
creased number of upward and lateral comparisons, but not
downward comparisons. We note that participants reported less
frequent comparisons relative to Studies 1 and 2. This is not
surprising: Because participants in the social media condition
were free to use the platforms in any way they chose (e.g.,
navigate away from their news feed to read an article, engage in
other activities simultaneously such as listening to music or a
podcast) rather than viewing only news feed posts (as in Studies
1 and 2), and reported all comparisons at the end of the social
media session, rather than after each post (as in Studies 1 and
2), they likely had fewer opportunities to make and/or take note
of their individual social comparisons. Although the overall
frequency of comparison was lower, we nevertheless observed
the predicted difference, with more upward comparisons in the
social media condition. See Table 4 for comparison frequency
overall and within each condition.
Self-esteem and upward comparison frequency. We then
tested whether self-esteem was associated with number of up-
ward comparisons made during the 10-min session. As
predicted, participants with lower self-esteem made a greater
number of upward comparisons than participants with higher
self-esteem, b � �0.38 [�0.52, �0.24], SE � 0.06, z � �6.63,
p � .001.
Postsession self-evaluations. Next, we tested whether con-
text influenced how participants felt about themselves after the
10-min smartphone session. Participants assigned to the social
media condition (M � 0.36, SE � 0.08) reported lower self-
evaluations at the end of the study than those in the no social
media condition (M � 0.58, SE � 0.08), b � �0.11, 95% CI
[�0.22, �0.005], SE � 0.06, t(413) � �2.03, p � .04. We then
tested whether making one or more upward comparisons (vs. no
upward comparisons) mediated this difference between the two
experimental conditions (social media vs. no social media; see
Figure 6).13,14 Given the nonnormal distribution of presence of
upward comparisons, our mediator, we conducted a nonlinear
mediation analysis based on the generalized linear model by
specifying a path model with binomial and Gaussian (normal)
distributions (Geldhof, Anthony, Selig, & Mendez-Luck, 2018).
We used nonlinear mediation analysis (Hayes & Preacher,
2010) to calculate conditional indirect effects (i.e., indirect
effects at specific values of X) and used a bootstrapping pro-
cedure to calculate confidence intervals with 5,000 resamples
12 We did not have sufficient power to test the full SEM model (i.e. a
potential three-way interaction between domain importance, closeness, and
context). However, differences in closeness across the two contexts did not
mediate the difference in comparison outcomes between contexts.
13 Given the limited variance in number of upward comparisons (1.24),
we transformed our count variable to a binary variable to test whether
making at least one upward comparison mediated the effect of self-esteem
on self-evaluations. Our indirect effects were nonsignificant when we
treated number of upward comparisons as a count variable; however, based
on the results of Study 1, we would predict that making more upward
comparisons would predict worse outcomes.
14 Our results for our simple and moderated mediation models remained
consistent when we controlled for number of downward comparisons and
number of lateral comparisons.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
13SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
using Mplus 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 –2012).15 Consistent
with our hypothesis, the indirect effect through presence versus
absence of upward comparisons was significant, ab � �0.16,
95% CI [�0.47, �0.04], SE � 0.12, and the direct effect
became nonsignificant, c’ � �0.14, 95% CI [�0.37, 0.08],
SE � 0.11, once this indirect effect was included. Taken to-
gether, these findings suggest that individuals felt worse after
using social media, relative to those who browse the Internet
without using social media, because they were more likely to
make one or more upward comparisons.
Self-esteem and postsession self-evaluations. Finally, we
tested a moderated mediation model in which the association
between dispositional self-esteem and self-evaluations at the end
of the study was mediated by making one or more upward com-
parisons (see Figure 7). We expected this indirect effect to be
larger in the social media condition than in the no social media
condition. Given the nonnormal distribution of the mediator, we
used a similar analytic strategy as that described above; however,
we amended this approach for a moderated mediation (Hayes,
2015). That is, we calculated conditional indirect effects at specific
values of the predictor, self-esteem (i.e., 1 SD above and below the
mean), and the moderator, experimental condition. These condi-
tional indirect effects are reported in Table 5. Consistent with our
hypothesis, the direct effect, c’ � 0.10, 95% CI [0.008, 0.19],
SE � 0.05, z � 2.19, p � .03, was reduced once we accounted for
the indirect effect through presence or absence of upward com-
parisons. Then, we conducted an omnibus test to determine
whether these conditional indirect effects at various levels of
self-esteem differed between experimental conditions. This test
indicated that this trend was not reliable, �2(2) � 5.84, p � .054.
Nonetheless we take this trend as tentative evidence that our
indirect effect was moderated by context. Taken together, these
findings suggest that individuals lower in dispositional self-esteem
may be more likely to make more upward comparisons than those
higher in self-esteem, particularly when using social media; this
greater number of comparisons on social media, in turn, is asso-
ciated with lower self-evaluations for low self-esteem individuals
after social media use.
Discussion
Study 3 demonstrates that spending 10 min on social media,
relative to other online activities, increases the chances of making
one or more upward social comparisons and, thus, feeling worse
about the self. Furthermore, the increased likelihood of making
upward comparisons mediated the link between social media use
and lowered self-evaluations: Individuals using social media feel
worse about themselves because they are likely to make more
frequent upward comparisons. Moreover, this effect occurs despite
the fact that social media comparisons involve targets no closer to
the self and in domains that are no more personally relevant than
comparisons made in other online contexts. This finding is theo-
retically significant because it suggests that social media compar-
isons may not play by the same rules as other comparisons. Past
studies, including research supporting the self-evaluation mainte-
nance model, indicates that comparisons to less close others are
less likely to result in either a threatening contrast effect or a
positive basking in reflected glory effect (e.g., Tesser, 1988).
Social media comparison targets are no more close than other
online comparison targets, yet they lead to more threatening up-
ward social comparisons. It may be that social media contacts,
while less psychologically close than individuals with whom one
has more recent or in-person contact, may nevertheless represent
important standards against which one measures one’s life suc-
cesses. One may not feel close to a former high school classmate,
but one may nevertheless feel a pang when one sees a post from
the classmate highlighting their superior career accomplishments,
their recent engagement, or their children’s academic successes.
Our findings suggest that further research will be important to
assess whether closeness is the best variable to evaluate the rele-
vance of social media contacts as comparison others.
This study also provides important evidence that low self-
esteem individuals are especially vulnerable to self-esteem threats
15 We describe this and other analyses in greater detail in our online
supplemental materials (https://osf.io/9vbw6/).
Table 4
Frequencies of Participants’ Comparison Activity (Study 3)
Type of
comparison
Comparison frequency
Total no. of
participantsParticipant group 1 2 3 3
Whole sample
(n � 415)
Upward 63 22 11 7 103
Downward 15 3 4 — 22
Lateral 23 4 — 1 28
No social
media condition
(n � 213)
Upward 22 9 2 2 35
Downward 3 — 3 — 6
Lateral 8 1 — — 9
Social media
condition
(n � 202)
Upward 41 13 9 5 68
Downward 12 3 1 — 16
Lateral 15 3 — 1 19
Figure 6. The association between comparison context (experimental
condition: social media vs. other online contexts) and self-evaluations is
mediated by greater likelihood of making one or more upward comparisons
(Study 3). Statistics are reported in body of text given the nonnormal
distribution of the mediator (presence/absence of upward comparisons).
Figure 7. The moderated mediation model we tested. The association
between self-esteem and self-evaluations is mediated by greater likelihood
of making one or more upward comparisons, and this indirect effect differs
depending on the comparison context (Study 3).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
14 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336.supp
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000336.supp
https://osf.io/9vbw6/
as a result of their social comparisons. They were more likely to
make upward comparisons than were high self-esteem individuals,
and this self-esteem effect tended to be more pronounced among
participants using social media. Contrary to our predictions, how-
ever, we did not find that individuals made more extreme upward
comparisons while using social media or that low self-esteem
individuals made especially extreme upward comparisons. One
possible explanation is that participants were reporting on all
comparisons at the end of the session and, thus, were unable to
identify or report subtle differences in comparison extremity. In
addition, we note that many individuals in the no social media
condition were viewing videos or other online material in which
they may have been exposed to celebrities or other very positive
content. Thus, it is possible that the relatively smaller number of
comparisons made in the no-social-media contexts were as ex-
tremely upward as the more numerous upward comparisons on
social media. That is, it may be that upward comparisons are more
extreme in all online media, whether social media, videos, or other
websites.
In Study 3, we asked participants about their social comparisons
at the end of the 10 min that participants spent on their phones,
rather than after each opportunity for comparison, as in Studies 1
and 2. This methodology reduces the likelihood that individuals
were reporting many comparisons because they felt prompted to
do so after each post viewed. Indeed, participants reported fewer
comparisons overall in Study 3 compared with Study 1, and
although this may be partially because of spending less time on
social media in Study 3, it is also likely because of reduced
demand characteristics. Although the total number of comparisons
made was lower, however, we nevertheless observed a greater
frequency of social comparisons on social media than in other
contexts; this difference across contexts cannot be attributed solely
to demand characteristics because both conditions received the
same instructions after using their phone and before reporting any
social comparisons they made.
Study 4
Study 3 provides important evidence that social media compar-
isons differ from those made in other technology-based contexts. It
did not, however, include comparisons that occur in offline con-
texts, such as face-to-face interactions. Thus, in Study 4, we
conducted an experience sampling study to examine participants’
social comparison behavior across all contexts. Participants in-
stalled a custom-made experience sampling app on their smart-
phone for 2 weeks that prompted them six times a day to complete
a short survey about any comparisons they had made since the
previous report. This enabled us to: (a) examine the direction,
frequency, extremity, and outcomes of social media comparisons
as they occur in daily life; (b) compare social comparisons made
on social media with those in all other contexts; and (c) assess
whether the amount of time spent on social media predicts upward
comparison frequency. Whereas the between-subjects design of
Study 3 allowed us to assess whether individual differences in
self-esteem predict different outcomes in different contexts (i.e.,
social media vs. other technology-based activities), Study 4 was
designed to further investigate within-person effects of context on
social comparison outcomes. That is, we were able to assess
whether individuals make more frequent and more extreme up-
ward comparisons when spending time on social media than they
do when spending time in other contexts.
Method
Participants. We recruited 87 undergraduate students from a
university in a large urban center to participate in a 2-week long
study on daily experiences.16 Two participants were unable to
participate because the smartphone app was incompatible with
their devices, and six participants did not complete any experience
sampling questionnaires after attending the intake session. In our
final dataset, we had experience sampling data from 79 partici-
pants (51 women, 26 men, and two person of other/undisclosed
gender; Mage � 20.15 years, SD � 2.40 years). Only 66 partici-
pants (83.54%) returned for the exit session and, thus, completed
all three components of the study. Participants were invited to take
part only if they owned a smartphone and reported that they used
social media. We compensated participants up to $60, depending
on the extent to which they participated in the study ($16 for the
intake session, $2 per day that they completed four or more
surveys, $3 for each completed week, and $10 for the exit session).
On average, participants provided 62.51 surveys (SD � 24.78) for
12 of the 14 survey days (SD � 4.0; median response rate � 100),
for a total of 4,382 completed surveys.
Procedure. Participants first came to the lab for an intake
session, during which they were asked to install the experience
sampling app (ExperienceSampler; Thai & Page-Gould, 2018); a
research assistant then instructed them on how to use the app and
how to recognize and accurately report on social comparisons.
Notifications were customized to participants’ sleep and wake
times and included an opportunity to specify different times for
weekdays and weekends. For the following 2 weeks, the app
randomly prompted participants to complete a short survey six
times per day, in which they indicated the time they had spent on
various activities (e.g., face-to-face interactions, texting, and social
media) and whether they had made a comparison since the previ-
16 At the outset of the study, we aimed to recruit as many participants as
possible, up to a maximum of 100. We ended recruitment at the conclusion
of the semester, which coincided with the end of the academic year. We
decided to end recruitment because we anticipated being unable to
follow-up with a significant portion of participants during the summer
months.
Table 5
Conditional Indirect Effects Through Number of Upward
Comparisons on Self-Evaluations at Specific Values of Self-
Esteem in Each Condition (Study 3)
Self-esteem
values
Experimental
condition
Indirect effect
estimate SE 95% CI
Low (�1 SD) No social media 0.080 0.05 [0.018, 0.195]
Social media 0.113 0.04 [0.039, 0.194]
High (1 SD) No social media 0.034 0.01 [0.01, 0.057]
Social media 0.099 0.04 [0.032, 0.193]
Note. CI � confidence interval.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
15SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
ous survey.17 Whenever participants indicated they had made a
comparison, they then reported to whom they had compared them-
selves (i.e., close friend, ordinary friend, acquaintance, past friend,
stranger, famous person, fictional character, sibling, other family
member, romantic partner, or other) and their closeness to that
person on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all
close) to 6 (extremely close). Participants then indicated the pri-
mary comparison domain from a list (see Table 1) and indicated
the importance of that domain to them on a 7-point scale with
endpoints ranging from 0 (not at all important) to 6 (extremely
important). Next, they indicated in what context that comparison
took place (i.e., in person/face-to-face, video call, voice-only
phone call, other voice chat, e-mail, texting/SMS, social media,
dating app/website, other media/online context, or in a thought/
daydream). If participants indicated the comparison was made
while using social media, they were also asked to indicate the
platform (e.g., Instagram, Facebook).18 Finally, participants indi-
cated the direction and outcomes of the comparison, using the
same questions as in Studies 1–3. At the end of the survey,
regardless of comparison behavior, participants reported their cur-
rent self-evaluations on a 7-point scale with endpoints ranging
from �3 (much worse about myself than usual) to 3 (much better
about myself than usual). After the 2-week experience sampling
period, participants returned to the lab for compensation and
debriefing.
Results
Overview of analyses. As in Studies 1 and 3, we first present
analyses conducted at the individual comparison-level, contrasting
social media comparisons to those made in other contexts (e.g.,
those made face-to-face, in a thought/daydream, or in any other
computer-mediated context other than social media). We then
present analyses for the overall experience sampling period, ex-
amining the total number of comparisons participants made on
social media compared with in other contexts.19
Individual comparisons.
Domains. Consistent with Studies 1 and 3, the two most
common domains of comparisons on social media were looks/
attractiveness and vacations/activities/lifestyle. In this study, un-
like Study 3, looks/attractiveness was also a common domain in
contexts other than social media, second only to academics/careers
(see Table 1). This difference may be because of the samples
involved in each study: Study 3 consisted of primarily middle-aged
adults, whereas Study 4 consisted of primarily undergraduate
students. Compared with middle-aged adults, undergraduate stu-
dents are less likely to have established careers and be in long-term
relationships, both of which may increase their interest in making
comparisons in the academics/careers and looks/attractiveness do-
mains. In the present study, a logistic multilevel model revealed
that looks/attractiveness comparisons were 77.50% more likely to
occur on social media than in other contexts, b � 1.24, SE �
0.001, z � 1311.77, p � .001, odds ratio (OR) � 3.44:1. That is,
although looks/attractiveness comparisons were common across
all contexts, they were especially likely when individuals were
using social media.
Domain importance. Relative to comparisons in other con-
texts, comparisons made while using social media were in domains
that participants rated as less personally important, b � �0.10,
95% CI [�0.19, �0.01], SE � 0.05, t(999.01) � �2.06, p � .039.
Thus, consistent with Study 3, we found that social media com-
parisons did not involve domains that were more personally im-
portant to participants than comparisons made in other contexts.
Closeness to target. Relative to comparisons in other contexts,
comparisons made while using social media involved targets that
participants rated as less close to the self, b � �0.36, 95% CI
[�0.49, �0.22], SE � 0.07, t(1135.62) � �5.23, p � .001. These
lower closeness targets nevertheless elicited a more negative im-
pact on the self, as will be discussed next.20
Comparison direction, extremity, and self-evaluations. Next,
we tested a mediational pathway such that comparisons made
while browsing one’s social media news feed, relative to compar-
isons made in other contexts (0 � other contexts; 1 � social media
news feed), were more extreme (i.e., more upward), resulting in
worse postcomparison self-evaluations. We conducted a boot-
strapped 1–1–1 multilevel mediation using a similar analytic strat-
egy as Studies 1 and 2 (see Figure 8). Because all predictors varied
within person, we separated the predictor (context) and mediator
(comparison extremity) into their between- and within-person
components, but we report the within-person components only
because we are primarily interested in the within-person differ-
ences in context. We also included time as a covariate in our
model. Finally, because the relationship between context and com-
parison extremity (i.e., a path) as well as the relationship between
comparison extremity and self-evaluations (i.e., b path) could vary
from person-to-person, we modeled these paths as random slopes.
Our analysis revealed that comparison context affected self-
evaluations as a function of its relationship with comparison ex-
tremity, ab � �0.26, 95% CI [�0.44, �0.13]: Comparisons made
on social media news feeds, relative to other contexts, did indeed
result in comparisons that were more extreme in their upwardness,
a � 0.74, 95% CI [0.37, 1.25], which in turn made participants feel
worse about themselves, b � �0.35, 95% CI [�0.40, �0.32].
Although the difference between comparison contexts on self-
evaluations was significant, c � �0.44, 95% CI [�0.76, �0.16],
the direct effect of context was not significant when the indirect
path through comparison extremity was taken into account,
c’ � �0.02, 95% CI [�0.31, 0.21]. The population covariance for
this model was estimated to be �ab � 0, 95% CI [�0.015, 0.015].
17 In accordance with recommendations outlined in Wheeler and Reis
(1991), we opted for signal-contingent sampling, as we expected social
comparisons to occur frequently (i.e., numerous times per day) and were
interested in their relative distribution across a variety of domains.
18 Participants were also asked to indicate which aspect of the social
media platform (i.e., public feed, private chat/message, or group chat/
message) they were using at the time of the comparison, to distinguish
comparisons made in private messages from those made while browsing
news feeds. Given that the news feed is a unique feature of social media
that does not exist in other contexts and our focus on the effects of news
feed posts on comparison behavior in Studies 1 and 2, we compared news
feed comparisons to comparisons made in all other contexts.
19 In contrast to Studies 1–3, we did not have the required number of
participants to test for small effects at the person-level (i.e. 85; Cohen,
1992), so we did not examine the role of self-esteem in this study.
20 As in Study 3, we did not have enough power to test the full SEM
model (i.e., a potential three-way interaction between domain importance,
closeness, and context). Furthermore, neither decreased closeness to the
comparison target nor lower domain importance mediated the effect of
context on comparison outcomes, indicating that these variables may not
play as much of a role in social media comparisons as in other contexts.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
16 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
This implies that the mediational model was consistent across
individuals. Thus, participants felt worse about themselves after
making comparisons on social media relative to comparisons in
other contexts, at least in part because these comparisons were
more extreme in their upwardness.21
Overall experience sampling period.
Context and comparison likelihood. We first examined
whether time spent on each activity predicted the likelihood of
making a social comparison for any given survey using a logistic
multilevel model with a random intercept, the within- and
between-person components of time spent on each activity, and
time as a covariate. When individuals spent more time on social
media than they usually did, they were more likely to report
making a social comparison when they were signaled, b � 0.01,
95% CI [0.008, 0.013], SE � 0.002, z � 5.07, p � .001. No other
within-person effects were significant, zs � 1.52, ps � .12. In
addition, individuals who used social media more, relative to other
participants, were more likely to report making a social compari-
son when they were signaled, b � 0.05, 95% CI [0.004, 0.07],
SE � 0.01, z � 3.28, p � .001. No other between-person effects
were significant, zs � 1.28, ps � .20.
Context and comparison frequency. Next, we examined
whether total time spent on each activity predicted total number of
comparisons.22 On average, participants reported making 1.44
comparisons per day (SD � 1.27), of which fewer were made on
social media (M � 0.22, SD � 0.42) than in other contexts (M �
1.26, SD � 1.08; t � �8.80, p � .001). However, a Poisson
regression that included total time spent on various activities
during the study and number of surveys completed revealed that
total minutes spent on social media, b � 0.0004, 95% CI [0.0002,
0.0008], SE � 0.00004, z � 9.87, p � .001, and dating apps, b �
0.01, 95% CI [�0.02, 0.02], SE � 0.002, z � 2.99, p � .003, were
positively associated with number of comparisons, whereas more
time spent face-to-face, b � �0.00004, 95% CI [�0.0002,
0.00004], SE � 0.00001, z � �3.07, p � .002, and on e-mail,
b � �0.003, 95% CI [�0.01, 0.01], SE � 0.0007, z � �4.37, p �
.001, were negatively associated with number of comparisons.
Time spent on all other activities did not predict number of
comparisons, zs � 1.75, ps � .08.
Furthermore, additional Poisson regressions revealed that num-
ber of social media minutes were associated with a greater number
of upward, b � 0.004, 95% CI [0.0001, 0.001], SE � 0.0001, z �
6.27, p � .001, lateral, b � 0.0005, 95% CI [0.00003, 0.001],
SE � 0.0001, z � 4.14, p � .001, and downward comparisons, b �
0.0005, 95% CI [0.0001, 0.001], SE � 0.0001, z � 5.54, p �
.001.23,24,25 Paired t tests indicated that upward comparisons were,
however, more common on social media than both lateral, t(78) �
3.14, p � .002, and downward comparisons, t(78) � 3.03, p �
.003. In other words, spending more time on social media was
associated with making more social comparisons, and these social
comparisons were most likely to be upward.
The total number of comparisons made on social media was less
than one might expect from Studies 1–3. It is possible that partic-
ipants in this experience sampling study were simply forgetting
about the less consequential comparisons made, or reporting rel-
atively few to avoid completing additional questions on the app.
Indeed, we allowed participants to report multiple comparisons
each time they were signaled; thus, survey length increased if
participants reported more comparisons. Past research has shown
that participants are more likely to satisfice (i.e., impose a limit on
how much effort they will apply to the survey) when responding to
more time-consuming diary protocols (Barta, Tennen, & Litt,
2012). Regardless, it is noteworthy that participants made over
10% of their social comparisons on social media. Furthermore,
other than spending time on dating apps (another context in which
people engage in strategic self-presentation; for a review, see
Finkel, Eastwick, Karney, Reis, & Sprecher, 2012), spending time
on social media was the only recorded activity that predicted
making more frequent comparisons. These results are consistent
with our hypothesis that individuals are especially likely to make
social comparisons when using social media.
21 We tested whether comparisons made on Facebook and Instagram, the
two most common social media platforms, differed in extremity. When
individuals reported making a comparison on Instagram, relative to a
comparison on Facebook, they reported a comparison that was more
extreme in its upwardness; however, this within-person effect was not
significant, b � 0.66, SE � 0.39, t(149.07) � 1.67, p � .09. We also tested
whether comparisons differed in terms of likelihood of being upward
depending on the platform. Facebook and Instagram comparisons were
equally likely to be upward, relative to other types of comparisons, b �
0.84, SE � 0.66, z � 1.29, p � .20.
22 The data for total time spent on each activity (except social media,
skewness � 1.61) was highly positively skewed, skewness � 2.90. Thus,
the bootstrapped confidence intervals for these coefficients may include 0.
23 Time spent on dating apps was the only other activity that predicted
making more upward comparisons, b � 0.008 [�0.02, 0.02], SE � 0.002,
z � 3.14, p � .002. More time spent emailing, b � �0.003, 95% CI
[�0.01, 0.007], SE � 0.001, z � �3.63, p � .001, face-to-face,
b � �0.00004, 95% CI [�0.0003, 0.0001], SE � 0.00002, z � �2.29, p �
.02, on video calls, b � �0.0006, 95% CI [�0.004, �0.0003], SE �
0.0002, z � �3.39, p � .001, and on other voice chat, b � �0.0007, 95%
CI [�.005, 0.001], SE � 0.0004, z � �2.05, p � .04, all predicted making
fewer upward comparisons. No other effects were significant, zs � 0.70,
ps � .49.
24 Time spent on dating apps was the only other activity that predicted
making more lateral comparisons, b � 0.01, 95% CI [�0.02, 0.03], SE �
0.005, z � 2.40, p � .02. More time spent face-to-face, b � �0.0001, 95%
CI [�0.0006, 0.00003], SE � 0.0001, z � �2.14, p � .03, predicted
making fewer lateral comparisons. No other effects were significant, zs �
1.84, ps � .066.
25 More time spent emailing, b � �0.004, 95% CI [�0.01, 0.009], SE �
0.001, z � �3.05, p � .002, predicted making fewer downward compar-
isons. No other effects were significant, zs � 1.81, ps � .07.
Figure 8. The association between comparison context (social media
news feeds vs. all other contexts) and self-evaluations is mediated by
differences in comparison extremity (Study 4). Unstandardized regression
coefficients and 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals are reported along
the paths they model. Statistics reported within parentheses represent the
total effect. The direct and total effects have not been parsed into within-
and between-person components.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
17SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
Discussion
In summary, consistent with Study 3, Study 4 demonstrates that
individuals are more likely to make upward than downward or
lateral comparisons when using social media, relative to other
contexts. Further, we found evidence that comparisons on social
media are more threatening and, in turn, result in worse self-
evaluations than those in other contexts, despite being to less close
others and in domains that are rated as less important. Addition-
ally, we demonstrated that, for any given individual, spending
more time on social media was associated with making more social
comparisons. This within-person finding is significant because
although previous studies have demonstrated associations between
time spent on social media and global, retrospective reports of
comparison frequency (e.g., de Vries & Kühne, 2015), it was
previously unclear whether this finding was because of the possi-
bility that people who report making more comparisons also spend
more time on social media. In this study, we show that, regardless
of whether an individual spends more or less time on social media
compared with other people, when that individual spends more
time on social media, relative to her or his other daily activities,
comparisons are more likely. In addition, relative to other contexts,
those comparisons made on social media are more extremely
upward and, thus, result in more negative self-evaluations. In
summary, this study provides the first clear evidence that time
spent on social media results in a greater number of upward
comparisons, comparisons that are more extreme in their upward-
ness, and, consequently, more negative self-evaluations.
General Discussion
Taken together, these studies provide the first comprehensive
analysis of how individuals make and respond to social compari-
sons on social media and the characteristics of these comparisons
that differ from those made in other contexts. Indeed, this research
provides compelling evidence that social media is associated with
frequent and extreme upward comparisons, which in turn have a
negative impact on individuals’ self-evaluations, mood, and life
satisfaction.
Upward Comparison Frequency on Social Media
First, we found that social comparisons on social media are
frequent, and especially likely to be upward. Although past studies
identified this upward direction as a significant feature of compar-
isons on social media, they did not separately assess the relative
frequency of upward and downward comparisons, instead focusing
only on upward comparison frequency (e.g., de Vries & Kühne,
2015; Hanna et al., 2017; Vogel et al., 2014) or general compar-
ison frequency (e.g., Cramer et al., 2016; Feinstein et al., 2013;
Gerson et al., 2016; Jang et al., 2016; Lee, 2014; Steers et al.,
2014). The present studies directly compared the frequency of
upward and downward comparisons made by individuals while
browsing their own social media news feeds (Study 1), immedi-
ately after using social media (Study 3), and in an experience
sampling paradigm during a 2-week period (Study 4), confirming
that upward comparisons consistently outnumber downward com-
parisons on social media. The preponderance of upward compar-
isons is consistent with past studies that have found that people
generally opt for superior or upward comparison targets (Gerber et
al., 2018). However, the present studies are the first to demonstrate
that upward comparisons are more frequent when individuals are
using social media than when they are using other online technol-
ogies (Study 3) or indeed than when they are engaging in most
other daily activities (Study 4).
These studies are also the first to demonstrate that the more
frequent upward comparisons that individuals experience while
using social media are associated with more negative outcomes for
the self, including worse mood, lower self-esteem, and decreased
life-satisfaction. We show that the upward comparisons that indi-
viduals make while using social media not only have an immediate
negative impact on their self-evaluations (Studies 1– 4) but also
cumulative negative effects on their self-esteem, mood, and life-
satisfaction (Studies 1 and 3). In past research, investigators typ-
ically have examined the outcome of a single comparison, or have
compared an upward to a downward comparison on various out-
comes such as self-evaluations (e.g., Morse & Gergen, 1970),
motivation (e.g., Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005; Lockwood
& Pinkus, 2008; Thai, Lockwood, & Boksh, 2020) affect (e.g.,
Buunk, Collins, Taylor, VanYperen, & Dakof, 1990; Gibbons &
Gerrard, 1989; Pinkus, Lockwood, Marshall, & Yoon, 2012; Sa-
lovey & Rodin, 1984), closeness (e.g., Thai, Lockwood, Pinkus, &
Chen, 2016), or domain relevance (e.g., Thai & Lockwood, 2015).
In the present studies, we were able to test the cumulative effects of
a series of comparisons, examining the relative impact of multiple
upward and downward comparisons on state self-esteem, life satis-
faction, and mood. Our studies demonstrate that upward rather than
downward comparisons have the greatest impact on individuals, and
that this impact is overwhelmingly negative. Indeed, any downward
or lateral comparisons did little to mitigate the sting of the more
prevalent upward comparisons. Further, because upward comparisons
are more frequent on social media relative to other contexts, these
negative outcomes are more pronounced when individuals are on
social media relative to other online contexts (Study 3) or engaging in
other daily activities more generally (Study 4).
The present studies also provide the first evidence that, com-
pared with other activities, spending time on social media in-
creases the likelihood of making upward comparisons (Studies 3
and 4). This research provides new evidence to explain the nega-
tive outcomes that have been associated with increased social
media use (Best et al., 2014) and may also at least partially account
for the associations between smartphone use and lower well-being,
especially among young adults (Matar Boumosleh & Jaalouk,
2017; Twenge, 2017). When using social media, individuals are
especially likely to compare themselves to superior, rather than
inferior, others (Studies 1, 3, and 4), and they subsequently feel
worse about themselves and less satisfied with their lives. The
more time they spend on social media, the more upward compar-
isons they make (Study 4), and the worse they subsequently feel
about themselves (Studies 1– 4). In summary, these studies suggest
that social media may be leading to changes in daily social com-
parison behavior; individuals now make more upward compari-
sons that are more threatening to the self.
Upward Comparison Extremity on Social Media
These studies provide evidence that upward comparisons are not
only more frequent on social media, they are also more extreme in
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
18 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
their upwardness. Because these studies allowed us to assess, for
the first time, the impact of upward comparison extremity within
each individual, we were able to evaluate the degree to which the
upwardness of each individual comparison would determine its
impact. We found that, for any given individual, a comparison that
was more extremely upward (than their other comparisons) re-
sulted in a more negative effect on their self-evaluations immedi-
ately after the comparison (Studies 1, 2, and 4). Additionally, by
comparing extremity across context, we were able to show that
upward comparisons are more extreme on social media than in
other daily activities (Study 4), and that this greater extremity
accounts, in part, for the more negative outcomes experienced by
individuals using social media. We note that the extremity results
in Study 3 did not reach significance; it is possible that we did not
have sufficient power to detect this effect. Alternatively, it may be
that comparisons that take place in any online context are often
more extreme in upwardness, given that much online content,
whether on social media, dating platforms, videos, or celebrity
news stories, may include carefully curated and predominantly
positive content. In the future, it will be important to examine
comparisons in additional contexts and evaluate their impact.
Self-Esteem and Comparisons on Social Media
Up to now, we have discussed the contribution of this research
to understanding the negative impact of more frequent and extreme
upward comparisons on social media. This research also provides
valuable insights into the role of self-esteem in determining com-
parison outcomes, in any context. Past research on self-esteem and
comparisons has yielded mixed results: Some research indicates
that lower self-esteem individuals are especially prone to making
upward comparisons (e.g., Locke & Nekich, 2000), whereas other
theory and research suggests that low self-esteem individuals may
be especially likely to avoid upward comparisons (e.g., Wood,
Giordano-Beech, Taylor, Michela, & Gaus, 1994; Wood, Michela,
& Giordano, 2000). Furthermore, other research has focused pri-
marily on this relationship in the reverse causal direction, suggest-
ing that more upward comparisons lead to lower self-esteem (e.g.,
de Vries & Kühne, 2015; Hanna et al., 2017; Leahey, Crowther, &
Mickelson, 2007; Vogel et al., 2014; Wood, 1989).
The present studies focused on the role of self-esteem in
determining comparison frequency and extremity. Specifically,
we found that individuals lower in trait self-esteem made more
frequent and more extreme upward comparisons, and as a
result, reported greater declines in state self-esteem, life satis-
faction, and self-evaluations, than individuals with higher self-
esteem. Moreover, we found evidence that lower self-esteem
individuals actually interpret information in a way that yields a
more extreme upward comparison: Even when information in a
social media post was held constant (Study 2), lower self-
esteem individuals were more likely to see the poster as supe-
rior, and as more extremely superior, than were higher self-
esteem individuals. Finally, the more frequent upward
comparisons made by low self-esteem individuals tended to
lead to negative self-evaluations among individuals in social
media, rather than other online, contexts (Study 3). Because
social media has dramatically increased opportunities for social
comparison, and upward comparison in particular, a browse
through a news feed appears to pose a higher risk for individ-
uals lower in self-esteem. We note that we had insufficient
power to test the moderating impact of self-esteem in Study 4;
accordingly, it will be important to examine how self-esteem
influences reactions to social comparisons in a variety of con-
texts beyond the online contexts evaluated in Study 3.
Domain Importance and Closeness
We found that comparisons on social media were especially
distressing; this was not, however, because of the greater impor-
tance of the comparison domains on social media. Social media
comparisons were in domains similar in importance to those in
other online contexts (Study 3) and were actually in domains less
important than those in offline contexts more generally (Study 4).
This suggests that social media comparisons are exerting a pow-
erful impact despite the fact that they take place in less conse-
quential domains. It may be that the carefully packaged informa-
tion provided in social media posts packs a punch even when it is
focused on relatively trivial domains such as restaurant meals or
entertainment viewed, or even when posts simply showcase ran-
dom (but attractively presented) selfies taken throughout the day.
We note, however, that we assessed domain importance after the
comparisons had been made, and a possible reaction to threatening
upward comparisons is devaluing the comparison domain (Tesser,
1988; Tesser & Paulhus, 1983). Thus, future studies should exam-
ine prior domain importance when examining the impact of social
media comparisons on self-evaluations.
Similarly, the more negative impact of social media compari-
sons was not because of greater closeness of the comparison
others. Indeed, although individuals compare with less close others
on social media than in other contexts, they nevertheless react
more negatively to these comparisons (Study 4). Social media has
opened up a vast array of individuals with whom one can compare,
and this broad pool of comparison targets appears to intensify
rather than dilute the outcome of comparisons. Instead of making
occasional comparisons to people from one’s past, at a reunion,
wedding or other event, one is forced to view the achievements of
less close contacts, whose positive outcomes are carefully pre-
sented for maximum impact, whenever one is browsing social
media. Every day has become like a high school reunion, in which
one is forced to confront the apparently more successful lives of
one’s former friends and acquaintances, and comparisons to these
remote contacts are no less threatening for their psychological
distance.
Future Directions and Conclusions
The present studies focused on social media comparisons in the
context of Facebook (Studies 1– 4) and Instagram (Studies 1, 3,
and 4), currently the most popular social media platforms world-
wide (Pew Research Center, 2019).26 Facebook, in particular, is
used by individuals of all ages for both social contact and business
(Greenwood et al., 2016). In recent years, however, a number of
26 In this factsheet, YouTube is listed as the most popular social media
channel; however, there is debate over whether it is, primarily, a social
media platform (e.g., Abramovich, 2015). Furthermore, because we were
interested in comparisons occurring in social media news feeds, we focused
on the two most popular platforms with this feature.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
19SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
other social media platforms have increased in popularity (Pew
Research Center, 2019), and it will be important to examine the
nature and impact of comparisons made on these other platforms.
Given that all social media platforms afford individuals an oppor-
tunity to carefully manage the self-image they present to the world,
we would expect to find a similar preponderance of upward social
comparisons.
Further, to the extent that different platforms are popular among
different age groups, it will be important to examine how social
media use may lead to different comparison experiences and
consequences among younger and older individuals. Indeed, we
note that two of our four studies used first-year psychology student
participants. It is possible that the type of posts that young adults
see and their reactions to them differ systematically from other
populations, such as older adults or those in a different stage of
life. First-year undergraduates may be especially likely to compare
on social media in general. They are in a period of transition (i.e.,
from high school to university) and, thus, may be more interested
in how well they are adjusting to their new life by comparing
themselves to others (Lockwood, Shaughnessy, Fortune, & Tong,
2012), evaluating what they need to do to succeed (Blakemore &
Mills, 2014; Suls, Martin, & Wheeler, 2002; Wheeler, Martin, &
Suls, 1997). The results of Studies 2 and 3, which involved online
community samples, suggest that our findings are not limited to
one age group. Nevertheless, future studies should examine social
media comparisons across age groups and life stages.
In addition, we note that the present studies focused on vertical
or status comparisons (upward vs. downward) rather than horizon-
tal comparisons (contrastive vs. connective; Locke, 2003). It is
possible that some individuals will use social media to seek out
lateral or similarity-based comparisons that would result in more
communal or positive outcomes (Locke & Nekich, 2000). Alter-
natively, it may be that opportunities for horizontal comparisons
are more plentiful offline than online, as the former context may
provide greater access to information about shared attributes,
across multiple domains. For example, joint participation in an
activity or discussion with a friend could make common interests
or opinions as salient as observations about differences in appear-
ance or particular skills. In future research, it will be important to
consider the full range of comparisons, both vertical and horizon-
tal, that may occur through social media and whether this differs
from other contexts.
Social media platforms offer individuals unprecedented oppor-
tunities to connect with friends, stay in touch with family, share
accomplishments, and feel part of a community. Indeed, past
research suggests that Facebook can have positive outcomes such
as helping maintain relationships (Ellison et al., 2007). The ben-
efits of these platforms, however, also come with potential costs.
The present studies reveal that upward comparisons on social
media are commonplace and have both immediate and cumulative
negative outcomes, especially for individuals with low self-
esteem. With ongoing growth of social media sites, continued
research on how these platforms affect their users— especially
those at vulnerable ages or life stages—is essential.
References
Abramovich, G. (2015, March 20). CMO.com wants to know: Is YouTube
a social media platform or a media play? CMO.com. Retrieved from
https://www.cmo.com/features/articles/2015/3/18/cwtk-youtube-social-
platform-or-media-play.html
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Alfasi, Y. (2019). The grass is always greener on my Friends’ profiles: The
effect of Facebook social comparison on state self-esteem and depres-
sion. Personality and Individual Differences, 147, 111–117. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.032
Appel, H., Crusius, J., & Gerlach, A. L. (2015). Social comparison, envy,
and depression on Facebook: A study looking at the effects of high
comparison standards on depressed individuals. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 34, 277–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015
.34.4.277
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between
Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Current Opin-
ion in Psychology, 9, 44 – 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10
.006
Back, M. D., Stopfer, J. M., Vazire, S., Gaddis, S., Schmukle, S. C., Egloff,
B., & Gosling, S. D. (2010). Facebook profiles reflect actual personality,
not self-idealization. Psychological Science, 21, 372–374. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0956797609360756
Barta, W. D., Tennen, H., & Litt, M. D. (2012). Measurement reactivity in
diary research. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of
research methods for studying daily life (pp. 108 –123). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Best, P., Manktelow, R., & Taylor, B. (2014). Online communication,
social media and adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review.
Children and Youth Services Review, 41, 27–36. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001
Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period
for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–
207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202
Burke, M., Marlow, C., & Lento, T. (2010). Social network activity and
social well-being. Proceedings of the 28th Conference on Human Fac-
tors in Computing Systems. Atlanta, GA.
Buunk, B. P., Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., VanYperen, N. W., & Dakof,
G. A. (1990). The affective consequences of social comparison: Either
direction has its ups and downs. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 59, 1238 –1249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.6
.1238
Chatard, A., Bocage-Barthélémy, Y., Selimbegović, L., & Guimond, S.
(2017). The woman who wasn’t there: Converging evidence that sub-
liminal social comparison affects self-evaluation. Journal of Experimen-
tal Social Psychology, 73, 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.05
.005
Chou, H-T., & Edge, N. (2012). “They are happier and having better lives
than I am”: The impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’
lives. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15, 117–121.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0324
Clement, J. (2020). Daily social media usage worldwide 2012–2019.
Retrieved from Statista website https://www.statista.com/statistics/
433871/daily-social-media-usage-worldwide/#statisticContainer
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social
comparison on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 51– 69.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.51
Cramer, E. M., Song, H., & Drent, A. M. (2016). Social comparison on
Facebook: Motivation, affective consequences, self-esteem, and Face-
book fatigue. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 739 –746. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.049
Davis, K. (2012). Friendship 2.0: Adolescents’ experiences of belonging
and self-disclosure online. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1527–1536.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.013
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
20 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
https://www.cmo.com/features/articles/2015/3/18/cwtk-youtube-social-platform-or-media-play.html
https://www.cmo.com/features/articles/2015/3/18/cwtk-youtube-social-platform-or-media-play.html
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.032
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.032
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360756
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360756
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1238
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1238
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.05.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.05.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0324
https://www.statista.com/statistics/433871/daily-social-media-usage-worldwide/#statisticContainer
https://www.statista.com/statistics/433871/daily-social-media-usage-worldwide/#statisticContainer
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.51
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.049
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.049
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.013
de Vries, D., & Kühne, R. (2015). Facebook and self-perception: Individ-
ual susceptibility to negative social comparison on Facebook. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 86, 217–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2015.05.029
de Vries, D. A., Möller, A. M., Wieringa, M. S., Eigenraam, A. W., &
Hamelink, K. (2018). Social comparison as the thief of joy: Emotional
consequences of viewing strangers’ Instagram posts. Media Psychology,
21, 222–245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2016.1267647
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–
75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Dorethy, M. D., Fiebert, M. S., & Warren, C. R. (2014). Examining social
networking site behaviors: Photo sharing and impression management
on Facebook. International Review of Social Sciences and Humanities,
6, 111–116.
Edwards, L. J., Muller, K. E., Wolfinger, R. D., Qaqish, B. F., & Scha-
benberger, O. (2008). An R2 statistic for fixed effects in the linear mixed
model. Statistics in Medicine, 27, 6137– 6157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
sim.3429
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Face-
book “friends:” Social capital and college students’ use of online social
network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12,
1143–1168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Fardouly, J., Pinkus, R. T., & Vartanian, L. R. (2017). The impact of
appearance comparisons made through social media, traditional media,
and in person in women’s everyday lives. Body Image, 20, 31–39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.11.002
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Negative comparisons about one’s
appearance mediate the relationship between Facebook usage and body
image concerns. Body Image, 12, 82– 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.bodyim.2014.10.004
Feinstein, B., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J., Meuwly, N., &
Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook and depres-
sive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism. Psychology of Popular
Media Culture, 2, 161–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033111
Fairchild, A. J., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2009). A General Model for Testing
Mediation and Moderation Effects. Prevention Science, 10, 87–99.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-008-0109-6
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., Karney, B. R., Reis, H. T., & Sprecher, S.
(2012). Online dating: A critical analysis from the perspective of psy-
chological science. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13,
3– 66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100612436522
Geldhof, G. J., Anthony, K. P., Selig, J. P., & Mendez-Luck, C. A. (2018).
Accommodating binary and count variables in mediation: A case for
conditional indirect effects. International Journal of Behavioral Devel-
opment, 42, 300 –308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025417727876
Gerber, J. P., Wheeler, L., & Suls, J. (2018). A social comparison theory
meta-analysis 60 years on. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 177–197.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000127
Gerson, J., Plagnol, A. C., & Corr, P. J. (2016). Subjective well-being and
social media use: Do personality traits moderate the impact of social
comparison on Facebook? Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 813– 822.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.023
Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social
comparison: Development of a scale of social comparison orientation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 129 –142. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129
Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1989). Effects of upward and downward
social comparisons on mood states. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 8, 14 –31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1989.8.1.14
Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons
arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 227–236.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.227
Greenwood, S., Perrin, A., & Duggan, M. (2016, November 11). Social
Media Update 2016. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/
11/11/social-media-update-2016/
Hanna, E., Ward, L. M., Seabrook, R. C., Jerald, M., Reed, L., Giaccardi,
S., & Lippman, J. R. (2017). Contributions of social comparison and
self-objectification in mediating associations between Facebook use and
emergent adults’ psychological well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking, 20, 172–179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber
.2016.0247
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi-
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation.
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50, 1–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00273171.2014.962683
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2010). Quantifying and testing indirect
effects in simple mediation models when the constituent paths are
nonlinear. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 45, 627– 660. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/00273171.2010.498290
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2014). Statistical mediation analysis with
a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathemati-
cal and Statistical Psychology, 67, 451– 470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
bmsp.12028
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a
scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60, 895–910. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.895
Howell, D. (2013). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Hwang, J., Cheong, P., & Feeley, T. (2009). Being young and feeling blue
in Taiwan: Examining adolescent depressive mood and online and
offline activities. New Media & Society, 11, 1101–1121. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/1461444809341699
Jang, K., Park, N., & Song, H. (2016). Social comparison on Facebook: Its
antecedents and psychological outcomes. Computers in Human Behav-
ior, 62, 147–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.082
Kalpidou, M., Costin, D., & Morris, J. (2011). The relationship between
Facebook and the well-being of undergraduate college students. Cyber-
psychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 183–189. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
Ko, H.-C., & Kuo, F.-Y. (2009). Can blogging enhance subjective well-
being through self-disclosure? CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12, 75–79.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.016
Krasnova, H., Wenninger, H., Widjaja, T., & Buxmann, P. (2013). Envy on
Facebook: A hidden threat to users’ life satisfaction? 11th International
Conference on Wirtschaftsinformatik (pp. 1–16). Leipzig, Germany:
Wirtschaftsinformatik.
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., . . .
Ybarra, O. (2013). Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-
being in young adults. PLoS ONE, 8, e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0069841
Leahey, T. M., Crowther, J. H., & Mickelson, K. D. (2007). The frequency,
nature, and effects of naturally occurring appearance-focused social
comparisons. Behavior Therapy, 38, 132–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.beth.2006.06.004
Lee, S. Y. (2014). How do people compare themselves with others on
social network sites? The case of Facebook. Computers in Human
Behavior, 32, 253–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009
Liu, D., Ainsworth, S. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). A meta-analysis of
social networking online and social capital. Review of General Psychol-
ogy, 20, 369 –391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000091
Liu, Q.-Q., Zhou, Z.-K., Yang, X.-J., Niu, G.-F., Tian, Y., & Fan, C.-Y.
(2017). Upward social comparison on social network sites and depres-
sive symptoms: A moderated mediation model of self-esteem and opti-
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
21SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.029
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.029
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2016.1267647
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.3429
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.3429
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.11.002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-008-0109-6
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1529100612436522
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025417727876
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000127
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.023
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1989.8.1.14
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.227
http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/11/11/social-media-update-2016/
http://www.pewinternet.org/2016/11/11/social-media-update-2016/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0247
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0247
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.962683
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.962683
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2010.498290
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2010.498290
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.12028
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bmsp.12028
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.895
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461444809341699
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461444809341699
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.082
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.06.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.06.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000091
mism. Personality and Individual Differences, 113, 223–228. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.037
Lo, I. S., & McKercher, B. (2015). Ideal image in process: Online tourist
photography and impression management. Annals of Tourism Research,
52, 104 –116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annals.2015.02.019
Locke, K. D. (2003). Status and solidarity in social comparison: Agentic
and communal values and vertical and horizontal directions. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 619 – 631. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-3514.84.3.619
Locke, K. D. (2005). Connecting the horizontal dimension of social com-
parison with self-worth and self-confidence. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 31, 795– 803. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0146167204271634
Locke, K. D., & Nekich, J. C. (2000). Agency and communion in natu-
ralistic social comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
26, 864 – 874. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200269011
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the
impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 91–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91
Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or
preventing failure: Cultural differences in motivation by positive and
negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31,
379 –392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271598
Lockwood, P., & Pinkus, R. T. (2008). The impact of social comparisons
on motivation. In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of
motivation science (pp. 251–264). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lockwood, P., Shaughnessy, S. C., Fortune, J. L., & Tong, M.-O. (2012).
Social comparisons in novel situations: Finding inspiration during life
transitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 985–996.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167212447234
Maniaci, M. R., & Rogge, R. D. (2014). Caring about carelessness:
Participant inattention and its effects on research. Journal of Research in
Personality, 48, 61– 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.008
Matar Boumosleh, J., & Jaalouk, D. (2017). Depression, anxiety, and
smartphone addiction in university students- A cross sectional study.
PLoS ONE, 12, e0182239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone
.0182239
Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. (1970). Social comparison, self-consistency,
and the concept of self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
16, 148 –156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029862
Muise, A., Christofides, E., & Desmarais, S. (2009). More information than
you ever wanted: Does Facebook bring out the green-eyed monster of
jealousy? Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12, 441– 444. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0263
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mech-
anisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472– 489. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.472
Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of
social comparison: Mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 832– 844. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0022-3514.87.6.832
Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (1999). Hypothesis-consistent testing and
semantic priming in the anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility
model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 136 –164. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1364
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998 –2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Page-Gould, E., & Sharples, A. (2016). Bootstrapping accurate indirect
effects in multilevel models. Retrieved from http://www.page-gould.com/
r/indirectmlm/
Patrick, H., Neighbors, C., & Knee, C. R. (2004). Appearance-related
social comparisons: The role of contingent self-esteem and self-
perceptions of attractiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
tin, 30, 501–514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203261891
Pew Research Center. (2019). Social media fact sheet. Retrieved from
http://www.pewinternet.org/fact-sheet/social-media/
Pinkus, R. T., Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Yoon, H. M. (2012).
Responses to comparisons in romantic relationships: Empathy, shared
fate, and contrast. Personal Relationships, 19, 182–201. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01347.x
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling
strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple me-
diator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879 – 891. http://dx.doi
.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
Qiu, L., Lin, H., Leung, A. K., & Tov, W. (2012). Putting their best foot
forward: Emotional disclosure on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behav-
ior, and Social Networking, 15, 569 –572. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
cyber.2012.0200
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/97814
00876136
Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of
social-comparison jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 47, 780 –792. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.780
Seidman, G. (2013). Self-presentation and belonging on Facebook: How
personality influences social media use and motivations. Personality and
Individual Differences, 54, 402– 407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid
.2012.10.009
Stapleton, P., Luiz, G., & Chatwin, H. (2017). Generation validation: The
role of social comparison in use of Instagram among emerging adults.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20, 142–149. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0444
Steers, M. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone
else’s highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive
symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33, 701–731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
Stolzenberg, R. M. (1980). The measurement and decomposition of causal
effects in nonlinear andnonadditive models. Sociological Methodology,
11, 459 – 488.
Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social comparison: Why, with
whom, and with what effect? Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
ence, 11, 159 –163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00191
Tandoc, E. C., Jr., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy,
and depression among college students: Is Facebooking depressing?
Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139 –146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.chb.2014.10.053
Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social
behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
chology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). New York, NY: Academic Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60227-0
Tesser, A., Millar, M., & Moore, J. (1988). Some affective consequences
of social comparison and reflection processes: The pain and pleasure of
being close. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 49 – 61.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.49
Tesser, A., & Paulhus, D. (1983). The definition of self: Private and public
self-evaluation management strategies. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology, 44, 672– 682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44
.4.672
Thai, S., & Lockwood, P. (2015). Comparing you � comparing me: Social
comparisons of the expanded self. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 41, 989 –1004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167215588094
Thai, S., Lockwood, P., & Boksh, R. J. (2020). Committed to succeed:
Commitment determines reactions to upward relationship comparisons.
Personal Relationships, 27, 303–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pere
.12316
Thai, S., Lockwood, P., Pinkus, R. T., & Chen, S. Y. (2016). Being better
than you is better for us: Attachment avoidance and social comparisons
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
22 MIDGLEY, THAI, LOCKWOOD, KOVACHEFF, AND PAGE-GOULD
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.037
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.037
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annals.2015.02.019
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271634
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271634
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200269011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271598
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167212447234
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182239
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182239
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029862
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0263
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0263
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.472
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.472
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.832
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.832
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1364
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1998.1364
http://www.page-gould.com/r/indirectmlm/
http://www.page-gould.com/r/indirectmlm/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203261891
http://www.pewinternet.org/fact-sheet/social-media/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01347.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01347.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0200
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0200
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9781400876136
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9781400876136
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.780
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0444
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0444
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00191
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601%2808%2960227-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.49
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.672
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.4.672
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167215588094
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pere.12316
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pere.12316
within romantic relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
ships, 33, 493–514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407515578826
Thai, S., & Page-Gould, E. (2018). ExperienceSampler: An open-source
scaffold for building smartphone apps for experience sampling. Psycho-
logical Methods, 23, 729 –739. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000151
Tien, S., & Aynsley, M. (2019, July 15). The best time to post on
Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, and LinkedIn. Retrieved from Hootsuite
website https://blog.hootsuite.com/best-time-to-post-on-facebook-
twitter-instagram/
Twenge, J. (2017). IGen: Why today’s super-connected kids are growing
up less rebellious, more tolerant, less happy—And completely unpre-
pared for adulthood—And what that means for the rest of us. New York,
NY: Simon & Schuster.
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking
sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-
esteem. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 9, 584 –590. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., . . .
Kross, E. (2015). Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-
being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 144, 480 – 488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
xge0000057
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do
social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A
critical review. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11, 274 –302. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12033
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Okdie, B. M., Eckles, K., & Franz, B. (2015).
Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation
on social media use and its outcomes. Personality and Individual Dif-
ferences, 86, 249 –256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.026
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Roberts, L. R., & Eckles, K. (2014). Social
comparison, social media, and self-esteem. Psychology of Popular Me-
dia Culture, 3, 206 –222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000047
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2004). Ego threats elicits different social
comparison process among high and low self-esteem people: Implica-
tions for interpersonal perceptions. Social Cognition, 22, 168 –191.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.1.168.30983
Walther, J. B. (2007). Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated
communication: Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language,
and cognition. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 2538 –2557. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2006.05.002
Wang, J.-L., Wang, H.-Z., Gaskin, J., & Hawk, S. (2017). The mediating
roles of upward social comparison and self-esteem and the moderating
role of social comparison orientation in the association between social
networking site usage and subjective well-being. Frontiers in Psychol-
ogy, 8, 771. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00771
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Wayment, H. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1995). Self-evaluation processes: Mo-
tives, information use, and self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 63,
729 –757. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.tb00315.x
Wheeler, L., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). The proxy model of social
comparison for self-assessment of ability. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Review, 1, 54 – 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957
pspr0101_4
Wheeler, L., & Miyake, K. (1992). Social comparison in everyday life.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 760 –773. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.760
Wheeler, L., & Reis, H. T. (1991). Self-recording of everyday life events:
Origins, types, and uses. Journal of Personality, 59, 339 –354. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00252.x
Wilson, R. E., Gosling, S. D., & Graham, L. T. (2012). A review of
Facebook research in the social sciences. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 7, 203–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612442904
Wood, J. V. (1989). Theory and research concerning social comparisons of
personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 231–248. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231
Wood, J. V., Giordano-Beech, M., Taylor, K. L., Michela, J. L., & Gaus,
V. (1994). Strategies of social comparison among people with low
self-esteem: Self-protection and self-enhancement. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 67, 713–731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.67.4.713
Wood, J. V., & Lockwood, P. (1999). Social comparisons in dysphoric and
low self-esteem people. In R. Kowalski & M. Leary (Eds.), The social
psychology of emotional and behavioral problems: Interfaces of social
and clinical psychology (pp. 97–135). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10320-004
Wood, J. V., Michela, J. L., & Giordano, C. (2000). Downward comparison
in everyday life: Reconciling self-enhancement models with the mood-
cognition priming model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
79, 563–579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.563
Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social media use in
adolescence is associated with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression
and low self-esteem. Journal of Adolescence, 51, 41– 49. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.008
Zhang, Z., Zyphur, M. J., & Preacher, K. J. (2009). Testing multilevel
mediation using hierarchical linear models. Organizational Research
Methods, 12, 695–719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428108327450
Zhao, S., Grasmuck, S., & Martin, J. (2008). Identity construction on
Facebook: Digital empowerment in anchored relationships. Computers
in Human Behavior, 24, 1816 –1836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb
.2008.02.012
Received January 22, 2019
Revision received June 24, 2020
Accepted June 25, 2020 �T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
23SOCIAL MEDIA COMPARISONS AND SELF-ESTEEM
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407515578826
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000151
https://blog.hootsuite.com/best-time-to-post-on-facebook-twitter-instagram/
https://blog.hootsuite.com/best-time-to-post-on-facebook-twitter-instagram/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12033
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12033
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.026
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000047
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.1.168.30983
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2006.05.002
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2006.05.002
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00771
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.tb00315.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.760
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.760
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00252.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00252.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612442904
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.713
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.713
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10320-004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.563
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428108327450
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.012
When Every Day Is a High School Reunion: Social Media Comparisons and Self-Esteem
Social Media Is Associated With Threatening Social Comparisons
Social Media and Upward Comparison Frequency
Social Media and Upward Comparison Extremity
Self-Esteem and Social Media Comparisons
The Present Research
Study 1
Method
Participants
Procedure
Comparison questions
Post-social media questionnaire
Results
Overview of analyses
Individual comparisons
Domains
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations
Self-esteem and self-evaluations
Overall session
Comparison frequency and postsession outcomes
Self-esteem and postsession outcomes
Discussion
Study 2
Method
Participants
Procedure
Results
Overview of analyses
Order effects
Comparison direction and extremity
Self-esteem and comparison extremity
Self-evaluations
Self-esteem and self-evaluations
Discussion
Study 3
Method
Participants
Procedure
Prescreen survey
Study questionnaire
Results
Overview of analyses
Individual comparisons
Domains
Domain importance
Closeness to target
Direction, extremity, and self-evaluations
Overall session
Comparison frequency
Self-esteem and upward comparison frequency
Postsession self-evaluations
Self-esteem and postsession self-evaluations
Discussion
Study 4
Method
Participants
Procedure
Results
Overview of analyses
Individual comparisons
Domains
Domain importance
Closeness to target
Comparison direction, extremity, and self-evaluations
Overall experience sampling period
Context and comparison likelihood
Context and comparison frequency
Discussion
General Discussion
Upward Comparison Frequency on Social Media
Upward Comparison Extremity on Social Media
Self-Esteem and Comparisons on Social Media
Domain Importance and Closeness
Future Directions and Conclusions
References
Journals/Journal 3
The Effects of Facebook on Mood in Emerging Adults
Erica K. Yuen, Erin A. Koterba, Michael J. Stasio, Renee B. Patrick, Cynthia Gangi, Philip Ash,
Kathleen Barakat, Virginia Greene, William Hamilton, and Briana Mansour
University of Tampa
Social media usage is on the rise, with the majority of American adults using Facebook. The present study
examined how Facebook activity affects mood in a subset of emerging adults, specifically undergraduates
attending a private 4-year university. Participants (N � 312) were randomly assigned to one of the
following 20-min activities: browse the Internet, passively browse others’ Facebook profiles, actively
communicate with others on Facebook via messages/posts, or update their own personal profile on
Facebook. Participants also completed questionnaires assessing mood, feelings of envy, and perceived
meaningfulness of their time online. The results demonstrated that using Facebook led to significantly
worsened mood compared with browsing the Internet, especially when participants passively browsed
Facebook. Furthermore, perceptions of meaningfulness, but not feelings of envy, mediated the relation-
ship between online activity and mood. Overall, these findings add to the mounting evidence that social
media use may, at times, adversely affect psychological well-being.
Public Policy Relevance Statement
College students reported lower mood when passively browsing Facebook compared with other
online activities, possibly due in part to feelings of wasted time. Results suggest that repeated use of
social media may adversely affect psychological well-being in some emerging adults.
Keywords: Facebook, social media, mood, envy
In the past decade, social media usage has grown dramatically.
In fact, the proportion of American adults using social media rose
from 7% in 2005% to 65% in 2015 (Perrin, 2015), an increase of
nearly 1,000%. Although many social media sites are popular,
Facebook (www.facebook.com) dominates (Duggan, Ellison,
Lampe, Lenhart, & Madden, 2015), currently boasting over one
billion daily and over 1.7 billion monthly users worldwide (Face-
book, 2016). Furthermore, out of users of only one social media
site, Facebook is the platform of choice for 79% (Duggan et al.,
2015). Therefore, it is important to understand the potential psy-
chological effects that using Facebook may have on its users.
Although not all individuals worldwide with a Facebook profile
use it regularly, the majority appear to do so. Indeed, 70% of
Facebook users logged in daily in 2014 (Duggan et al., 2015). In
fact, most (87%; Perrin, 2015) are using Facebook multiple times
a day, logging in an average of seven times per day (Junco, 2013)
at all hours of the day (Pempek, Yermolayeva, & Calvert, 2009).
Most studies examining daily Facebook usage rates suggest an
average of roughly 30 min of use per day (Jelenchick, Eickhoff, &
Moreno, 2013; Pempek et al., 2009). Others report much higher
average usage rates, ranging from roughly 60 min per day (Kal-
pidou, Costin, & Morris, 2011; Skues, Williams, & Wise, 2012) to
145 min per day (Junco, 2013).
Emerging adults, defined as individuals aged 18 –29 (Arnett,
2000), are particularly frequent users of social media. In fact, in
2015, 90% of emerging adults used social media (Perrin, 2015), an
increase from 84% in 2013 (Duggan et al., 2015). College stu-
dents, a subset of emerging adults, are often studied for their social
media usage, and their use of Facebook in particular has received
considerable empirical attention. Ample empirical evidence sug-
gests that Facebook is ingrained into college students’ daily lives
(Pempek et al., 2009; Wilson, Gosling, & Graham, 2012). This is
particularly apparent in the staggering 94% of college students
who have a Facebook profile (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007).
With so many emerging adults logging on frequently, it is essential
to understand how Facebook affects this population specifically.
An ample literature has investigated the emotional conse-
quences of Facebook use in adults and adolescents. However, no
clear answer has emerged; some studies have linked Facebook use
with negative outcomes, (O’Keeffe & Clarke-Pearson, 2011;
Skues et al., 2012), whereas others find no such link (Jelenchick et
al., 2013). Some studies even point to positive outcomes associated
with Facebook use (Gentile, Twenge, Freeman, & Campbell,
2012; Gonzales & Hancock, 2011).
Negative effects of Facebook use have been reported for many
psychological constructs such as depression. Several studies have
This article was published Online First January 18, 2018.
Erica K. Yuen, Erin A. Koterba, Michael J. Stasio, Renee B. Patrick,
Cynthia Gangi, Philip Ash, Kathleen Barakat, Virginia Greene, William
Hamilton, and Briana Mansour, Department of Psychology, University of
Tampa.
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in
the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Erica K.
Yuen, Department of Psychology, University of Tampa, 401 West Ken-
nedy Boulevard, Box Q, Tampa, FL 33606. E-mail: eyuen@ut.edu
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Psychology of Popular Media Culture © 2018 American Psychological Association
2019, Vol. 8, No. 3, 198 –206 2160-4134/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000178
198
http://www.facebook.com
mailto:eyuen@ut.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000178
shown that as time spent on Facebook increases, depressive symp-
toms also increase (Pantic et al., 2012). Experience sampling
techniques have demonstrated that being on Facebook is linked to
worsened mood later in that same day (Kross et al., 2013). In fact,
some researchers have gone so far as to suggest that Facebook
itself creates a form of depression termed “Facebook depression”
(O’Keeffe & Clarke-Pearson, 2011). In other words, spending too
much time on social media leads to increased depressive symp-
toms, which are in turn tied to other unhealthy behaviors such as
substance abuse and risky sexual behaviors.
The relationship between Facebook use and depressive symp-
toms might not be as straightforward as these studies suggest,
however. Several studies have reported no link between Facebook
usage and negative consequences such as depression (Jelenchick et
al., 2013) or well-being (Lee, Lee, & Kwon, 2011). Other studies
found only indirect links between Facebook use and depression;
for example, frequently using Facebook was associated with
higher levels of envy, which in turn predicted depression (Tandoc,
Ferrucci, & Duffy, 2015). Interestingly, Facebook users are not
necessarily aware of their feelings of envy (Krasnova, Wenninger,
Widjaja, & Buxmann, 2013). These findings suggest that several
psychosocial processes, many of which are unknown, may mediate
the causal relationship between Facebook use and depressed mood.
Sagioglou and Greitemeyer (2014) suggested that the emotional
effects of using Facebook are determined by users’ perception of
the meaningfulness of the act of using Facebook. In their experi-
ment, participants were assigned to one of three conditions: spend
20 min using Facebook, spend 20 min browsing the Internet
(without accessing social media), or no additional Internet activity
(control group). Participants then rated (a) how meaningful the
past 20 min had been, and (b) their current mood levels on the
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark,
& Tellegen, 1988). Results suggested that those who spent time on
Facebook rated their time as being significantly less meaningful
than those who did not spend time on Facebook. Also, the Face-
book group reported a significantly lower mood compared with the
other groups, which was mediated by perceived meaningfulness of
activity. Thus, the authors concluded that if Facebook users be-
lieve their time spent on the platform was not meaningful, then
detrimental links between Facebook use and mood are present.
However, two weaknesses of this study raise cause for inter-
preting these results with caution. First, the authors combined the
distinct positive and negative scales of the PANAS by reverse
scoring the negative affect items to create an aggregate measure of
positive affect rather than assessing both dimensions of mood
separately. Second, the study used Mechanical Turk, which does
not monitor participant activity. Therefore, the authors could not
observe whether participants actually completed their assigned
tasks, such as using Facebook for 20 min. The authors themselves
call for an experimental laboratory study in which oversight of the
study tasks can take place to address this issue (Sagioglou &
Greitemeyer, 2014).
An alternative explanation for the discrepant research findings
regarding the effects of Facebook use might be due to the specific
Facebook activities participants engage in. Facebook is a broad
platform with many features that afford different types of use
(Smock, Ellison, Lampe, & Wohn, 2011). For example, features
include individual user profile pages (including a “Timeline” and
“About Me” section) where one posts information about the self,
including hobbies, interests, photos, videos, and “status updates.”
In addition, users can post messages, photos, or videos to other
people’s profile pages. Another popular feature of Facebook is the
News Feed, where information regarding online friends appears.
This section includes friends’ status updates, photos, and videos, as
well as advertiser-sponsored links. Facebook has private functions
as well, such as a messenger application allowing information to
be shared privately between specific users.
Facebook users’ behaviors can be classified as “passive” or
“active” based on whether the user is actively posting information.
For example, one could passively view the News Feed, or one
could actively engage by posting photos and status updates or
typing comments on others’ profiles. Indeed, several studies con-
firm that users interact with Facebook in a myriad of ways (Ryan
& Xenos, 2011) and engage with Facebook passively more often
than actively (Pempek et al., 2009).
Many studies approach Facebook use and its subsequent effects
in a global fashion rather than parsing out the effects of different
features. However, perhaps Facebook researchers should concep-
tualize Facebook as a collection of separate tools rather than a
global platform (Smock et al., 2011) and investigate the effects of
various functions individually. It is possible that how users engage
with this particular social media platform might dictate how it
affects them. Specifically, perhaps whether one actively posts
versus passively views information on Facebook will affect users
in different ways (Nadkarni & Hofmann, 2012; Rosen, Whaling,
Rab, Carrier, & Cheever, 2013).
Some evidence suggests that passive Facebook use can lead to
increased envy and worsened mood due to upward comparison.
Social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954) suggests that we obtain
valuable information about ourselves through the process of com-
parison with others. Comparisons can take many forms. They can
be upward (viewing another as better than the self), downward
(viewing another as inferior to the self), or nondirectional. Time
spent on Facebook has indeed been linked to increased social
comparisons (Lee, 2014), particularly upward comparisons (John-
son & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2014). Social comparisons online
have been tied to increased depressive symptomatology (Steers,
Wickham, & Acitelli, 2014). Interestingly, the link between com-
parisons on Facebook and increased depressive symptoms holds
regardless of the direction of the comparison, which is not the case
in offline interactions.
Both envy and rumination potentially explain this link. When
Facebook comparisons induce envy, depressive symptoms in-
crease (Tandoc et al., 2015) and life satisfaction decreases (Kras-
nova et al., 2013). Also, engaging in comparisons on Facebook is
significantly tied to rumination, which in turn significantly predicts
depression (Feinstein et al., 2013). Therefore, it appears as though
an indirect link between passive Facebook use and depression
exists, but that link is mediated by several factors.
Not all evidence suggests that online interactions are detrimental
to psychological health. In fact, recent literature has documented
the supportive and positive effects of actively engaging with
Facebook (Oh, Ozkaya, & LaRose, 2014). For those who are
actively engaging with the platform (e.g., by posting messages), it
appears that loneliness is reduced (Wilson et al., 2012) and self-
esteem is enhanced (Gentile et al., 2012; Gonzales & Hancock,
2011). This effect extends to receiving comments from others
(Greitemeyer, Mügge, & Bollermann, 2014). These results are
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
199EFFECTS OF FACEBOOK ON MOOD
particularly meaningful given that the majority of Facebook users
use Facebook “actively” at least some of the time (Duggan et al.,
2015; Pempek et al., 2009), even if one’s overall time on Facebook
is more passive in nature.
It is also possible that the source of the information posted (e.g.,
a good friend vs. an acquaintance) or the valence of information
viewed (e.g., positive news vs. negative news) might lead to the
varying mood outcomes described above. Indeed, Facebook users
have a variety of online “friends” including family members,
current or former friends or romantic partners, work colleagues,
and more (Duggan et al., 2015). Viewing information from these
sources might differentially induce mood. Furthermore, the emo-
tional tone of others’ posts may be passed along to others through
emotional contagion. A recent, albeit controversial, study by Fa-
cebook employees (Kramer, Guillory, & Hancock, 2014) manip-
ulated users’ news feeds to change the emotional tone of the
majority of messages users were viewing. Completed without the
knowledge of its users, this study assigned nearly 700,000 Face-
book users to one of two conditions. One group’s news feeds were
manipulated to include more negative posts, whereas the second
group was exposed to a higher proportion of positive posts. Ex-
perimenters then recorded the emotions present in participants’
subsequent posts. Indeed, exposure to more positive posts in-
creased positive posts and reduced negative posts, and exposure to
more negative posts increased negative posts and reduced positive
posts. Therefore, it appears as though emotional contagion occurs
via online means, not just in offline or in-person contexts. This
study provides a compelling counterargument that users are
“catching” others’ emotions, in contrast to the social comparison
hypothesis regarding Facebook use.
A final issue exists regarding the literature addressing the effects
of Facebook. Namely, the majority of studies investigating the
effects of Facebook have been questionnaire-based and nonexperi-
mental. As a consequence, it is difficult to determine what actually
causes the differences that are present in the literature. Therefore,
the present study sought to clarify several issues inherent in the
Facebook literature using an experimental design. Specifically, we
aimed to examine in college students (a) the effects of active
versus passive Facebook use on mood, (b) potential mediators
(e.g., envy, perceived meaningfulness) of Facebook’s effect on
mood, and (c) the relationship between type of information (i.e.,
source, valence) viewed on Facebook and mood. Participants with
a Facebook account were randomly assigned to participate in one
of the following 20-min activities: browse the Internet (control
group), passively browse others’ Facebook profiles, actively com-
municate with others on Facebook via messages and posts, or
update their own personal profile on Facebook. At the end of their
online activity, participants completed questionnaires assessing
their mood, feelings of envy, and perceived level of meaningful-
ness of their online activity. Our hypotheses were as follows: (1)
Using Facebook is associated with lower mood (i.e., lower positive
affect, higher negative affect); (2) Actively posting on Facebook
(e.g., messaging others or updating one’s profile) results in higher
mood compared with passively using Facebook; (3) Perceived
meaningfulness of online activity mediates the relationship be-
tween online activity and mood; (4) Envy mediates the relationship
between online activity and mood; (5) Viewing information posted
by friends, family, or current romantic partner is associated with
higher mood; (6) Viewing information posted by a former roman-
tic partner is be associated with lower mood; and (7) Viewing
positive news posted by others is associated with lower mood
(upward comparison).
Method
Participants
Three hundred twelve undergraduates (79% women), ranging in
age from 18 to 25 (M � 18.8), were recruited from general
psychology classes to participate in the study. Inclusion criteria
included having an existing Facebook account. Those who met the
criteria were randomly assigned to one of four conditions, which
resulted in 88 participants in the Surveillance condition, 67 in the
Communicate condition, 86 in the Profile condition, and 71 in
Control condition.
The majority of participants were freshmen (70.5%), followed
by sophomores (19.2%), juniors (5.4%), and seniors (4.8%). Most
participants described their race as Caucasian (76.9%), followed
by African American/Black (7.1%), Multiracial (6.7%), Asian/
Pacific Islander (4.2%), Other (4.2%), and Native American
(0.3%). Participants identified their ethnicity as Hispanic (17.6%)
and Non-Hispanic (81.4%). Students earned extra edit for partic-
ipating.
As for their social media usage, the majority of participants were
frequent Facebook users and reported using the site several times
a day (51%) or week (31%). Participants also reported visiting
other social media sites several times a day: Instagram (77.6%),
Twitter (44.9%), and YouTube (24%). Social media sites with
fewer daily visits included Tumblr (9.0%), Pinterest (8.3%),
Google Plus (7.7%), LinkedIn (0.6%), and Reddit (0.6%).
Measures
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. This widely
established measure consists of 20 adjectives describing emotions
(e.g., excited, irritable, ashamed; Watson et al., 1988). Participants
indicate to what extent they are currently experiencing each emo-
tion on a scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5
(extremely). The measure is divided into separate subscales for
positive affect (PANAS-PA; � � .92 for this sample) and negative
affect (PANAS-NA; � � .82 for this sample), with scores ranging
from 10 to 50 for each subscale. In our study, participants com-
pleted this measure at two different points in time— baseline and
directly after completing their assigned online activity.
Facebook Envy Scale. This is a seven-item composite scale
that measures envy on a 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (strongly
agree) to 5 (strongly disagree; � � .81 for this sample; Tandoc et
al., 2015). Sample items include “I wish I can travel as much as
some of my friends do,” and “Many of my friends have a better life
than me.”
Perceived Meaningfulness of Activity. After completing
their randomly assigned online activities, participants were asked
to reflect on the meaningfulness of these activities (Sagioglou &
Greitemeyer, 2014). The measure consisted of the following three
questions (� � .82 for this sample): “How much do you feel like
you have spent your time on something meaningful?”; “How much
do you feel like you wasted your time?”; and “How much do you
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
200 YUEN ET AL.
feel like you have done something useful?” Responses were given
using a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much).
Online Activity Questionnaire. Participants were asked to
report on their online activities, which included the following:
number of messages sent/posted, time (in minutes) spent viewing
information posted by specific individuals (e.g., family members,
friends, acquaintances, strangers, current romantic partner, former
romantic partner), and time spent viewing positive, negative, and
neutral information.
Procedure
The present study was approved by the university’s institutional
review board, and all participants provided informed consent.
Participants visited a laboratory in small groups. To minimize bias,
participants were told that the purpose of the study was to inves-
tigate college students’ reactions to various websites. After consent
was given, participants completed pretest measures (via Survey-
Monkey) consisting of a demographic questionnaire, a question-
naire about frequency of social media use, and the PANAS. To
mask the true purpose of the experiment, participants also com-
pleted distractor questions that queried preferences for website
designs/icons of well-known web browsers, as well as the impor-
tance of specific website design elements (e.g., attractiveness,
helpfulness, efficiency, controllability).
Next, participants were randomly assigned to one of the four
conditions in which they participated in an online activity for 20
min. In the Surveillance condition, participants logged into Face-
book and were instructed to browse and view material but refrain
from posting pictures, sending messages, or “liking” posts. In the
Communicate condition, participants logged into Facebook and
were instructed to actively communicate with others by posting on
others’ timelines, commenting on others’ posts, and sending mes-
sages. Participants assigned to the Profile condition were in-
structed to stay on their own profile pages and view, edit, or add
content to their “About Me” section and own timeline, as well as
to “like” or respond to others’ posts on their own profile pages. In
the Control condition, participants were instructed to browse the
web but refrain from visiting e-mail sites, social networking sites,
chatrooms, message boards, and dating sites.
At the conclusion of the 20-min activity period, participants
were administered posttest measures (again via SurveyMonkey)
consisting of the PANAS, Facebook Envy Scale, Perceived Mean-
ingfulness of Activity, and the Online Activity Questionnaire.
Participants were debriefed and thanked before dismissal.
Results
The overall goal of the present study was to investigate the
psychological consequences of Facebook use experimentally.
First, the preliminary analyses are presented. Then, the results of
the main analyses are organized by hypothesis.
Preliminary Analyses
Of the 312 participants, seven were excluded from the analyses
because they did not perform their assigned activity. These in-
cluded four participants who did not write any messages in the
Communicate condition (final n � 63), one participant who spent
zero minutes on their profile page in the Profile condition (final
n � 85), and two participants who spent their time working on
their schoolwork online in the Control condition (final n � 69).
Preliminary analyses also revealed significant differences in
baseline mood by condition, F(3, 301) � 4.32, p � .01. Partici-
pants in the Communicate condition reported significantly higher
baseline positive mood than those in the Surveillance and Control
conditions. Due to these baseline inconsistencies, all analyses
pertaining to mood effects were conducted using analyses of
covariance (ANCOVAs; with baseline mood as a covariate) rather
than using pretest–posttest change scores. Research comparing the
statistical validity of these two strategies has demonstrated the
superiority of the former strategy under circumstances in which
baseline values differ by condition (Van Breukelen, 2006).
We then conducted descriptive analyses on participants’ online
activities to identify patterns of behavior within each condition.
Participants in the surveillance condition reported spending the
most time (in minutes) viewing items posted by friends, followed
by organizations, family members, acquaintances, strangers, cur-
rent romantic partners, and then former romantic partners. Regard-
ing the valence of information, participants spent the most time
viewing “neutral” and positive information (Table 1).
For the communicate condition, participants spent the majority
of their time actively communicating with friends, followed by
family members, acquaintances, current romantic partners, orga-
nizations, strangers, and then former romantic partners. The over-
whelming majority of messages/posts were private messages to
one other person. Regarding the valence of information, they spent
the most time viewing positive and neutral posts (Table 2).
Participants in the profile condition were most likely to report
viewing/editing/adding to their timeline and/or photograph al-
bums. The next commonly reported activity was viewing/editing/
adding to their “About Me” section (Table 3).
Lastly, those participants in the control condition visited various
websites. The types of websites most commonly visited, in order
from most to least time spent, were as follows: search engines,
commerce, entertainment, news, and other informational pages
such as Wikipedia (Table 4).
Table 1
Reported Facebook Activity for the Surveillance Condition
Category M (no. of mins) SD
Source of viewed information
Friends 5.12 3.40
Organizations 3.58 4.27
Family 2.55 2.72
Acquaintances 2.38 2.51
Strangers 0.94 1.48
Current romantic partners 0.65 1.77
Former romantic partners 0.59 2.17
Valence of viewed information
Neutral information/news 7.20 5.40
Specific people’s good news (e.g.,
posts, messages, and pictures) 5.46 3.69
General positive information 3.89 3.08
Specific people’s bad news (e.g.,
posts, messages, and pictures) 2.06 2.51
General negative information 1.95 2.48
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
201EFFECTS OF FACEBOOK ON MOOD
General Facebook Use and Mood
First we tested Hypothesis 1 that using Facebook is associated
with lower mood compared with browsing the Internet. Specifi-
cally, we conducted an ANCOVA using a dichotomous condition
variable (all Facebook groups combined vs. Internet browsing),
and with post PANAS-PA as the dependent variable (DV) and pre
PANAS-PA as the covariate. The results demonstrated that Face-
book use led to significantly lower positive mood (M � 23.26, Adj.
M � 22.95, SD � 9.37) compared with browsing the Internet
(M � 24.26, Adj. M � 25.33, SD � 9.04), F(1, 302) � 7.18, p �
.01, �p2 � .02 (Table 5).
To explore effects on negative affect, a similar analysis was
conducted with post PANAS-NA as the DV and pre PANAS-NA
as the covariate. No significant main effect of condition was found,
F(1, 302) � 1.02, p � .31. Negative mood did not differ signifi-
cantly between participants using Facebook (M � 12.56, SD �
4.19) and participants browsing the web (M � 12.16, SD � 3.65).
Active Versus Passive Facebook Use
To test Hypothesis 2 that actively posting on Facebook (i.e.,
Communicate and Profile conditions) results in higher mood com-
pared with passively using Facebook (i.e., Surveillance condition),
we conducted an ANCOVA, with post PANAS-PA as the DV,
condition (all four separated) as the independent variable, and pre
PANAS-PA as the covariate. There was a significant main effect
of condition, F(3, 300) � 3.25, p � .02, �p2 � .03. Simple contrasts
revealed that participants in the Surveillance condition had a
significantly lower positive mood compared with the control
group, 95% confidence interval (CI) � [�5.28, �1.19], p � .01.
In other words, viewing Facebook pages passively lead to signif-
icantly lower positive mood compared with simply browsing the
Internet. No other significant differences for positive mood were
found (p � .05). A similar ANCOVA for negative mood revealed
no significant effect, F(3, 300) � 1.51, p � .21. As a result, we
found only partial support for our hypothesis that active and
passive Facebook use differentially affect mood.
Perceived Meaningfulness of Activity as a Mediator
In addition to a significant effect on positive mood, univariate
analyses yielded a significant effect of activity on perceived mean-
ingfulness, F(1, 303) � 26.51, p � .01, �p2 � .09. Participants who
used Facebook (regardless of the nature of usage) perceived the
activity as significantly less meaningful (M � 3.50, SD � 1.58)
than those who browsed the Internet (M � 4.58, SD � 1.43).
Next, we sought to test Hypothesis 3 that perceived meaning-
fulness of online activity mediates the relationship between online
activity (using Facebook vs. browsing the Internet) and positive
mood. The mediation model and results (with standardized coef-
ficients) are presented in Figure 1. Multiple regression analyses
(controlling for baseline positive mood) were conducted to assess
each component of the model. In concordance with previous
findings, condition was a significant predictor of meaningfulness,
B � �1.14, t(303) � �5.43, p � .01. Also, condition
[B � �2.38, t(303) � �2.68, p � .01] and meaningfulness [B �
0.96, t(303) � 4.05, p � .01] were significant predictors of
positive mood, respectively. We then tested for mediation using
the bootstrapping method with bias-corrected CIs (Hayes, 2009).
The 95% CI for the indirect effect was obtained using 5,000
bootstrapped samples. The results (controlling for baseline mood)
revealed that meaningfulness did indeed mediate the relationship
between condition and positive mood, B � �1.10, CI �
[�1.90, �.50]. Furthermore, the direct effect of condition on
positive mood became nonsignificant when controlling for mean-
ingfulness, B � �1.29, p � .16. In summary, Facebook usage
(compared with browsing the Internet) was viewed as a less
meaningfulness activity which, in turn, led to lowered positive
mood.
Table 3
Reported Facebook Activity for the Profile Condition
Category M (no. of mins) SD
Viewing/editing/adding to personal
timeline and/or photograph albums 13.41 6.26
Viewing/editing/adding to personal
“About Me” section 5.04 5.22
Viewing nonprofile pages 1.49 3.70
Table 4
Reported Online Activity for the Control Condition
Category M (no. of mins) SD
Search engines 4.26 4.31
Commerce 3.22 5.73
Entertainment 3.09 6.00
News 2.66 4.38
Other informational webpages
(e.g., Wikipedia) 2.21 4.45
Table 2
Reported Facebook Activity for the Communicate Condition
Category M SD
Recipient of posts/messages (No. of mins)
Friends 7.25 4.13
Family 3.81 4.20
Acquaintances 1.85 2.39
Current romantic partners 1.76 3.34
Organizations 1.68 2.43
Strangers 0.69 1.53
Former romantic partners 0.11 0.66
Valence of viewed information (No. of mins)
Specific people’s good news (e.g.,
posts, messages, and pictures) 5.28 3.81
Neutral information/news 2.90 3.45
General positive information 2.61 2.52
Specific people’s bad news (e.g.,
posts, messages, and pictures) 1.47 2.28
General negative information 1.11 2.15
Type of messages/posts (No. of posts/
messages)
Private (to one other individual) 13.44 28.72
Public 1.78 2.13
Group 0.71 1.77
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
202 YUEN ET AL.
Envy as a Mediator
To test Hypothesis 4 that envy mediates the relationship be-
tween online activity and mood, we first conducted a one-way
ANOVA to determine if Facebook envy differed between condi-
tions. However, no significant effect was found, F(1, 286) � 0.54,
p � .46. In addition, partial correlations did not find a significant
relationship between envy and mood, r � �.07, p � .25. There-
fore, a mediational analysis was not conducted.
Source of Viewed Information
We next tested Hypothesis 5 (viewing information posted by
friends, family, or current romantic partner is associated with
higher mood) and Hypothesis 6 (viewing information posted by a
former romantic partner is associated with lower mood). A corre-
lational analysis was conducted to look for relationships between
mood and the number of minutes spent viewing information by
various sources (e.g., friend, acquaintance, stranger, current ro-
mantic partner, former romantic partner) for the surveillance con-
dition (Table 6). A partial positive correlation (controlling for
baseline negative affect) was found between negative mood and
viewing information posted by a former romantic partner, r � .49,
p � .01. Hypothesis 5, that viewing information posted by a
former partner is associated with lower mood, was supported.
A partial positive correlation (controlling for baseline positive
affect) was also found between positive mood and viewing infor-
mation posted by a current romantic partner, r � .26, p � .02.
Viewing information posted by a current partner was associated
with increased positive affect (higher mood), thus supporting Hy-
pothesis 6. Interestingly, those with higher baseline negative affect
were more likely to view information from one’s current partner,
r � .31, p � .01. Viewing information posted by acquaintances
was associated with decreased positive affect, r � �.36, p � .01.
No other significant partial correlations between mood and source
of information were found.
Valence of Viewed Information
We conducted another correlational analysis to test Hypothesis
7 that viewing positive news posted by others is associated with
lower mood (upward comparison; Table 7). No significant partial
correlations between mood and valence of information (e.g., time
spent viewing specific individuals’ good or bad news, general
positive or negative information, or neutral information) were
found.
Discussion
Of continuing interest to researchers is whether social media
use, especially Facebook use, negatively impacts users’ mood. The
existing literature is mixed, with some studies finding an associ-
ation between Facebook use and depressed mood, and other stud-
ies finding no link or even a relationship to positive well-being
(see reviews by Appel, Gerlach, & Crusius, 2016; Steers, 2016).
Furthermore, most studies were questionnaire based and did not
use an experimental design, thus limiting causal conclusions. The
present study investigated this unresolved question by using an
experimental design to compare the effects of Facebook use and
Internet browsing on mood among a subset of emerging adults.
Differences in mood were explored for those who actively or
passively engaged in Facebook. Also, special attention was paid to
possible mediating factors (e.g., perceived meaningfulness) that
potentially play a role in the Facebook–mood connection.
Table 5
Means and Standard Deviations by Condition
DV
Surveillance Communicate Profile Control
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Positive mood (pre) 24.24 8.25 28.33 8.28 27.13 7.72 24.68 8.10
Positive mood (post) 20.67 8.44 25.52 10.04 24.26 9.26 24.26 9.04
Negative mood (pre) 14.11 4.84 14.43 5.05 14.24 4.89 14.25 4.92
Negative mood (post) 12.88 4.96 12.17 3.92 12.51 3.47 12.16 3.64
Meaningfulness 3.30 1.47 3.68 1.66 3.56 1.61 4.58 1.43
Envy 19.69 4.59 20.36 5.68 19.41 5.77 20.30 5.24
Figure 1. Standardized coefficients for the indirect effect of condition on positive mood through perceived
meaningfulness. �� p � .01, all two-tailed.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
203EFFECTS OF FACEBOOK ON MOOD
Our findings indicate that Facebook use may have a detrimental
effect on mood for some individuals. Specifically, Facebook use
leads to lower positive affect compared with browsing the Internet,
among emerging adults. Despite a small effect size, this is still
concerning given that many emerging adults are heavy users of
Facebook (Duggan et al., 2015); hence, cumulative negative ef-
fects on mood over time could be substantial. Indeed, over half of
the participants in this study alone reported using Facebook several
times a day, suggesting a possible elevated risk for lowered mood
for these users.
Our study also addressed whether active (e.g., posting) versus
passive (e.g., reading news feed) engagement in Facebook would
affect users differently. Although no difference in mood was
detected between active and passive Facebook users, the results
did indicate that passive use of Facebook in particular led to
decreased positive affect compared with browsing the Internet.
However, contrary to our expectations, there was no evidence to
suggest that feelings of envy mediated the relationship between
Facebook activity and diminished mood (cf. Tandoc et al., 2015).
Participants in the surveillance condition did spend more of their
time passively looking at neutral content (compared with viewing
other people’s positive news), so there may have been less oppor-
tunity for envy-related feelings to form via upward comparison.
Several other experimental studies that did find social comparison/
envy to underlie the relationship between Facebook use and low-
ered affect had participants view appealing “constructed SNS
profiles” meant to induce such feelings (for a review, see Appel et
al., 2016).
In contrast, the present experiment allowed participants to op-
erate in a more genuine social media environment that included
their own friends and family, exposure to other types of more
general information (e.g., news articles from organizations), and
the ability to apply filters to hide the content of certain individuals
that they do not wish to view. It is plausible that the everyday
Facebook experience may not necessarily conjure up these envy-
related feelings, at least among emerging adults. Emerging adults
(relative to adolescents whose self-identities are more underdevel-
oped) may spend less time engaged in social comparison, further
explaining why no association was found in our study (Appel et
al., 2016; Nesi & Prinstein, 2015). In addition, it is also possible
that negative mood from social comparisons occurs more so when
viewing the profiles of acquaintances rather than friends. Our
study found that viewing information posted by acquaintances was
associated with decreased positive affect; however, our partici-
pants spent more time viewing information posted by friends,
family, and organizations, which could explain why we did not
find evidence for envy to mediate the relationship between Face-
book use and mood. Interestingly, a recent review article (Appel et
al., 2016) concluded that there is not yet enough evidence to
support the theory that depressed mood from using Facebook is
mediated by social comparison or feelings of envy.
Instead, we found that Facebook use in general (vs. browsing
the Internet) is perceived as a less meaningful online activity,
which, in turn, decreases positive affect. Currently, there seems to
be only one other study (Sagioglou & Greitemeyer, 2014) that has
investigated and found perceived meaningfulness to be a mediat-
ing factor. Furthermore, the present study addressed two important
methodological issues present in this prior research. As mentioned
previously, for example, Sagioglou and Greitemeyer (2014) used
an aggregate measure of positive affect by combining the items
from the PANAS-PA with reverse scored items from the PANAS-
NA. However, as positive and negative affect have been found to
be two separate constructs (Watson et al., 1988), we examined the
negative and positive mood subscales separately. Moreover, we
addressed a stated limitation/directive in the aforementioned study
that concerned the need for participants’ activities to be supervised
in a laboratory setting. All of our participants were monitored
throughout the duration of the experiment to ensure they complied
with study protocol. These modifications help to bolster the sup-
porting evidence for the underlying role of perceived meaningful-
ness in the connection between Facebook use and mood.
We agree with Sagioglou and Greitemeyer’s (2014) assertion
that it seems counterintuitive for young adults to involve them-
selves in an activity that they see little value in and that ultimately
results in lowered mood. These researchers found that individuals
may still use Facebook because they incorrectly anticipate that this
activity will make them feel better. Although this may generally be
the case, there could be special instances when anticipations ring
true. For example, our study found that individuals who reported a
lower affective state before using Facebook were more likely to
look at information posted by their current significant other. Im-
portantly, reading postings from one’s existing partner was linked
to elevated positive mood, so we can extrapolate that those who
engaged in this particular Facebook activity correctly anticipated a
mood-related boost. In contrast, viewing information posted by a
former romantic partner was associated with increased negative
affect.
It is also important to acknowledge that emerging adults are
engaging in other types of social media in addition to Facebook.
We found that many individuals in our sample (51%) reported
Table 7
Correlation Between Mood and Valence of Viewed Information
Valence
Positive
mood
Negative
mood
Specific people’s good news (e.g., posts,
messages, and pictures) �.09 �.08
Specific people’s bad news (e.g., posts,
messages, and pictures) .06 .06
General positive information (e.g., news article) .19 �.13
General negative information (e.g., news article) �.02 �.03
Neutral information/news �.07 .11
Note. n � 86.
Table 6
Correlation Between Mood and Source of Viewed Information
Source Positive mood Negative mood
Organization/Group .09 �.17
Current romantic partner .26� .11
Former romantic partner .14 .49��
Family members �.09 �.04
Friends .11 �.09
Acquaintances �.36�� .01
Strangers �.19 �.15
Note. n � 86.
� p � .05. �� p � .01.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
204 YUEN ET AL.
visiting Facebook several times a day, but far more (78%) reported
visiting Instagram several times a day. According to a Pew Re-
search Center survey administered in 2014, social media sites such
as Instagram and Twitter are growing in popularity; among emerg-
ing adults who use the Internet, slightly over half (53%) now use
Instagram and 37% use Twitter (Duggan et al., 2015). Could these
more recent forms of social media better fit some emerging adults’
preferences and/or lifestyle, hence making engagement on these
other sites a more meaningful, constructive avenue for boosting
mood? Emerging adults often turn to Instagram (a video/photo
sharing mobile application) to connect with friends, to record their
social activity and to become more popular within their peer group
(Sheldon & Bryant, 2016). Therefore, it may benefit future re-
searchers to investigate the differential effects of various types of
social media on mood.
Limitations of our study include its reliance on self-report data
and convenience sampling. It is conceivable that individuals may
have misunderstood questions, provided socially desirable an-
swers, and/or misremembered pertinent information. For example,
participants reported the numbers of minutes they spent on specific
Facebook activities based on memory. Also, the sample was cho-
sen based on college students’ willingness to volunteer, which may
have limited the generalizability of our findings (Miller, 2013). It
would be interesting to explore the effects of Facebook on mood
with nonstudents in the emerging adult age-group who may view
and experience this type of online activity differently; it is esti-
mated that nonstudents account for about half of all emerging
adults (Mitchell & Syed, 2015).
Future studies may also consider conducting a cross-cultural
examination of the differential effects of social media on mood.
Kim, Sohn, and Choi (2011) found that American college students
are highly motivated to use social networking sites for entertain-
ment purposes, whereas Korean students are more interested in
receiving information and support from others. Perhaps these
different motivations for using social media could influence how
this activity is perceived (e.g., meaningfulness) and its potential
impact on mood. Continued research into the many ways that using
social media influences the emotional experiences of emerging
adults will add to our understanding of adjustment and mental
health issues during this important developmental time in life.
References
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between
Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Current Opin-
ion in Psychology, 9, 44 – 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10
.006
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469 –
480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469
Duggan, M., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C., Lenhart, A., & Madden, M. (2015).
Social media update 2014. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Face-
book “friends:” Social capital and college students’ use of online social
network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12,
1143–1168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Facebook. (2016). Retrieved from http://newsroom.fb.com/company-info/
Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J. A., Meuwly, N., &
Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook and depres-
sive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism. Psychology of Popular
Media Culture, 2, 161–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033111
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
Relations, 7, 117–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202
Gentile, B., Twenge, J. M., Freeman, E. C., & Campbell, W. K. (2012).
The effect of social networking websites on positive self-views: An
experimental investigation. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1929 –
1933. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.05.012
Gonzales, A. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my Facebook
wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 79 – 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
cyber.2009.0411
Greitemeyer, T., Mügge, D. O., & Bollermann, I. (2014). Having respon-
sive Facebook friends affects the satisfaction of psychological needs
more than having many Facebook friends. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 36, 252–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2014
.900619
Hayes, A. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis
in the new millenium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408 – 420.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360
Jelenchick, L. A., Eickhoff, J. C., & Moreno, M. A. (2013). “Facebook
depression?” social networking site use and depression in older adoles-
cents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52, 128 –130. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008
Johnson, B. K., & Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2014). Glancing up or down:
Mood management and selective social comparisons on social network-
ing sites. Computers in Human Behavior, 41, 33–39. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.009
Junco, R. (2013). Comparing actual and self-reported measures of Face-
book use. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 626 – 631. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
Kalpidou, M., Costin, D., & Morris, J. (2011). The relationship between
Facebook and the well-being of undergraduate college students. Cyber-
psychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 183–189. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
Kim, Y., Sohn, D., & Choi, S. M. (2011). Cultural difference in motiva-
tions for using social network sites: A comparative study of American
and Korean college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 365–
372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.08.015
Kramer, A. D., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental
evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 111, 8788 – 8790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320040111
Krasnova, H., Wenninger, H., Widjaja, T., & Buxmann, P. (2013, Febru-
ary). Envy on Facebook: A hidden threat to users’ life satisfaction?
Paper presented at the 11th International Conference on Wirtschaftsin-
formatik, Leipzig, Germany.
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., . . .
Ybarra, O. (2013). Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-
being in young adults. PLoS ONE, 8, e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0069841
Lee, G., Lee, J., & Kwon, S. (2011). Use of social-networking sites and
subjective well-being: A study in South Korea. Cyberpsychology, Be-
havior, and Social Networking, 14, 151–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
cyber.2009.0382
Lee, S. Y. (2014). How do people compare themselves with others on
social network sites? The case of Facebook. Computers in Human
Behavior, 32, 253–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009
Miller, S. A. (2013). Developmental research methods (4th ed.). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mitchell, L. L., & Syed, M. (2015). Does college matter for emerging
adulthood? Comparing developmental trajectories of educational
groups. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 2012–2027. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0330-0
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
205EFFECTS OF FACEBOOK ON MOOD
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
http://newsroom.fb.com/company-info/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.05.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0411
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0411
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2014.900619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2014.900619
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.05.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0061
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.08.015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320040111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0382
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0382
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0330-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0330-0
Nadkarni, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Why do people use Facebook?
Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 243–249. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007
Nesi, J., & Prinstein, M. J. (2015). Using social media for social compar-
ison and feedback-seeking: Gender and popularity moderate associations
with depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43,
1427–1438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0020-0
Oh, H. J., Ozkaya, E., & LaRose, R. (2014). How does online social
networking enhance life satisfaction? The relationships among online
supportive interaction, affect, perceived social support, sense of com-
munity, and life satisfaction. Computers in Human Behavior, 30, 69 –78.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.053
O’Keeffe, G. S., & Clarke-Pearson, K. (2011). The impact of social media
on children, adolescents, and families. Pediatrics, 127, 800 – 804. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0054
Pantic, I., Damjanovic, A., Todorovic, J., Topalovic, D., Bojovic-Jovic, D.,
Ristic, S., & Pantic, S. (2012). Association between online social net-
working and depression in high school students: Behavioral physiology
viewpoint. Psychiatria Danubina, 24, 90 –93.
Pempek, T. A., Yermolayeva, Y. A., & Calvert, S. L. (2009). College
students’ social networking experiences on Facebook. Journal of Ap-
plied Developmental Psychology, 30, 227–238. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.010
Perrin, A. (2015). Social networking usage: 2005–2015. Washington, DC:
Pew Research Center.
Rosen, L. D., Whaling, K., Rab, S., Carrier, L. M., & Cheever, N. A.
(2013). Is Facebook creating “iDisorders”? The link between clinical
symptoms of psychiatric disorders and technology use, attitudes and
anxiety. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 1243–1254. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.012
Ryan, T., & Xenos, S. (2011). Who uses Facebook? An investigation into
the relationship between the Big Five, shyness, narcissism, loneliness,
and Facebook usage. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 1658 –1664.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.02.004
Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Facebook’s emotional conse-
quences: Why Facebook causes a decrease in mood and why people still
use it. Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 359 –363. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003
Sheldon, P., & Bryant, K. (2016). Instagram: Motives for its use and
relationship to narcissism and contextual age. Computers in Human
Behavior, 58, 89 –97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.059
Skues, J. L., Williams, B., & Wise, L. (2012). The effects of personality
traits, self-esteem, loneliness, and narcissism on Facebook use among
university students. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 2414 –2419.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.012
Smock, A. D., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C., & Wohn, D. Y. (2011). Facebook
as a toolkit: A uses and gratification approach to unbundling feature use.
Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 2322–2329. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.chb.2011.07.011
Steers, M.-L. N. (2016). ‘It’s complicated’: Facebook’s relationship with
the need to belong and depression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 9,
22–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.007
Steers, M.-L. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing
everyone else’s highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to de-
pressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33,
701–731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
Tandoc, E. C., Jr., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy,
and depression among college students: Is facebooking depressing?
Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139 –146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.chb.2014.10.053
Van Breukelen, G. J. P. (2006). ANCOVA versus change from baseline
had more power in randomized studies, more bias in nonrandomized
studies [corrected]. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59, 920 –925.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.02.007
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Wilson, R. E., Gosling, S. D., & Graham, L. T. (2012). A review of
Facebook research in the social sciences. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 7, 203–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612442904
Received July 20, 2017
Revision received November 20, 2017
Accepted November 28, 2017 �
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
206 YUEN ET AL.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0020-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0054
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0054
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.010
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.010
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.02.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.059
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.07.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.07.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.8.701
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.02.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691612442904
The Effects of Facebook on Mood in Emerging Adults
Method
Participants
Measures
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
Facebook Envy Scale
Perceived Meaningfulness of Activity
Online Activity Questionnaire
Procedure
Results
Preliminary Analyses
General Facebook Use and Mood
Active Versus Passive Facebook Use
Perceived Meaningfulness of Activity as a Mediator
Envy as a Mediator
Source of Viewed Information
Valence of Viewed Information
Discussion
References
Journals/journal 4
Social Media and Depression Symptoms: A Network Perspective
George Aalbers
University of Amsterdam
Richard J. McNally
Harvard University
Alexandre Heeren
Université Catholique de Louvain
Sanne de Wit and Eiko I. Fried
University of Amsterdam
Passive social media use (PSMU)—for example, scrolling through social media news feeds— has been
associated with depression symptoms. It is unclear, however, if PSMU causes depression symptoms or
vice versa. In this study, 125 students reported PSMU, depression symptoms, and stress 7 times daily for
14 days. We used multilevel vector autoregressive time-series models to estimate (a) contemporaneous,
(b) temporal, and (c) between-subjects associations among these variables. (a) More time spent on PSMU
was associated with higher levels of interest loss, concentration problems, fatigue, and loneliness. (b)
Fatigue and loneliness predicted PSMU across time, but PSMU predicted neither depression symptoms
nor stress. (c) Mean PSMU levels were positively correlated with several depression symptoms (e.g.,
depressed mood and feeling inferior), but these associations disappeared when controlling for all other
variables. Altogether, we identified complex relations between PSMU and specific depression symptoms
that warrant further research into potentially causal relationships.
Keywords: social media, depression, loneliness, stress, network analysis
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
In the past decade, social media such as Facebook and Twitter
have become central to everyday life. Despite their popularity,
controversy abounds regarding their impact on mental health
(Twenge, 2017). Although some studies have shown that social
media use is associated with beneficial effects (e.g., higher self-
esteem; Gonzales & Hancock, 2011), others have identified po-
tential negative effects on well-being via the promotion of stress
(Meier, Reinecke, & Meltzer, 2016), loneliness (Liu & Baumeis-
ter, 2016), and depression symptoms (Appel, Gerlach, & Crusius,
2016).
Social media’s adverse effects may come from passive social
media use (PSMU)—that is, scrolling through news feeds or brows-
ing photographs of friends. Experimental research has shown that
PSMU decreases affective well-being (Verduyn et al., 2015), sense of
belonging (Tobin, Vanman, Verreynne, & Saeri, 2015), and life
satisfaction (Wenninger, Krasnova, & Buxmann, 2014). Furthermore,
cross-sectional research indicates that PSMU positively correlates
with depressed mood (Frison & Eggermont, 2016). As depressed
mood is a core symptom of and a strong predictor of depression
(Boschloo, van Borkulo, Borsboom, & Schoevers, 2016), this obser-
vation suggests that PSMU may constitute a risk factor for depression.
To investigate this possibility, we assessed the link between PSMU
and depression symptoms from a network perspective.
According to the network perspective on psychopathology, de-
pression is a complex, dynamic network of symptoms that cause
each other (Borsboom, 2017). Consider the example of a divorced
man who, ruminating about his romantic loss, cannot sleep for
days and grows tired as time wears on. He descends into a state of
hopelessness and anhedonia, causing him to withdraw from social
life and alienate his loved ones. At night, he worries about his
problems, causing more stress and sleep loss. As illustrated by this
example, the network perspective posits that external conditions,
such as stress (Fried, Nesse, Guille, & Sen, 2015), could trigger
symptoms that activate other symptoms. Therefore, from a net-
This article was published Online First December 3, 2018.
George Aalbers, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amster-
dam; Richard J. McNally, Department of Psychology, Harvard University;
Alexandre Heeren, Department of Psychological Sciences Research Institute
and Institute of Neuroscience, Université Catholique de Louvain; Sanne de
Wit, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam; Eiko I.
Fried, Department of Psychological Research Methods, University of Amster-
dam.
Eiko I. Fried is now at the Department of Clinical Psychology, Leiden
University.
We thank Payton J. Jones, Olivia Losiewicz, and Sacha Epskamp for their
comments on this study. The research design and findings in this article have
been discussed in the authors’ lab meetings (McNally Laboratory, Richard J.
McNally; Habit Lab, Sanne de Wit) and orally presented to a student audience
at the University of Amsterdam. Data, materials, and findings have not been
shared online prior to resubmitting the revised article. George Aalbers devel-
oped the study concept under the supervision of Richard J. McNally, Eiko I.
Fried, Sanne de Wit, and Alexandre Heeren. All authors contributed to the
study design. Data collection and analysis, and interpretation of results were
performed by George Aalbers under the supervision of Eiko I. Fried. George
Aalbers drafted the article, and all other authors provided critical revisions. All
authors approved the final version of the article for submission.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to George
Aalbers, who is now at the Nederlands Psychoanalytisch Instituut (NPI),
Domselaerstraat 128, 1093MB Amsterdam, the Netherlands. E-mail: h.j.g
.aalbers@gmail.com
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
© 2018 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 148, No. 8, 1454 –1462
0096-3445/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528
1454
mailto:h.j.g.aalbers@gmail.com
mailto:h.j.g.aalbers@gmail.com
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528
work perspective, PSMU could function as a depression risk factor
if it triggers individual depression symptoms (e.g., depressed
mood) or conditions (e.g., stress) that trigger other depression
symptoms.
Previous research points to several pathways from PSMU to
depression symptoms. First, PSMU could worsen symptoms (i.e.,
depressed mood and loss of interest) by undermining affective
well-being (Verduyn et al., 2015). Furthermore, PSMU may re-
duce a sense of belonging (Tobin et al., 2015) that predicts lone-
liness (Mellor, Stokes, Firth, Hayashi, & Cummins, 2008). By
indirectly increasing loneliness, PSMU could increase depression
symptoms (Fried et al., 2015) and stress (DeBerard & Kleinknecht,
1995), and, in turn, stress may ultimately reinforce depression
symptoms (Fried et al., 2015). Finally, by exposing individuals to
the highly curated lives of their social media contacts—who (on
average) seem happier and more popular than themselves (Bollen,
Goncalves, van de Leemput, & Ruan, 2017)—PSMU could in-
crease feelings of inferiority (Appel, Crusius, & Gerlach, 2015),
leading to increases in depression symptoms (Blease, 2015).
Conversely, depression symptoms, loneliness, and stress might
increase PSMU. Longitudinal evidence demonstrates that loneli-
ness predicts more social media use (Kross et al., 2013). Further-
more, as individuals use the Internet to alleviate depressed mood
and loneliness (LaRose, Lin, & Eastin, 2003), it is conceivable that
they also use social media to do so. Although researchers have not
assessed directly whether PSMU ameliorates depression symp-
toms and loneliness, social media users do report that they use
PSMU to reduce stress and to relieve boredom (Whiting & Wil-
liams, 2013), which is positively associated with loss of interest
(Goldberg, Eastwood, LaGuardia, & Danckert, 2011). Finally,
repeated PSMU to reduce aversive states may become habitual. By
that point, aversive states could trigger PSMU automatically and
outside awareness (LaRose, 2010). Accordingly, we hypothesized
that PSMU increases depression symptoms, loneliness, and stress,
and vice versa.
We instructed participants to report social media use, depression
symptoms, loneliness, and stress seven times a day for 14 days.
From this high-intensive time-series dataset, we estimated three
types of network structures: contemporaneous associations, rep-
resenting how variables are associated within the same timeframe
(e.g., associations between depressed mood and fatigue within a
timeframe of 2 hr; these likely reflect fast-moving temporal pro-
cesses occurring at a time interval quicker than the sampling
interval; Epskamp, Waldorp, Mõttus, & Borsboom, 2018); tempo-
ral associations, representing how variables are associated from
one time point to next (e.g., fatigue predicts depressed mood
during the next timeframe; such temporal prediction satisfies the
temporal requirement for causation—i.e., that causes must precede
effects—which means that temporal associations could suggest
potential causal pathways between variables); and between-
subjects associations, representing how within-person mean levels
of variables are associated on a larger time-scale (e.g., mean level
of fatigue across participants relates to the mean level of loss of
interest). Investigating networks by using this threefold framework
has become standard practice, and allows complementary views of
the data (e.g., Epskamp, Borsboom, & Fried, 2018; Epskamp et al.,
2017). We expected that PSMU, depression symptoms, loneliness,
and stress would be interconnected by positive temporal and
contemporaneous associations, and included between-subjects as-
sociations to explore whether, on average, participants with higher
levels on PSMU were also higher on depression symptoms, lone-
liness, and stress.
Method
Participants
We recruited undergraduate psychology students (N � 132; 91
females, 41 males) via an online study participation platform. A
priori power analysis has not yet been developed for the analytic
approach used here, so we tried to collect as many participants as
possible within the timeframe of 3 months for George Aalbers’s
master’s thesis. Notably, the sample size is larger than many
recently published studies using the same methodology (e.g., De-
Jonckheere et al., 2017; Pe et al., 2015). Prior to data analysis, we
excluded seven participants who failed to respond to a minimum
number of measurements (�29 out of 98), a cut-off that we chose
after consulting with an experience sampling methodology (ESM)
expert (M.C. Wichers, personal communication, May 15, 2017).
Included participants (N � 125; 87 females, 38 males) had a mean
age of 20.44 years (SD � 1.96) and completed an average of 66.18
measurements (SD � 15.10), with a range between 29 and 92.
Participants received research credits required to complete their
curriculum’s mandatory study participation.
Procedure
At fixed times, participants received prompts on their smart-
phones to complete a 12-item questionnaire (measuring PSMU,
depression symptoms, loneliness, and stress) seven times daily for
14 days. We used the LifeData Company’s RealLife Exp app
(https://www.lifedatacorp.com/) to prompt participants and collect
data (Runyan et al., 2013). We chose to separate measurements by
brief intervals (�2 hr) to investigate subtle dynamical interplay
among the measured variables. During an initial one-on-one in-
structional session with each participant, George Aalbers demon-
strated the smartphone app and defined PSMU as: “You are using
social media without commenting, posting, sharing, or chatting—
that is, you are scrolling through the news feed, looking at photos,
videos, and status updates shared by your social media contacts or
public profiles that you follow.” This procedure was approved by
the University of Amsterdam’s Institutional Review Board.
Materials
We constructed a 12-item questionnaire for the present ESM
study. To minimize participant burden, we prioritized brevity and
selected items most relevant in light of the literature on the effects
of PSMU on depression symptoms. We paraphrased seven items
that commonly occur in the most widely used depression ques-
tionnaires (Fried, 2017) to measure depressed mood, loss of inter-
est, fatigue, concentration problems, feelings of loneliness, inferi-
ority, and hopelessness. To measure stress, we modified a
validated one-item stress measure (Elo, Leppänen, & Jahkola,
2003). In addition to an item measuring time spent on PSMU, we
included an item to measure time spent on active social media use
(ASMU), thereby enabling us to disentangle the effects of passive
versus active use of social media. Furthermore, we paraphrased an
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1455SOCIAL MEDIA AND DEPRESSION SYMPTOMS
https://www.lifedatacorp.com/
item from the Self-Report Habit Index (Verplanken & Orbell,
2003) to measure PSMU automaticity. However, because so many
participants failed to answer the automaticity item, in light of a lot
of missing data, we excluded this item from analyses. Finally, to
distract participants from the study goal, we included an item
measuring whether participants had received news concerning
politics, public events, and issues through social media. Each item
was assessed with a visual analog scale (0 � not at all; 100 � very
much) to prevent restricted range. At each prompt, all items
appeared in randomized order and the following statement pre-
ceded the items: “Please indicate to what extent the following
statements applied to you in the past 2 hours.”
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics. For each participant, we calculated the
mean (i.e., within-person mean) and standard deviation (i.e.,
within-person standard deviation) of each variable. For instance, if
a participant reported depressed mood 70 times, we summed all 70
reported values and divided by 70. We repeated this procedure for
all variables in all participants, resulting in a set of within-person
means of all variables. From these values, we calculated a mean
and standard deviation for each variable (reported in Table 1, first
column). The same procedure was used to calculate the within-
person standard deviation, which resulted in a set of within-person
standard deviations for each variable. We calculated the mean and
standard deviation of these values (reported in Table 1, second
column).
Assumption checks. We used Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests to
check whether each variable was normally distributed. A require-
ment for the statistical model estimated in the present study (i.e.,
multilevel vector auto-regression [VAR]) is that stationarity holds.
This means that the mean and variance of a variable do not change
as a function of time. For each variable of each participant, we
tested for stationarity using the Kwiatkowksi-Phillips-Schmidt-
Shin unit root test (following Bringmann, 2016; Kwiatkowski,
Phillips, Schmidt, & Shin, 1992). For both assumption checks, we
applied Bonferroni correction to adjust p values for multiple test-
ing.
Network estimation and visualization. Using the R package
mlVAR (Epskamp et al., 2018), we estimated contemporaneous
correlations, temporal correlations, between-subjects correlations,
and between-subjects partial correlations among depression symp-
toms, stress, PSMU, and ASMU. Contemporaneous correlations
represent how variables are associated within the same timeframe,
and represent associations that remain after partialing out all other
variables in the network within the same timeframe, and after
partialing out temporal associations among variables. For instance,
a positive contemporaneous correlation between PSMU and lone-
liness indicates that, within the same timeframe of 2 hr, higher
levels of PSMU co-occur with feeling lonelier, after controlling for
all other contemporaneous and temporal relationships. Although a
direction of effect cannot be established in these undirected net-
works, associations likely come from temporal relationships, and
especially relationships that occur at a shorter timeframe than
sampled in the present study (minutes, not hours) will end up in the
contemporaneous network structure. Temporal correlations indi-
cate how a variable is predicted by all other variables (including
itself) at a previous timeframe; these are “partial” correlations
because they represent an association after controlling for all other
temporal effects.
The multilevel VAR model has intercepts for each item, which
represents the mean item level across time. Each variable in each
individual has a mean level, and we can calculate between-subjects
correlations between these mean levels. A positive between-
subjects correlation between PSMU and loneliness indicates that
participants who on average spend more time on PSMU also tend
to have a higher level of loneliness. In the between-subjects partial
correlations network, associations represent correlations between
mean levels of variables while controlling for all other variables in
the network.
The package mlVAR estimates the contemporaneous, temporal,
and between-subjects correlations in two steps. In the first step,
mlVAR estimates temporal and between-subjects associations. In
the present study, this is done by estimating 10 multiple regression
equations. In each equation, one of the variables in our ESM study
is predicted by all variables (including itself) at a previous time-
frame (t�1). Consider the following example equation: depressed
moodt � �0 � depressed moodt�1
��1 � PSMUt�1
��2. In this
equation, �2 represents the partial correlation between PSMU at a
previous timeframe (t�1) and depressed mood at a subsequent
timeframe (t), after controlling for depressed mood at a previous
timeframe (t�1). The intercept of these equations (�0) represents
the value of depressed mood at time t when depressed moodt�1
and PSMUt�1 are equal to zero. Because mlVAR estimates a
random intercept, we can obtain an intercept for each variable in
each participant—the mean value of the variable across 2 weeks.
mlVAR uses these person-specific intercepts (means) to estimate
the between-subjects associations, which are partial correlations
between the person-specific means of all variables. The model
estimated in the first step does not fit the data perfectly; stated
differently, the original data points are not always equal to the
values predicted by the model. In the second step, the residuals of
this model (i.e., the differences between the original data points
and predicted values) are used to estimate partial contemporaneous
associations. mlVAR does this by estimating how the residuals of
one variable are predicted by the residuals of all other variables at
the same timeframe.
To make the model computationally tractable, we forced random
effects of temporal and contemporaneous associations to be orthog-
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations of Within-Person Means and
Within-Person Standard Deviations for All Variables
ESM variable M (SD) SD (SD)
Stress 20.55 (14.53) 16.84 (7.42)
Depressed mood 13.01 (11.23) 12.51 (7.48)
Loss of interest 25.18 (14.71) 20.44 (8.09)
Fatigue 37.07 (17.37) 23.49 (6.67)
Concentration problems 26.56 (14.38) 21.00 (7.19)
Loneliness 11.90 (11.07) 10.88 (7.22)
Feeling inferior 10.36 (10.53) 9.10 (6.53)
Feeling hopeless 11.85 (10.89) 10.84 (6.66)
PSMU 31.27 (14.08) 25.03 (6.03)
ASMU 21.27 (15.38) 18.85 (7.76)
Note. ESM � experience sampling methodology; PSMU � Passive
social media use; ASMU � active social media use.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1456 AALBERS, MCNALLY, HEEREN, DE WIT, AND FRIED
onal (i.e., random slopes and intercepts were uncorrelated). In the
undirected (i.e., contemporaneous and between-subjects) networks,
the model predicts A by B and B by A, resulting in two p values for
the association between the variables. To avoid estimating false pos-
itive associations, we used the conservative AND rule, which means
we only included associations in the model if both coefficients were
significant at a level of p � .05. The AND rule does not apply to
temporal associations, because these are estimated only once. mlVAR
deals with missing data by removing all measurement moments that
include at least one missing observation.
mlVAR returned a between-subjects correlation matrix with many
implausibly high correlations. This might be because mlVAR esti-
mates between-subjects partial correlations and then calculates
between-subjects correlations from these associations, which can lead
to unstable results (S. Epskamp, personal communication, May 29,
2017). To resolve this issue, we consulted with the package author (S.
Epskamp, personal communication, May 29, 2017) who suggested a
different way to calculate these associations (to calculate within-
person means for each variable, and then estimate the correlations
between them). We followed this procedure and report these corre-
lations in the Results section.
Using the R package qgraph, we visualized all aforementioned
associations as networks. These graphs comprise nodes, which
represent the variables, and edges, which represent the associations
between the variables. We plotted all networks with the same
layout, which we determined by averaging over the layout of all
networks based on the Fruchterman-Reingold algorithm (Fruch-
terman & Reingold, 1991). We only included correlations in the
graphs with p values smaller than .05. All adjacency matrices are
available in the online supplementary materials.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
For all ESM variables that we included in the analysis, Table 1
contains means and standard deviations of within-person means
and within-person standard deviations. Responses ranged from 0 to
100 for all variables.
Assumption Checks
Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests indicated that no variable was nor-
mally distributed (p � .001). Distributions indicated bimodality
for some variables (fatigue, concentration problems, loss of inter-
est, stress, PSMU, and ASMU) and right-skew for others (de-
pressed mood, feelings of inferiority, loneliness, and hopeless-
ness). Within-person mean levels were normally distributed for
fatigue, concentration problems, and loss of interest (p � .05), but
not for depressed mood, stress, PSMU, ASMU, and feelings of
loneliness, inferiority, and hopelessness (p � .001). Kwiatkowski-
Phillips-Schmidt-Shin unit root tests suggested stationary data for
all variables in all participants.
Contemporaneous Network
The contemporaneous network in Figure 1 shows the direct
associations between the variables within the same timeframe after
controlling for all other temporal and contemporaneous relations.
PSMU is positively associated with concentration problems, loss
of interest, fatigue, and loneliness, but unrelated to stress and
feeling inferior, hopeless, or depressed. Furthermore, there are
positive associations between ASMU and feelings of inferiority,
and ASMU and concentration problems. Moreover, depression
symptoms are positively associated. For instance, loss of interest is
positively associated with fatigue and concentration problems.
Finally, several depression symptoms, such as concentration prob-
lems and depressed mood, are positively associated with stress.
Temporal Network
The temporal network in Figure 2 demonstrates how PSMU,
ASMU, depression symptoms, loneliness, and stress predict each
other from one timeframe to the next. Fatigue, loneliness, and
Figure 1. Partial contemporaneous correlation network of depression symptoms, stress and social media use.
Solid lines represent positive partial correlations. Thicker lines represent stronger partial correlations. The
thickest line (between active social media use [ASMU] and Passive social media use [PSMU]) represents a
partial correlation of 0.33; the thinnest line (between PSMU and loneliness) represents a partial correlation of
0.03. We only plotted correlations with p-values � .05. See the online article for the color version of this figure.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1457SOCIAL MEDIA AND DEPRESSION SYMPTOMS
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
ASMU positively predict PSMU, but PSMU positively predicts
only AMSU. Furthermore, loneliness and stress positively predict
ASMU, and ASMU negatively predicts fatigue. Moreover, several
depression symptoms predict each other bidirectionally across
time. For instance, depressed mood positively predicts loneliness,
and vice versa. However, not all depression symptoms directly
predict each other. There is no temporal association between, for
example, depressed mood and concentration problems. Finally,
stress positively predicts and is predicted by concentration prob-
lems, depressed mood, and feeling hopeless. Loneliness positively
predicts stress, but not vice versa.
Between-Subjects Network
The between-subjects network in Figure 3 depicts the corre-
lations between intraindividual mean levels of PSMU, ASMU,
depression symptoms, loneliness, and stress. As Figure 3
shows, mean levels of PSMU correlated positively with mean
levels of ASMU, depressed mood, and feelings of loneliness,
hopelessness, and inferiority. ASMU correlated positively with
the same symptoms as well as with stress and concentration
problems. This means that, for instance, students who on aver-
age spent more time on PSMU tended to have a higher mean
level of loneliness.
However, as can be seen in the between-subjects partial corre-
lations network (see Figure 3), PSMU showed only one direct
relation with another variable: ASMU. Thus, the positive zero-
order correlation between PSMU and other nodes decreased upon
controlling for all other items in the network, and became nonsig-
nificant (feeling inferior predicted by PSMU, p � .09; PSMU
predicted by feeling inferior, p � .20; depressed mood predicted
by PSMU, p � .81; PSMU predicted by depressed mood, p � .48;
loneliness predicted by PSMU, p � .89; PSMU predicted by
loneliness, p � .75; hopelessness predicted by PSMU, p � .19;
PSMU predicted by hopelessness, p � .25). One interpretation is
that these relationships cease to exist at the level of partial corre-
lations; another is that the present study lacked sufficient power to
detect small edge coefficients in the partial correlation network.
Finally, we see several positive partial correlations among depres-
sion symptoms, loneliness, and stress; for instance, stress and
fatigue feature unique positive associations, as do depressed mood
and loneliness.
Discussion
Summary of Findings
In an experience sampling study of 125 students, with seven
prompts per day, engaging in PSMU did not predict depression
symptoms, loneliness, or stress. Instead, previous fatigue and
loneliness predicted PSMU, indicating that these symptoms might
lead participants to scroll through social media pages. Within the
same timeframe, PSMU co-occurred with loss of interest, concen-
tration problems, fatigue, and loneliness. These contemporaneous
relations have been commonly interpreted in the literature as
indicative of fast-moving causal processes (Epskamp et al., 2018),
but the lack of temporal precedence does not allow for insights into
the direction of the effects, that is, if PSMU leads to depression
symptoms, vice versa, or both. Finally, we found that participants
who spent more time passively using social media also experi-
enced higher mean levels of depressed mood, loneliness, hopeless-
ness, and feeling inferior. However, when controlling for all vari-
ables in this network structure, PSMU was unrelated to all
variables except for active social media usage. This means either
that there are no partial correlations between PSMU and depres-
Figure 2. Temporal network of depression symptoms, stress and social media use. An arrow from variable A
to variable B represents partial temporal correlations between variable A at t�1 and variable B at t. Solid arrows
represent positive temporal correlations, and the dotted arrow represents a negative temporal correlation. Thicker
arrows represent stronger correlations. The thickest arrow (from depressed mood to loneliness) represents a
partial correlation of 0.09; the thinnest arrow (from stress to feelings of hopelessness) represents a partial
correlation of 0.03. For interpretability, we did not plot auto-regressive correlations (see online supplementary
materials for a graph including all auto-regressive correlations). We only plotted correlations with p-values �
.05. See the online article for the color version of this figure.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1458 AALBERS, MCNALLY, HEEREN, DE WIT, AND FRIED
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
sive symptoms, or that these relations are too weak for detection in
the present analysis.1
Importance of Findings
We believe this is the first study to show that PSMU is con-
temporaneously associated with concentration problems, fatigue,
loneliness, and loss of interest. Given the undirected nature of
these associations, we do not know if PSMU causes these symp-
toms, or vice versa, or both. However, the observation that PSMU
is associated with concentration problems aligns with research
demonstrating that individuals who spend more time on PSMU
tend to have lower attentional control (Alloway & Alloway, 2012).
Possibly, people with poor attentional control tend to get distracted
and are unable to inhibit habitual checking of Facebook. As we
found this effect in the contemporaneous but not in the temporal
network, the present study suggests that this effect occurs on a
small timescale. Furthermore, because loss of interest reflects
reduced positive affect (Nutt et al., 2007), the present study offers
one potential explanation regarding the way PSMU may decrease
affective well-being (Kross et al., 2013; Verduyn et al., 2015).
The positive contemporaneous association between PSMU and
fatigue is in line with research suggesting that social media use
might cause fatigue in individuals who feel overwhelmed by social
media (e.g., because they receive too many messages; Lee, Son, &
Kim, 2016). However, a different interpretation can be derived by
looking at the temporal network, which indicates that PSMU could
be part of a (beneficial) self-regulatory feedback loop: fatigue
¡ � PSMU ¡ � ASMU ¡ � fatigue. Additionally, whereas
Lee, Son, and Kim (2016) hypothesize that the effect of social
media on fatigue is mediated by stress, the present investigation
finds no contemporaneous association between social media and
stress, no temporal relation from social media use to stress and no
(direct) temporal relation from stress to fatigue. One potential
explanation is that the contemporaneous and temporal relations in
the present study pertain to a narrow time window, whereas Lee et
al. (2016) analyzed cross-sectional survey data, which encompass
a larger time window, like the between-subjects partial correlations
network in the present study. However, this network is inconsistent
with findings by Lee et al. (2016): Although fatigue and stress are
positively associated, social media use and stress are not. Alto-
gether, these findings call into question the hypothesis that social
media causes fatigue via stress caused by information overload.
Consistent with Kross et al.’s (2013) findings, we found that
loneliness predicted PSMU and ASMU, but not vice versa, which
suggests a unidirectional relationship between social media use
and loneliness. Unlike Kross et al. (2013; and Verduyn et al.,
2015), we did not find that more time on social media predicts
lower affective well-being across time. This discrepancy might be
explained by differences in operationalization of affective well-
being (momentary affect in previous research vs. mood measures
in the present study) and statistical analysis. Kross et al. (2013) and
Verduyn et al. (2015) estimated the correlation between momen-
tary affect and time spent on social media in the past 2 hr, which
were measured at the same moment. Possibly, this procedure could
have led to recall bias (e.g., when a person feels negative, they
1 In a sensitivity analysis (see online supplementary materials), total
time spent on social media (i.e., sum of PSMU and ASMU) was positively
related to feeling inferior in the between-subjects partial correlation net-
work.
Figure 3. Between-subjects correlations (left panel), and between-subjects partial correlation network (right
panel), representing the correlations between mean levels of depression symptoms, stress, and social media use.
Solid lines represent positive correlations; dotted lines represent negative correlations. Thicker lines indicate
stronger associations. In the left panel, the thickest line (between loneliness and depressed mood) represents a
correlation of 0.94; the thinnest line (between loss of interest and Passive social media use [PSMU]) represents
a correlation of 0.10. In the right panel, the thickest line (between loneliness and depressed mood) represents a
partial correlation of 0.69; the thinnest line (between fatigue and feeling inferior) represents a partial correlation
of 0.18. We only plotted associations with p-values � .05. See the online article for the color version of this
figure.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1459SOCIAL MEDIA AND DEPRESSION SYMPTOMS
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
might overestimate the time they spent on social media in the past
two hours). This issue is less likely to occur in the present study,
because temporal associations pertain to relationships between
variables at different measurement moments. A second reason for
this discrepancy could be that Kross et al. (2013) and Verduyn et
al. (2015) examined the effects of social media in an American
student sample, whereas we did so in a European student sample.
This could suggest that social media use has a different effect on
American students than on European students. A final possibility
is that social media use does predict depressed mood and loss of
interest, but when controlling for all other variables in the network,
these are statistically nonsignificant. However, when we reran our
analyses including only social media use, depressed mood, and
loss of interest, we found no temporal associations from social
media use to mood items.
Furthermore, we did not find evidence that social media causes
individuals to compare themselves with their (ostensibly) superior
contacts, which could cause depression symptoms by increasing
feelings of inferiority (Blease, 2015). Although research shows
that social media exposes individuals to the highly curated lives of
social media users who (on average) seem happier and more
popular (Bollen et al., 2017), present findings suggest that—in a
student population—there is no direct influence from PSMU to
feelings of inferiority.
Finally, this study adds to a growing body of research demon-
strating that individual depression symptoms are differentially
associated with nonsymptom variables, such as psychosocial func-
tioning (Fried & Nesse, 2014). Thereby, it underscores the impor-
tance of modeling individual depression symptoms in research
instead of sum scores or diagnoses that obfuscate crucial informa-
tion (Fried & Nesse, 2015).
Strengths and Limitations
To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to apply a
network perspective to the link between social media and mental
health. Our work extends prior work (e.g., Kross et al., 2013;
Verduyn et al., 2015) in that we went beyond affect, including
more detailed mental health facets such as fatigue and concentra-
tion problems, and applied a recently developed statistical analytic
procedure. An important quality of the present study is that its
experience sampling protocol was almost twice as intensive as
those applied in previous studies on social media and psycholog-
ical well-being (Kross et al., 2013; Verduyn et al., 2015). Experi-
ence sampling is considered an approach with high ecological
validity because it monitors people in daily life. We make all ESM
data, R-syntax, model output, and the correlation matrix of the data
available in the online supplementary materials.
Our study does have several limitations. First, as this was a
student sample, mean levels of depression symptoms, loneliness,
and stress were fairly low. Further, items might have been inter-
preted differently by this nonclinical sample than they would have
been in a clinical sample. For instance, to individuals without
depression, endorsing “I had little interest in doing anything” could
mean they felt bored, whereas in individuals with depression, this
could represent anhedonia. As social media’s adverse effects ap-
pear to be demonstrably stronger in depressed than in nonde-
pressed individuals (Appel, Crusius, & Gerlach, 2015), it remains
possible that clinical samples show stronger and more (temporal)
relations between social media use and depression symptoms.
Therefore, future extensions of the present work in clinical sam-
ples is needed. Finally, although our ESM questionnaire included
a distractor item for social media use (i.e., news), we did not
include additional items to distract from negative affect items. As
a consequence, some participants might have been aware of our
study’s purpose, which could have influenced their responses in
the direction of our hypotheses (e.g., reporting greater loss of
interest when also reporting more PSMU). This will be an inter-
esting challenge moving forward in the emerging field of ESM
studies, with the goal to balance the need of focusing on few items
to reduce participant burden with including sufficient distractors.
This is particularly important when such research has an intensive
sampling protocol as in the present study. One possibility is that
future studies include one or two positive or neutral affect items to
counterbalance the aforementioned potential effects, or include a
social desirability question.
Second, as PSMU may be initiated with minimal awareness
when performed habitually, self-reports may not always accurately
reflect actual PSMU. Conversely, it is possible that individuals
sometimes overestimate how much time they spend on social
media (Junco, 2013). Using self-tracking applications that can
accurately estimate time spent on social media might solve this
problem. We consider this an important direction for future work
on this topic.
Third, although tests suggested that the present data met the
assumption of stationarity, sensitivity analyses suggested that this
assumption might have been violated. One way to deal with this
issue is to detrend nonstationary individual time series data before
running group level analyses. Furthermore, our data did not meet
all assumptions of multivariate normality. This is not unusual in
psychology, but as it is unclear at present how robust the employed
methods are to such violations, results need to be interpreted with
caution. Following a reviewer’s suggestion, we log transformed
the data and reran analyses (reported in the online supplementary
materials as sensitivity analysis), which did not affect the overall
pattern of results. As an additional assumption check, suggested by
two reviewers, we also tested if the residuals of the contempora-
neous network followed a normal distribution. We found that this
was not the case for any of the variables, violating some of the
model assumptions. Because some variables were bimodally dis-
tributed, future research into this issue might benefit from logistic
rather than linear regression. However, to this date, logistic regres-
sion approaches have not been made readily available in common
multilevel analysis routines of ESM data, and these and related
challenges of non-normal residuals require urgent attention in
future methodological research. Finally, network models in cross-
sectional data have greatly benefitted from recent investigations
into the accuracy and stability of network parameters such as edge
weights and centrality estimates (Epskamp et al., 2018). Unfortu-
nately, such checks are not yet available for mlVAR models as
estimated in R, and we look forward to methodological develop-
ments in this field.
Fourth, the present modeling framework is limited to estimating
linear relationships, and cannot capture higher-order interactions
among variables. For instance, it is possible that PSMU only
predicts feelings of inferiority when individuals feel very lonely.
We are looking forward to statistical developments that would
allow to tackle these issues.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1460 AALBERS, MCNALLY, HEEREN, DE WIT, AND FRIED
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
Constraints on Generality
The sample of participants is representative of Dutch undergrad-
uate psychology students at a large university. The present study
included participants from a relatively narrow age band. Because
social media use is highly differentiated by age, it is important to
note that our results are more likely to generalize to young than to
old individuals. Replication studies, using the present study’s ESM
questionnaire (see online supplementary materials), could be con-
ducted in other student populations, age groups, and in clinical
populations, such as depressed individuals. The present findings
depend on algorithms that social media use to determine news feed
content. Changes in these algorithms might lead to different re-
sults.
A Network Approach to Social Media and
Psychopathology
These limitations notwithstanding, we believe the network per-
spective provides important insights into the complex, dynamic
relation between social media and psychopathology. Until re-
cently, this perspective has focused primarily on symptom net-
works (Fried et al., 2017); however, several studies have now
estimated networks that include nonsymptom variables (e.g., Bern-
stein, Heeren, & McNally, 2017; Heeren & McNally, 2016). These
studies and our own study align with the recently proposed ex-
panded network approach, which aims to uncover the network
structure of all variables—symptoms and nonsymptoms—that
could be causally relevant to psychopathology (Fried & Cramer,
2017; Jones, Heeren, & McNally, 2017). We believe the present
study illustrates the utility of this approach and we hope it encour-
ages researchers to investigate the network structure of symptoms
and beyond.
Context of the Research
The general idea for this study was developed by combining
ideas and findings in clinical psychology, psychological research
methods, and communication science. We consider network anal-
ysis an important tool to quantitatively integrate empirical research
from different fields of study, and the present study aimed to do so
for clinical psychology and communication science. Our findings
are best situated in the authors’ novel research program—the
expanded network approach (Fried & Cramer, 2017; Jones et al.,
2017)—which examines the network structure of variables that are
causally relevant to mental health. Future extensions of the present
research are direct replications in clinical samples (e.g., in indi-
viduals with depression, or body image disorders), and other
cultures, and studies that include symptoms of other psychiatric
syndromes (e.g., compulsively checking nonexistent or minor
flaws in physical appearance in individuals with body dysmorphic
disorder).
References
Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2012). The impact of engagement with
social networking sites (SNSs) on cognitive skills. Computers in Human
Behavior, 28, 1748 –1754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.015
Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). The relationship
between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem:
Findings from a large national survey. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 287–
293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.006
Appel, H., Crusius, J., & Gerlach, A. L. (2015). Social comparison, envy,
and depression on Facebook: A study looking at the effects of high
comparison standards on depressed individuals. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 34, 277–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015
.34.4.277
Appel, H., Gerlach, A. L., & Crusius, J. (2016). The interplay between
Facebook use, social comparison, envy, and depression. Current Opin-
ion in Psychology, 9, 44 – 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10
.006
Bernstein, E. E., Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2017). Unpacking rumi-
nation and executive control: A network perspective. Clinical Psycho-
logical Science, 5, 816 – 826. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/216770
2617702717
Blease, C. R. (2015). Too many ‘friends’, too few ‘likes’? Evolutionary
psychology and ‘Facebook depression’. Review of General Psychology,
19, 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000030
Bollen, J., Goncalves, B., van de Leemput, I., & Ruan, G. (2017). The
happiness paradox: Your friends are happier than you. EPJ Data Sci-
ence, 6, 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1140/epjds/s13688-017-0100-1
Borsboom, D. (2017). A network theory of mental disorders. World Psy-
chiatry, 16, 5–13.
Boschloo, L., van Borkulo, C. D., Borsboom, D., & Schoevers, R. A.
(2016). A prospective study on how symptoms in a network predict the
onset of depression. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 85, 183–184.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000442001
Bringmann, L. F. (2016). Dynamical networks in psychology: More than a
pretty picture? (master’s thesis). University of Groningen, Groningen,
the Netherlands.
DeBerard, M. S., & Kleinknecht, R. A. (1995). Loneliness, duration of
loneliness, and reported stress symptomatology. Psychological Reports,
76, 1363–1369. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.76.3c.1363
Dejonckheere, E., Bastian, B., Fried, E. I., Murphy, S. C., & Kuppens, P.
(2017). Perceiving social pressure not to feel negative predicts depres-
sive symptoms in daily life. Depression and Anxiety, 34, 836 – 844.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.22653
Elo, A. L., Leppänen, A., & Jahkola, A. (2003). Validity of a single-item
measure of stress symptoms. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environ-
ment & Health, 29, 444 – 451. http://dx.doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.752
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psycholog-
ical networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research
Methods, 50, 195–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1
Epskamp, S., van Borkulo, C. D., van der Veen, D. C., Servaas, M. N.,
Isvoranu, A. M., . . . Cramer, A. O. (2017). Personalized network
modeling in psychopathology: The importance of contemporaneous and
temporal connections. Clinical Psychological Science, 6, 416 – 427.
Epskamp, S., Waldorp, L. J., Mõttus, R., & Borsboom, D. (2018). The
Gaussian graphical model in cross-sectional and time-series data. Mul-
tivariate Behavioral Research. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/00273171.2018.1454823
Fried, E. I. (2017). The 52 symptoms of major depression: Lack of content
overlap among seven common depression scales. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 208, 191–197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.019
Fried, E. I., Bockting, C., Arjadi, R., Borsboom, D., Amshoff, M., Cramer,
A. O., . . . Stroebe, M. (2015). From loss to loneliness: The relationship
between bereavement and depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 124, 256 –265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000028
Fried, E. I., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2017). Moving forward: Challenges and
directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 999 –1020. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/1745691617705892
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1461SOCIAL MEDIA AND DEPRESSION SYMPTOMS
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000528.supp
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2015.34.4.277
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702617702717
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702617702717
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1140/epjds/s13688-017-0100-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000442001
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.76.3c.1363
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.22653
http://dx.doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.752
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2018.1454823
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2018.1454823
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.019
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000028
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691617705892
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691617705892
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2014). The impact of individual depressive
symptoms on impairment of psychosocial functioning. PLoS ONE, 9,
e90311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090311
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2015). Depression sum-scores don’t add up:
Why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Medi-
cine, 13, 72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12916-015-0325-4
Fried, E. I., Nesse, R. M., Guille, C., & Sen, S. (2015). The differential
influence of life stress on individual symptoms of depression. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 131, 465– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
acps.12395
Fried, E. I., van Borkulo, C. D., Cramer, A. O., Boschloo, L., Schoevers,
R. A., & Borsboom, D. (2017). Mental disorders as networks of prob-
lems: A review of recent insights. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 52, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1319-z
Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). Exploring the relationships between
different types of Facebook use, perceived online social support, and
adolescents’ depressed mood. Social Science Computer Review, 34,
153–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0894439314567449
Fruchterman, T. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Graph drawing by force-
directed placement. Software, Practice & Experience, 21, 1129 –1164.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/spe.4380211102
Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., & Danckert, J. (2011).
Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or
depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30, 647– 666.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
Gonzales, A. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my Facebook
wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 79 – 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
cyber.2009.0411
Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2016). An integrative network approach to
social anxiety disorder: The complex dynamic interplay among atten-
tional bias for threat, attentional control, and symptoms. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 42, 95–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016
.06.009
Jones, P. J., Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2017). Commentary: A network
theory of mental disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1305. http://dx
.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01305
Junco, R. (2013). Comparing actual and self-reported measures of Face-
book use. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 626 – 631. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., . . .
Ybarra, O. (2013). Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-
being in young adults. PLoS ONE, 8, e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0069841
Kwiatkowski, D., Phillips, P. C. B., Schmidt, P., & Shin, Y. (1992).
Testing the null hypothesis of stationarity against the alternative of a unit
root. Journal of Econometrics, 54, 159 –178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
0304-4076(92)90104-Y
LaRose, R. (2010). The problem of media habits. Communication Theory,
20, 194 –222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2010.01360.x
LaRose, R., Lin, C. A., & Eastin, M. S. (2003). Unregulated internet usage:
Addiction, habit, or deficient self-regulation? Media Psychology, 5,
225–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0503_01
Lee, A. R., Son, S. M., & Kim, K. K. (2016). Information and communi-
cation technology overload and social networking service fatigue: A
stress perspective. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 51– 61. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.011
Liu, D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Social networking online and per-
sonality of self-worth: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Person-
ality, 64, 79 – 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.024
Meier, A., Reinecke, L., & Meltzer, C. E. (2016). Facebocrastination?
Predictors of using Facebook for procrastination and its effects on
students’ well-being. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 65–76. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.011
Mellor, D., Stokes, M., Firth, L., Hayashi, Y., & Cummins, R. (2008).
Need for belonging, relationship satisfaction, loneliness, and life satis-
faction. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 213–218. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.020
Nutt, D., Demyttenaere, K., Janka, Z., Aarre, T., Bourin, M., Canonico,
P. L., . . . Stahl, S. (2007). The other face of depression, reduced positive
affect: The role of catecholamines in causation and cure. Journal of
Psychopharmacology, 21, 461– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269
881106069938
Pe, M. L., Kircanski, K., Thompson, R. J., Bringmann, L. F., Tuerlinckx,
F., Mestdagh, M., . . . Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Emotion-network density in
major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 292–300.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702614540645
Runyan, J. D., Steenbergh, T. A., Bainbridge, C., Daugherty, D. A., Oke,
L., & Fry, B. N. (2013). A smartphone ecological momentary assess-
ment/intervention “app” for collecting real-time data and promoting
self-awareness. PLoS ONE, 8, e71325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal
.pone.0071325
Tobin, S. J., Vanman, E. J., Verreynne, M., & Saeri, A. K. (2015). Threats
to belonging on Facebook: Lurking and ostracism. Social Influence,
4510, 1–12.
Twenge, J. M. (2017). iGen: Why today’s super-connected kids are grow-
ing up less rebellious, more tolerant, less happy—and completely un-
prepared for adulthood. New York, NY: Atria.
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., . . .
Kross, E. (2015). Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-
being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 144, 480 – 488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge
0000057
Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A
self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
ogy, 33, 1313–1330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003
.tb01951.x
Wenninger, H., Krasnova, H., & Buxmann, P. (2014, June). Activity
matters: Investigating the influence of Facebook on life satisfaction of
teenage users. Proceedings of the European Conference on Information
Systems (ECIS) 2014, Tel Aviv, Israel.
Whiting, A., & Williams, D. (2013). Why people use social media: A uses
and gratifications approach. Qualitative Market Research, 16, 362–369.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/QMR-06-2013-0041
Received March 16, 2018
Revision received September 20, 2018
Accepted September 23, 2018 �
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
1462 AALBERS, MCNALLY, HEEREN, DE WIT, AND FRIED
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090311
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12916-015-0325-4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acps.12395
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acps.12395
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1319-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0894439314567449
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/spe.4380211102
http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0411
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0411
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.06.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.06.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01305
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01305
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.007
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-4076%2892%2990104-Y
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-4076%2892%2990104-Y
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2010.01360.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0503_01
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.024
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881106069938
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881106069938
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702614540645
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071325
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071325
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/QMR-06-2013-0041
Social Media and Depression Symptoms: A Network Perspective
Method
Participants
Procedure
Materials
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics
Assumption checks
Network estimation and visualization
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Assumption Checks
Contemporaneous Network
Temporal Network
Between-Subjects Network
Discussion
Summary of Findings
Importance of Findings
Strengths and Limitations
Constraints on Generality
A Network Approach to Social Media and Psychopathology
Context of the Research
References
Journals/journal 5
An Investigation Into Audiences’ Reactions to Transgressions by
Liked and Disliked Media Figures
Mu Hu and James Young
West Virginia Wesleyan College
Jun Liang and Yuntao Guo
Anhui University
The present study investigates audiences’ parasocial relationship (PSR) reduction,
parasocial breakup (PSB), attribution of causes, and forgiveness of their liked and
disliked media figures for the figures’ transgressions. Using a 2 (media figure: liked or
disliked) � 2 (transgression severity: minor or major) between-subjects factorial
design, an experiment was conducted. There were significant main effects of media
figure and transgression severity on PSR reduction, attribution of causes, and forgive-
ness. Attribution of causes partially mediated the relationship between PSR and
forgiveness.
Public Policy Relevance Statement
Media figures’ transgressions undermine people’s liking of them. Furthermore,
people would feel more hurt by their liked media figures’ transgressions than by
their disliked figures’ transgressions. However, people were found to be more likely
to forgive their liked media figures. They tended to attribute their liked figures’
transgressions to situational factors but their disliked figures’ transgressions to
dispositional factors. Such an attribution bias partly influences the previously
mentioned relationship between liking and forgiveness.
Keywords: parasocial relationship, parasocial breakup, forgiveness, fundamental attri-
bution error
Parasocial interaction (PSI) refers to audi-
ences’ illusive interaction with media figures or
characters (Horton & Wohl, 1956). These fig-
ures and characters are called “personae.” PSI is
illusive because it “characteristically is one-
sided, nondialectical, controlled by the per-
former, and not susceptible to mutual develop-
ment” (p. 215). Despite its illusive nature,
audiences may interpret personae’s behavior as
genuine and reciprocal. For instance, audiences
sometimes feel that the personae on screen are
directly addressing them and then respond to the
personae verbally and nonverbally (Auter,
1992; Auter & Davis, 1991; Hartmann & Gold-
hoorn, 2011; Schramm & Wirth, 2010). PSI
occurs within audiences’ viewing processes,
and over time PSI can evolve into a parasocial
relationship (PSR). PSR can exist outside of
audiences’ viewing processes and influence var-
ious aspects of audiences’ life (Dibble, Hart-
mann, & Rosaen, 2015). Audiences may keep
following certain personae, actively seek news
about them, and miss them if they don’t see
them for a long time.
One of the recent advances in PSI and PSR
research is parasocial breakup (PSB) studies.
PSB is defined as audiences’ negative reactions
when their PSRs with liked personae come to an
This article was published Online First May 1, 2017.
Mu Hu and James Young, Department of Communica-
tion, West Virginia Wesleyan College; Jun Liang and Yun-
tao Guo, School of Journalism and Communication, Anhui
University.
This research was supported in part by a grant from the
Public Sentiment and Regional Image Research Center of
Anhui University, China. Preparation and revision of this
article was supported in part by a Visiting Professor’s Grant
from Anhui University, China.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Mu Hu, Department of Communication, West
Virginia Wesleyan College, 59 College Avenue, Buck-
hannon, WV 26201. E-mail: hu_m@wvwc.edu
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Psychology of Popular Media Culture © 2017 American Psychological Association
2018, Vol. 7, No. 4, 484 – 498 2160-4134/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000146
484
mailto:hu_m@wvwc.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000146
end (Cohen, 2003). Earlier PSB research fo-
cuses on involuntary PSB due to hypothetical or
real incidents when audiences’ liked personae
are taken off the air (Cohen, 2003, 2004; Eyal &
Cohen, 2006; Lather & Moyer-Guse, 2011),
whereas in the past few years researchers have
investigated audiences’ voluntary PSB caused
by personae’s transgressions (Cohen, 2010; Hu,
2016).
PSB studies have formed a well-documented
line of research, but a few weaknesses impede
its further development. First, almost all the
PSB research focuses on audiences’ favorite or
liked personae but PSB with disliked personae
is barely touched. Second, current PSB litera-
ture seldom investigates audiences’ forgiveness
of personae’s transgressions, although people
often consider the forgiveness decision after
transgressions and it has been well studied in
interpersonal relationship research. Third, few
if any studies have examined the mechanism of
audiences’ forgiveness of personae for their
transgressions.
Therefore, there are three goals of the present
study. First, it explores audiences’ PSB with
liked and disliked media figures. Second, it
compares audiences’ forgiveness of their liked
and disliked media figures’ transgressions.
Third, it investigates how fundamental attribu-
tion error (FAE) influences audiences’ reactions
to media figures’ transgressions. It needs to be
clarified that “media figures” in this study refer
to those who are direct representations of real
people (e.g., actors or actresses, sports stars,
singers, and newscasters, etc.) but not fictional
media characters (e.g., “Rachel” in Friends).
PSB and Media Figures’ Transgressions
Early PSB research primarily centers on in-
voluntary PSB. Researchers either investigate
audiences’ imagined PSB toward hypothetical
disappearance of their favorite TV personalities
(Cohen, 2003, 2004) or examine audiences’ ac-
tual PSB toward real PSR termination incidents
(Eyal & Cohen, 2006; Lather & Moyer-Guse,
2011). Later researchers have attempted to
study voluntary PSB caused by media figures’
transgressions.
Transgressions, such as former president Bill
Clinton’s sex scandal, refer to the incidents
committed by actors that violate observers’ ex-
pectations of how the actors should behave
(Thompson et al., 2005). Media figures’ trans-
gressions can erode audiences’ interest in them
and lead to PSR dissolution (Cohen, 2003; Eyal
& Cohen, 2006). Cohen (2010) compared peo-
ple’s anticipated reductions in closeness to
friends and media figures as a result of moral,
trust, and social expectancy violations. Respon-
dents reported a greater sense of closeness to
their friends than to media figures. However, for
both major and minor moral violations, antici-
pated closeness reductions were greater for
PSRs than for friendships. The researcher also
found that the closeness reductions were related
to the types of media figures. For major moral
violations, reduction in closeness was greatest
for sports figures than for other types of figures
(actors, singers, and TV hosts). For social vio-
lations, TV hosts scored highest in anticipated
reduction in closeness. The four types of figures
did not differ in closeness reduction of minor
moral violations and trust violations. Hu (2016)
found that a scandal involving a celebrity un-
dermined audiences’ PSR with him. Further-
more, audiences’ PSR with the celebrity was
positively related to PSB. However, audiences’
PSI with the celebrity in a talk show and a
character played by the celebrity in a movie was
not affected by the scandal.
Transgressions and Forgiveness
Forgiveness is the next step that people con-
sider after being hurt by transgressions (Finkel,
Rusbult, Kumashiro, & Hannon, 2002). Al-
though the empirical explorations of forgive-
ness have flourished in interpersonal communi-
cation research (Thompson et al., 2005), it
remains unknown in PSB literature why some
audiences forgive media figures for their trans-
gressions whereas some others don’t. As far as
the authors of the present study know, only one
work has been published that specifically exam-
ines forgiveness in the parasocial domain. Sand-
erson and Emmons (2014) analyzed the post-
ings regarding the baseball star Josh Hamilton’s
scandal of alcohol addiction. The researchers
discovered that some people expressed their
forgiveness and justification of Hamilton’s be-
havior. In comparison, some others did not for-
give Hamilton but blamed him for lack of will
power, trying to seek attention, and insincerity.
Despite the illuminating results of this study, it
provides limited evidence showing why people
485REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
are different in extending or withholding for-
giveness.
In order to investigate why audiences are
different in terms of their forgiveness for media
figures’ transgressions, we need to point out a
couple of characteristics of forgiveness in the
present study. First, forgiveness in this study is
regarded as a continuum but not an all-or-
nothing dichotomy. In many transgression
cases, complete forgiveness is difficult and thus
partial or conditional forgiveness is granted.
Finkel et al. (2002) proposed that although com-
plete forgiveness is the logical endpoint of the
forgiveness dimension, the magnitude of for-
giveness should be considered. Second, forgive-
ness suggests the reduction of avoidance ten-
dency. According to McCullough et al. (1998),
interpersonal forgiveness in close relationships
is reflected through the reduction of two mo-
tives: revenge and avoidance. As to revenge,
because audiences are not the direct victims of
media figures’ transgressions, it is not conceiv-
able that the figures’ transgressions would trig-
ger the audiences’ motive of revenge. Accord-
ingly, it is not conceivable either that audiences’
forgiveness would include the reduction of the
revenge motive. In comparison, people may try
to avoid perpetrators even though they are not
the direct victims. They don’t want to be asso-
ciated with the perpetrators at the risk of stig-
matization (Newman, 1987). Similarly, if audi-
ences find their PSRs unsatisfying they are free
to withdraw at any moment (Horton & Wohl,
1956). Therefore, the reduction of avoidance
motive seems to be applicable to forgiveness in
PSR. On the basis of the discussion above, we
view forgiveness in this study as the degree to
which media figures’ transgressions are ex-
cused, suggesting audiences’ inclination to con-
tinue PSRs with the figures and their unwilling-
ness to withdraw from PSRs.
PSI, PSR, and PSB With Disliked
Media Figures
Relationships are built upon not only per-
sonal preference, but also physical locations
(e.g., neighbor), work environment (e.g., col-
leagues and professional connections), and bio-
logical bonds (e.g., family). Therefore, people
sometimes have to engage in involuntary rela-
tionships with disliked others (Thibaut & Kel-
ley, 1959/1986). In order to alleviate the dis-
comfort brought by involuntary relationships,
people adopt distancing behaviors such as ex-
pressing detachment, avoiding involvement,
and showing antagonism (Hess, 2000). In con-
trast to romantic relationships and friendships,
relationships with disliked others are character-
ized by conflicts during formation and aggres-
sion and avoidance during maintenance (Card,
2007).
Given the prevalence of mass media, it is
quite possible that people are exposed to both
liked and disliked personae in media. However,
PSB research, similar to PSI and PSR research,
has predominantly revolved around audiences’
liked or favorite personae. However, research-
ers point out that not only audiences’ reactions
toward “positive” or “attractive” personae but
also their reactions to “negative” or “ugly”
personae should be studied (Hoorn & Konijn,
2003; Konijn & Hoorn, 2005). Hartmann,
Stuke, and Daschmann (2008) argued that PSR
contains cognitive components and thus PSR
research should include both positive PSRs and
hostile PSRs. Because positive PSR has been
found to be similar to friendships or romantic
relationships, negative PSRs should resemble
relationships of hatred and disgust.
In PSI literature, a number of studies have
been conducted to examine PSI with disliked
personae. Tian and Hoffner (2010) asked their
research participants to select their liked, neu-
tral, or disliked characters from the ABC drama
Lost. The researchers measured the participants’
perceived similarity, identification, PSI with the
characters, and the extent to which they had
tried to change their life to be more like the
characters. The scores of the four variables were
higher for liked and neutral characters than for
disliked characters. PSI was higher for liked
characters than for neutral characters. In com-
parison, Dibble and Rosaen (2011) focused on
audiences’ PSI experience during viewing. It
was found that audiences can have the “conver-
sational give and take” originally described by
Horton and Wohl (1956) with both liked and
disliked characters. However, audiences re-
ported stronger PSI experience with liked char-
acters than with disliked characters.
Interpersonal relationship literature has
shown that when learning of others’ transgres-
sions, people may be exposed to information
that is dissonant with their previous assump-
tions about the others, and such dissonance can
486 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
cause profound distress (Thompson et al.,
2005). This may explain why media figures’
transgressions can lead to audiences’ PSR re-
duction and PSB (Cohen, 2010; Hu, 2016).
Moreover, Hu’s study (2016) showed that audi-
ences who had a stronger PSR with a media
figure experienced higher levels of PSB caused
by the figure’s transgressions. According to
Heider (1958), people expect their liked others
to do good things and their disliked others to
perform bad actions. Therefore, in contrast to
the disliked people’s transgressions, the liked
people’s transgressions should cause more dis-
sonance. It is reasonable to predict that when
audiences learn that their liked media figures
have committed transgressions, they should ex-
perience more PSR reduction and stronger PSB.
The first two hypotheses of the present study are
as follows.
H1: In contrast to the disliked media fig-
ures’ transgressions, liked media figures’
transgressions would cause greater PSR
reduction.
H2: In contrast to the disliked media fig-
ures’ transgressions, liked media figures’
transgressions would cause greater PSB.
Finkel et al. (2002) pointed out that although
forgiveness is an individualized reaction toward
transgressions, it is influenced by two factors:
relational history and severity of the transgres-
sions (as cited in Sanderson & Emmons, 2014).
As to relational history, it has been found that
forgiveness is positively related to the level of
closeness to perpetrators (McCullough et al.,
1998). Likable offenders are perceived as more
morally responsible and less capable of future
transgressions and hence are more likely to be
forgiven (Miller & Vidmar, 1981). In compari-
son, people tend to expect more punishment for
unlikable or unattractive offenders (Tedeschi &
Felson, 1994). This difference can be explained
from the perspective of dissonance as well.
Transgressions cause dissonance between what
audiences expect the media figures to do and
what the figures actually do. When people are
distressed by dissonance, they are motivated to
seek strategies to reduce it (Festinger, 1957).
Forgiveness can be such a strategy because it
involves the motivation to continue the relation-
ships with the perpetrators (Beatty, 1970; Sand-
erson & Emmons, 2014). According to disso-
nance theory, the magnitude of dissonance is
positively related to the motivations to reduce
the dissonance (Festinger, 1957). As mentioned
earlier, liked media figures’ transgressions
cause greater dissonance in audiences. There-
fore, the audiences should be more motivated to
reduce the dissonance caused by their liked
media figures’ transgressions and thus more
willing to forgive their liked media figures. Our
third hypothesis is as follows.
H3: Audiences are more likely to forgive
liked media figures’ transgressions
than to forgive disliked media figures’
transgressions.
As to transgression severity, it refers to the
degree to which transgression consequences are
negative and enduring. More severe transgres-
sions have more profound and irreversible con-
sequences and thus are more difficult to forgive
(Bennett & Earwaker, 1994; Girard & Mullet,
1997; Newman, 1987). Hu (2016) doubted the
previous PSB research findings based on a sin-
gle transgression incident, and Cohen’s (2010)
study shows that audiences’ reactions to media
figures’ transgressions vary by transgression se-
verity. Interpersonal dissolution literature may
shed some light on revealing the relationship
between severity and forgiveness in PSRs.
Boon and Sulsky (1997) and Girard and Mullet
(1997) examined their respondents’ reactions
toward a series of transgression scenarios, and
both studies found an inverse relationship be-
tween forgiveness and transgression severity. In
contrast, Finkel et al. (2002), McCullough et al.
(1998), and McCullough, Fincham, and Tsang
(2003) asked people to recall real life experi-
ences of being hurt by transgressions and found
that transgression severity was negatively asso-
ciated with the extent to which participants for-
gave the perpetrators. Based on these research
findings, we propose that—
H4: The severity of a media figure’s trans-
gression is negatively related to audiences’
forgiveness of the media figure.
Attribution of Transgressions Causes and FAE
People constantly try to find out the causes of
others’ behavior because of their innate desire
to understand and control the external world
(Heider, 1958). This process is called attribu-
487REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
tion. Attribution plays an important role in de-
termining how they react to others’ behavior
(Kelley & Michela, 1980). During the attribu-
tion process, an observer focuses on either the
internal factors (e.g., an actor’s dispositions or
personality) or external factors (e.g., environ-
ment or situation, Reeder, 1982).
Although attribution theories demonstrate
that human beings are logical creatures, the
process of attribution can sometimes be biased.
One of the common attribution biases is FAE.
FAE refers to people’s tendency to overestimate
the influence of internal factors while underes-
timating the influence of external factors during
attribution processes (Amabile, Ross, & Stein-
metz, 1977; Jones & Harris, 1967; Ross, 1977).
FAE can occur for a couple of reasons. First,
people are cognitive misers and they tend to
seek quick and easy answers to the causes of
others’ behavior. Therefore, they pay more at-
tention to the more stable and easily accessible
information such as an actor’s dispositions,
rather than the more fragmented and less obvi-
ous information such as situations (Forgas,
1998). Second, people may lack necessary
knowledge of situations (Gilbert & Malone,
1995). An observer may know very little about
the context of an actor’s behavior and thus rely
primarily on the actor’s dispositional factors to
explain the behavior.
Observers’ liking of an actor influences
whether they attribute the actor’s behavior to
internal factors or external factors (Kelley &
Michela, 1980). When observers’ liked people
do good things and disliked people do bad
things, the observers treat the actions as typical
actions for the actors and thus make internal
attributions. In contrast, when there is a “mis-
match” between the actor’s actions and the ob-
servers’ expectations, the observers tend to be-
lieve that such actions are forced by the
situations and thus make external attributions
(Regan, Strauss, & Fazio, 1974).
Research has shown that people’s attribution
of media figures’ behaviors is similar to their
attribution of their real social relations’ behav-
iors (Perse & Rubin, 1989; Tal-Or & Papirman,
2007). Given the above-mentioned two causes
of FAE, audiences’ attributions of media fig-
ures’ transgressions may be particularly suscep-
tible to FAE. First, usually PSRs are less strong
than interpersonal relationships because people
invest less in PSRs (Branch, Wilson, & Agnew,
2013; Cohen, 2010). Therefore, people are less
motivated to make efforts in finding out media
figures’ transgression causes. Lack of such a
motivation restricts audiences from actively
collecting and analyzing the complex situa-
tional cues surrounding the transgressions.
Rather, they tend to take a cognitive “shortcut”
by primarily focusing on the relatively stable
and easily comprehensible information, such as
the transgressors’ dispositional traits and the
previous knowledge of the transgressors. Sec-
ond, in real social relationships, people have
opportunities to ask transgressors or other wit-
nesses about situational information. However,
in PSRs, audiences usually rely on media re-
ports to obtain the situational cues of media
figures’ transgressions. PSR’s one-sided nature
prevents people from obtaining further situa-
tional information of a particular transgression.
Consequently, they tend to rely on what they
already know about the media figures, usually
the internal and stable factors (e.g., personali-
ties, dispositions, and behavioral patterns), to
make inferences about the causes of transgres-
sions. Therefore, we propose the following hy-
pothesis.
H5: Audiences tend to attribute their liked
media figures’ transgressions to external
factors while their disliked media figures’
transgressions to internal factors.
Attribution may further influence audiences’
emotional and behavioral reactions toward me-
dia figures’ transgressions (Kepplinger, Geiss,
& Siebert, 2012). In the above-mentioned Sand-
erson and Emmons’s (2014) study, those who
granted forgiveness made external attributions
and defended the transgressor, whereas those
who withheld forgiveness made internal attribu-
tions and called for punishment. Forgiveness, as
an important reaction to transgressions, some-
times is granted because of positive relational
history (e.g., liking) and less blameful interpre-
tations of transgressions (Finkel et al., 2002).
Finkel and colleagues’ (2002) research shows
that the cognitive interpretation of betrayals
partially mediates the relationship between re-
lational commitment and interpersonal forgive-
ness. If people’s liking of transgressors is high,
they are more motivated to attend to the envi-
ronments and come up with interpretations that
are more favorable to the transgressors. These
488 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
interpretations in turn facilitate their willingness
to grant forgiveness. Therefore, our sixth hy-
pothesis of the present study is as follows.
H6: Audiences’ attribution of the causes
(internal vs. external) of media figures’
transgressions mediates the relationship
between their PSRs with the figures and
their forgiveness of the figures.
Method
Sample
A total of 137 participants were recruited
from a Midwestern liberal arts college for this
study. The sample ranged in age from 17 to 41
(M � 19.70, SD � 2.52). Thirty-one percent
were male (N � 42) and 68% were female (N �
93). Two participants did not disclose their gen-
der information. Eighty percent of the partici-
pants were Caucasian (N � 80), 9% were Af-
rican Americans (N � 12), .7% were Latin
Americans (N � 1), 2% were Asian Americans
(N � 3), and 7% (N � 10) of the participants
identified their ethnicity as “others.” Two par-
ticipants did not reveal their ethnicity informa-
tion.
Procedure
A 2 (media figure: liked or disliked) � 2
(transgression severity: minor or major) be-
tween-subjects factorial design was employed
in the present study. We chose George Clooney
as the liked media figure and Charlie Sheen as
the disliked media figure. George Clooney has
been rated as one of the top five celebrities with
the best reputations (“Jennifer Lawrence has the
best celebrity reputation, just as we suspected,”
2014). We compiled a biographical profile of
Clooney which highlighted the awards he had
received, the charity activities he had organized,
and the funds he had launched for the refugees
of international terrorism. In comparison, Char-
lie Sheen, despite the success of his perfor-
mance career, has long been suffering from
scandals. We also compiled a profile of Charlie
Sheen but it focused on his imprudent private
life characterized by drug abuse, physical vio-
lence, damaging public properties, and sex
scandals.
We tested our hypotheses with two newspa-
per articles. These two articles reported two
transgressions of minor and major severity re-
spectively. The participants were told that the
news articles were published in The New York
Times but they were actually written by the
researchers. The participants were also told that
the transgressions occurred just at the night
before they participated in the study. Each arti-
cle was printed on a page with The New York
Times logo on its top margin, which looked like
a news page from The New York Times website.
In the minor transgression story, the actor who
was in a hurry (either Clooney or Sheen, de-
pending on the conditions) opened a door in a
bar without looking back and the door hit a
person behind him in the face. This incident left
the victim a few bruises. In the major transgres-
sion story, the actor cut into the front of the line
to enter a bar. When a man in the line com-
plained, the actor struck him in the face. The
victim was sent to Ronald Reagan UCLA Med-
ical Center.
The participants were randomly assigned to
four conditions. In Condition 1 (liked figure and
minor transgression news, N � 33) and Condi-
tion 2 (liked figure and major transgression
news, N � 32), the participants first read Cloo-
ney’s profile, and filled out a questionnaire test-
ing their PSR with the actor as a pretest. Then
the participants in these two conditions read the
minor and major transgression news articles,
respectively. After reading the articles, the par-
ticipants were instructed to complete another
questionnaire including a question asking how
serious the participants thought the transgres-
sion was, a posttest PSR Scale, a PSB Scale
measuring their reactions to the transgression
news, a scale of attribution of causes, and a
scale of forgiveness. In Condition 3 (disliked
figure and minor transgression news, N � 36)
and Condition 4 (disliked figure and major
transgression news, N � 36), the procedure was
the same except that the participants read the
profile of Charlie Sheen and the transgressor in
the news was Charlie Sheen.
Measures
In addition to such demographic variables as
age, gender, and ethnicity, PSR, severity of
transgressions, PSB, attribution of causes of
transgressions, and forgiveness were measured.
PSR. Both the pretest and the posttest of
PSR were measured with the 10-item PSI Scale
489REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
devised by Rubin and Perse (1987). Over de-
cades this scale has been adapted to measure
PSR with a wide range of personae and consis-
tently proved to be valid and reliable. We
slightly modified the wording of the scale to
make it better fit the present study. The PSR
measure used in this study included such items
as “I look forward to seeing him on TV or
movies,” “I see him as a natural, down-to-earth
person,” and “He makes me feel comfortable, as
if I am with a friend.” The items were measured
on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 � strongly
disagree, 5 � strongly agree). The Cronbach’s
alpha of both the pretest scale and the posttest
scale was .90.
Severity. Severity was measured with a
question asking the participants how severe the
incidents were. This question was measured on
a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 � not severe at
all, 5 � very severe).
PSB. PSB was measured with 5 items se-
lected from the 13-item PSB Scale devised by
Cohen (2003). The original scale was used to
measure audiences’ reactions to involuntary
PSB (e.g., favorite characters were off the air),
whereas we intended to measure audiences’ vol-
untary PSB after reading the transgression
news. Therefore, we selected 5 items that could
be applied to this study. The items measured
how angry, sad, disappointed, betrayed, and
lonely the respondents felt when they read the
transgression news. The items were measured
on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 � strongly
disagree, 5 � strongly agree). The scale’s
Cronbach’s alpha was .78.
Attribution of causes. Attribution of
causes was measured with the locus of causality
subscale of the Causal Dimension Scale devised
by Russell (1982). This subscale measures
whether a certain behavior is attributable to
external or internal factors. It was composed of
three items asking whether the cause of the
incident is the situation or the actor himself,
outside of the actor or inside of the actor, and
something about himself or something about
others (reversely coded). The items were mea-
sured on a 7-point semantic differential scale.
Higher scores suggested higher tendency to at-
tribute the incident to internal factors. The
scale’s Cronbach’s alpha was .72.
Forgiveness. A PSR is different from a real
interpersonal relationship because it is an illu-
sive, one directional, and nondialectical rela-
tionship. Therefore, those widely used forgive-
ness scales developed from interpersonal
relationship research cannot be directly used in
the current study (e.g., McCullough et al.’s
Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motiva-
tions Inventory or TRIM). In this study, forgive-
ness was viewed as the degree to which media
figures’ transgressions are excused, reflecting
audiences’ willingness to continue PSRs and
unwillingness of avoidance. Therefore, we used
three items to measure forgiveness, “I would
forgive this actor,” “I would remain loyal to this
actor,” and “I would not follow this actor any-
more” (reversely coded). The items were mea-
sured on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 �
strongly disagree, 5 � strongly agree). The
Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was .77.
Results
Manipulation Check
Before testing the hypotheses, we conducted
manipulation checks to test the validity of the
experimental manipulations. First, we per-
formed a t test to compare the means of PSR
pretests with the liked actor (Condition 1 and 2
in which the participants read the profile of
George Clooney) and the disliked actor (Con-
dition 3 and 4 in which the participants read the
profile of Charlie Sheen). The pretest of PSR
with the liked actor (M � 3.31, SD � .64) was
significantly higher than the pretest of PSR with
the disliked actor (M � 2.13, SD � .61),
t(135) � 11.05, p � .001. The effect size for
this analysis (d � 1.89) was found to exceed
Cohen’s (1988) convention for a large effect
(d � .80). Therefore, our manipulation of liking
was successful. Second, we conducted another t
test to compare the means of severity of the
minor and major transgressions. The severity
score in the major transgression (M � 3.19,
SD � .55) was significantly higher than that in
the minor transgression (M � 2.24, SD � .52),
t(1,133) � 5.66, p � .001. The effect size for
this analysis (d � 1.78) was also found to
exceed Cohen’s (1988) convention for a large
effect (d � .80). Therefore, our manipulation of
minor and major transgressions was also suc-
cessful.
490 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Gender Difference
Both the liked and disliked media figures in
the present study were male, so it is necessary to
examine whether there are any gender differ-
ence between male and female participants in
terms of their pretest of PSR, posttest of PSR,
PSB, attribution of causes, and forgiveness. We
compared the means of these variables in the
liked actor conditions (Condition 1 and 2) and
disliked actor conditions (Condition 3 and 4)
respectively. In the liked actor conditions, there
were no significant gender differences on these
variables. In the disliked actor conditions, fe-
males’ pretest and posttest PSR scores were
significantly lower than males’ scores. These
results suggest that the female respondents’
PSRs with Charlie Sheen were weaker than the
male respondents’ PSRs both before and after
they learned of his transgression stories. There
were no gender differences on attribution, PSB,
and forgiveness (see Table 1 for details).
Hypotheses 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5
In order to test the first five hypotheses, a 2 �
2 multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
factorial model was employed in the present
study with media figure (liked or disliked) and
transgression severity (minor or major) as the
independent variables and PSR reduction, PSB,
forgiveness, and attribution of causes as the
dependent variables. PSR reduction score was
the result of the PSR pretest score minus the
PSR posttest score, so higher scores represented
more reduction of PSR. We ran a series of t tests
comparing the PSR pretests and posttests in the
four conditions respectively. As is shown in
Table 2, for the liked actor (Condition 1 and
Condition 2), the PSR posttest scores were sig-
nificantly lower than the pretest scores in both
minor and major transgression conditions. In
contrast, for the disliked actor (Condition 3 and
Condition 4), the posttest score was signifi-
cantly lower than the pretest score only in the
major transgression condition.
The results of MANOVA revealed significant
multivariate effects for media figure, Wilks’
� � .63, F(4, 128) � 18.91, p � .001, and
transgression severity, Wilks’ � � .66, F(4,
128) � 16.40, p � .001. There was also a
significant interaction effect, Wilks’ � � .90,
F(4, 128) � 1.91, p � .01.
In examining the multivariate effect for me-
dia figure, three significant univariate tests were
observed, including PSR reduction, forgiveness,
and attribution of causes. PSR reduction in the
liked media figure conditions was significantly
higher than that in the disliked media figure
conditions. Forgiveness in the liked media fig-
ure conditions was significantly higher than that
in the disliked media figure conditions. As to
attribution of causes, the attribution scores in
the disliked media figure conditions were sig-
nificantly higher than the scores in the liked
media figure conditions, suggesting that the dis-
liked media figure’s transgressions were more
likely than liked media figures’ transgressions
to be attributed to internal factors. Therefore,
our first, third, and fifth hypothesis was sup-
ported. However, the univariate test for PSB
was not significant, so there was no difference
between PSB toward the liked media figure’s
Table 1
Mean Comparisons of PSR Pretest, PSR Posttest, Attribution of Causes, PSB, and Forgiveness Between
Female and Male Participants in the Liked Actor Conditions and the Disliked Actor Conditions
Variable
Liked actor
conditions
t-values and p-values
Disliked actor
conditions
t-values and p-valuesFemale Male Female Male
PSR pretest 3.29 3.35 t(63) � �.40, p � .69 2.04 2.41 t(68) � �2.35, p � .05
PSR posttest 3.07 3.02 t(63) � .29, p � .78 1.97 2.34 t(68) � �2.15, p � .05
Attribution of causes 4.01 3.89 t(63) � .35, p � .73 4.33 4.77 t(68) � �1.05, p � .30
PSB 2.17 2.26 t(63) � �.43, p � .67 2.13 2.19 t(68) � �.32, p � .75
Forgiveness 3.65 3.58 t(63) � .41, p � .68 2.68 3.07 t(68) � �1.76, p � .08
Note. PSR � parasocial relationship; PSB � parasocial breakup. PSR pretest, PSR posttest, PSB, and forgiveness scale
items were measured on a 5-point Likert scale (1 � strongly disagree, 5 � strongly agree). Attribution of causes scale items
were measured on a 7-point semantic differential scale (higher scores suggested higher tendency to attribute the incident to
the internal factors).
491REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
and disliked media figure’s transgressions.
Therefore, the first, third, and fifth hypotheses
were supported but the second hypothesis was
not. The results of these analyses are reported in
Table 3.
In examining the multivariate effect for trans-
gression severity, four significant univariate
tests were observed, including PSR reduction,
PSB, forgiveness, and attribution of causes.
PSR reduction in the minor transgression con-
ditions was significantly lower than that in the
major transgression conditions. PSB in the mi-
nor transgression conditions were significantly
lower than the major transgression conditions.
Forgiveness in the minor transgression condi-
tions was significantly higher than that in the
major transgression conditions. Therefore, our
fourth hypothesis regarding forgiveness and se-
verity of transgression was supported. As to
attribution of causes, attribution scores in the
minor transgression conditions were signifi-
cantly lower than those in the major transgres-
sion conditions. The results of these analyses
are reported in Table 4.
In examining the multivariate interaction ef-
fect, the univariate test for PSB was significant,
F(1, 131) � 4.30, p � .05. The other three
univariate tests were not significant, PSR reduc-
tion, F(1, 131) � 3.75, p � .06, forgiveness,
F(1, 131) � 2.56, p � .11, and attribution of
causes, F(1, 131) � 3.21, p � .08.
Hypothesis 6
In the sixth hypothesis, we anticipated that
attribution of causes would mediate the rela-
tionship between PSR and forgiveness. In order
to test this mediating effect, we followed Baron
and Kenny’s procedure (1986) and ran a series
of regression models. In the first step, we ex-
amined whether the independent variable (PSR
pretest) had a direct effect on the dependent
variable (forgiveness). The effect was con-
firmed because PSR pretest was significantly
related to forgiveness, � � .61, p � .001. In the
second step, we tested whether the independent
variable (PSR pretest) had an effect on the me-
diating variable (attribution). The relationship
was also confirmed, � � �.19, p � .05. In the
third step, forgiveness (dependent variable) was
set as the criterion variable and both PSR pretest
(independent variable) and attribution (mediat-
ing variable) were used as predictors in a re-
gression equation. Attribution had a significant
effect on forgiveness after PSR pretest was con-
trolled, � � �.45, p � .001, and the equation of
regressing forgiveness onto both attribution and
PSR reached significance, F(2, 134) � 83.23,
p � .001, R2 � .56. In the fourth step, the effect
of PSR pretest on forgiveness controlling for
attribution was examined. After the mediating
variable (attribution) was controlled, the size of
the direct effect identified in the first step (be-
tween PSR pretest and forgiveness) was re-
duced from � � .61 to �’ � .52, suggesting that
attribution partially mediated the relationship
Table 2
Mean Comparisons of PSR Pretest and Posttest
by Condition
Condition
PSR
pretest
PSR
posttest t-values and p-values
Condition 1 3.26 3.18 t(32) � 2.12, p � .05
Condition 2 3.36 2.92 t(31) � 4.76, p � .001
Condition 3 2.19 2.18 t(34) � .13, p � .90
Condition 4 2.09 1.96 t(34) � 2.19, p � .05
Note. PSR � parasocial relationship. Both PSR pretest
and PSR posttest scale items were measured on a 5-point
Likert scale (1 � strongly disagree, 5 � strongly agree).
Table 3
Univariate Tests of PSR Reduction, PSB, Forgiveness, and Attribution of Causes
of Transgressions Between the Liked Media Figure and the Disliked
Media Figure
Variable
Liked
media figure
Disliked
media figure F values and p values
PSR reduction .26 .06 F(1, 131) � 10.15, p � .001
PSB 2.21 2.15 F(1, 131) � .25, p � .62
Forgiveness 3.62 2.79 F(1, 131) � 51.16, p � .001
Attribution of causes 3.98 4.45 F(1, 131) � 4.86, p � .05
Note. PSR � parasocial relationship; PSB � parasocial breakup.
492 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
between PSR pretest and forgiveness (see Fig-
ure 1). Therefore, the sixth hypothesis was par-
tially supported.
Discussion
The present study’s contribution is reflected
in the following aspects. First, it extends the
current literature by comparing audiences’ re-
actions to their liked media figures’ and disliked
media figures’ transgressions. In contrast to a
disliked figure’s transgressions, audiences’
liked figure’s transgressions caused greater PSR
reduction. Second, it explores audiences’ for-
giveness and fills the void of research on the
reactions following PSR reduction and PSB.
Although a liked media figure’s transgressions
resulted in greater PSR reduction, audiences
were more likely to forgive him. Third, it ex-
amines the mechanism of audiences’ forgive-
ness of media figures for their transgressions. It
discovers the influence of FAE on audiences’
attribution of transgression causes and their for-
giveness of the media figures. The audiences
attributed the liked actor’s transgressions to ex-
ternal factors but the disliked actor’s transgres-
sions to internal factors. Such an attribution
partially mediated the relationship between peo-
ple’s PSR and their forgiveness. These results
are similar to the findings of interpersonal rela-
tionship studies, suggesting that the conse-
quences of the dissolution process in a PSR
resemble those in an interpersonal relationship.
This resemblance further implies that media are
a core element in people’s life. Mediated expe-
rience and real experience are so interwoven
with each other that the boundaries between the
two experiences are blurring. Although PSRs
are not as strong as interpersonal relationships
(Cohen, 2010), media frequently and routinely
bring media figures into people’s daily life and
mold these figures into the substance of their
living experiences (Piccirillo, 1986).
It is intriguing that our first hypothesis on
PSR reduction was supported but the second
hypothesis regarding PSB was not. This may be
due to the fact that we assigned two media
figures to the participants rather than let the
participants choose their own liked and disliked
figures. This approach influenced people’s PSB
scores with the liked actor in particular. The
mean score of the PSR pretest with Clooney
(Condition 1 and Condition 2) was 3.31 on a
5-point scale, suggesting that the audiences’
PSR with him might not be as strong as their
PSRs with the liked personae of their choosing.
Table 4
Univariate Tests of PSR Reduction, PSB, Forgiveness, and Attribution of Causes
of Transgressions by Transgression Severity
Variable Minor Major F values and p values
PSR reduction .04 .29 F(1, 135) � 15.64, p � .001
PSB 1.99 2.37 F(1, 135) � 9.76, p � .01
Forgiveness 3.53 2.87 F(1, 135) � 31.90, p � .001
Attribution of causes 3.51 4.93 F(1, 135) � 44.18, p � .001
Note. PSR � parasocial relationship; PSB � parasocial breakup.
β = -.19 β = -.45
β = .61, β’ = .52 (after attribution was controlled)
Attribution
PSR Forgiveness
Figure 1. Mediating effect of attribution on the relationship between PSR and forgiveness.
� � .61, �’ � .52 (after attribution was controlled).
493REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
However, PSB measured pretty strong feelings
such as anger, sadness, disappointment, sense of
being betrayed, and loneliness. Therefore, Cloo-
ney’s transgressions, although caused signifi-
cant PSR reduction, might not arouse such
strong PSB that could be differentiated from the
PSB caused by Sheen’s transgressions.
This study also revealed the effects of trans-
gression severity. The main effects of the trans-
gression severity (minor and major) on PSR
reduction, PSB, attribution, and forgiveness in-
dicate that increasing severity of transgressions
would cause greater PSR reduction, higher
PSB, more likeliness of internal attribution, and
less forgiveness. This was consistent with the
above-reviewed research examining severity of
transgression in interpersonal relationships and
PSRs. It is worth noting that for the liked media
figure, there was significant reduction of PSR in
both minor and major transgressions, whereas
for the disliked media figure only major trans-
gression caused significant reduction of PSR.
These results may confirm our proposition that
transgressions yield dissonance in audiences.
As to their liked figures, people have high ex-
pectations of what the figures ought to do, so
when they learn of their even minor transgres-
sions, they would experience some dissonance.
In contrast, people’s expectations of disliked
figures may be low due to the figures’ negative
behavioral patterns in the past. Therefore, their
minor transgression may not cause much disso-
nance in the audiences.
Another illuminating finding of this study is
the partial mediating effect of attribution on the
relationship between PSR and forgiveness. This
finding indicates that people’s forgiveness is not
completely explained by relational history.
Rather, how they interpret the transgressions,
especially in terms of attribution of causes, in-
fluences the relationship between PSR and for-
giveness. This finding is consistent with Finkel
et al.’s (2002) research on forgiveness of be-
trayal in close relationships, which shows that
the association between relational commitment
and forgiveness was partially mediated by attri-
bution of causes.
Despite the contribution, however, the results
should be interpreted with caution given the
limitations of this study. First, the media figures
we used in the present study were actors. How-
ever, there are other types of media figures, such
as politicians, athletes, religious leaders, news-
casters, and so forth. People’s expectations of me-
dia figures’ behavior may vary by figure type. For
example, people may expect a religious leader to
be more prudent than a pop star in his lifestyle.
Therefore, a lifestyle transgression committed by
a religious leader may be perceived as more se-
vere and harder to forgive than such a transgres-
sion by a pop star. Future researchers need to
explore how media figure type influences audi-
ences’ reactions toward media figures’ transgres-
sions.
Second, people often make apologies after
transgressions, and media figures often use
apologies as a strategy of crisis management.
Interpersonal forgiveness literature shows that
apology plays an important role in the process
of forgiveness (Baumeister, Exline, & Sommer,
1999; McCullough et al., 1998). Apology is a
constructive step to repair media figures’ public
images and restore their relationships with au-
diences. Audiences’ reactions to media figures’
transgression are influenced by the figures’ at-
titudes to the transgressions and their apology
strategies (Sanderson & Emmons, 2014). How-
ever, we did not take apology into consideration
in this study, and future parasocial studies
should further explore this topic.
Third, in the majority of PSI or PSR studies,
the research participants are asked to name their
favorite or liked media figure either in general
or from a certain TV program, whereas in the
present study the participants didn’t get to
choose their own media figures. Rather, in each
condition, the researchers assigned a media fig-
ure to them and then asked them to report their
reactions toward a transgression committed by
the figure. Therefore, the transgression stories
may not have resulted in stronger feelings.
However, it would be difficult to test the influ-
ence of transgressions on people’s reactions to-
ward a media figure of their choosing. It is
unknown when people’s favorite media figures
would commit transgressions and thus it is chal-
lenging to capture the PSR reduction caused by
the transgressions. If we employ the present
study’s approach of using concocted news sto-
ries, people may not believe the stories because
people are probably very familiar with the me-
dia figures of their choosing and know about the
most recent news about those figures. If we use
hypothetical transgression stories and ask peo-
ple to imagine their reactions, this approach
compromises response accuracy because there
494 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
may be differences between what people predict
they would do and what they actually would do.
Fourth, each participant in this study only
read a single news report of an actor’s trans-
gression. In addition, the participants were
asked to report their reactions immediately after
reading the news report. However, in real life a
media figure’s transgression may be reported by
a series of news in sequence. Sometimes the
news reported later is different from or even
contradictory to the news released earlier. Be-
cause audiences keep learning about new pieces
of the transgression over time (e.g., O. J. Simp-
son’s case, Clinton-Lewinsky scandal, etc.),
their reactions to media figures’ transgressions
may fluctuate over time as well. Finkel et al.’s
study (2002) shows that people’s immediate
reactions to transgressions are more negative
than their delayed reactions. Therefore, the dif-
ference between how audiences were exposed
to transgression news in this study and how they
are exposed to such news in real life threatens
the external validity of the present study. Future
research should study the process of audiences’
sequential reactions to media figures’ transgres-
sions.
Fifth, during the time when this study was
conducted, Charlie Sheen’s HIV-positive status
was disclosed by news media. Although this
was not mentioned in Charlie Sheen’s profile in
the present study, the participants were proba-
bly aware of the news. Research has shown that
HIV-infected people risk discrimination and
stigmatization (Kelso et al., 2014; Liamputtong,
2013). Therefore, Charlie Sheen’s HIV-positive
status news may taint the way some participants
viewed this study’s fictional news stories about
him because they might already have prejudice
against the disease.
Sixth, although this study focuses on media
figures’ transgressions, audiences’ reactions to-
ward fictional media characters should not be
ignored. In Cohen’s survey (2003), when asked
to name their favorite personae, nearly two
thirds of the respondents chose hosts of news,
current events, or talk shows. Meanwhile, over
one third of them chose fictional characters
from TV series or movies. Despite the fictional
nature of the characters, audiences may develop
very intimate PSRs with them. As mentioned
above in Tal-Or and Papirman’s (2007) study,
fictional characters’ traits were so compelling
that audiences believed that the actors who play
the characters actually possessed such traits.
Not only the incidents that occur on media
figures but also those that happen to fictional
characters can cause strong and profound reac-
tions from audiences. Eyal and Cohen (2006)
pointed out that successful TV series and movie
sequels can last years even decades. If audi-
ences keep following these TV series and movie
sequels, they can “keep in touch” with the char-
acters and hence develop long-term PSRs. The
length of relationships predicts the distress over
relationship dissolution (Simpson, 1987), so the
long-term audiences are particularly vulnerable
to the impact of PSR dissolution with the char-
acters (Eyal & Cohen, 2006; Lather & Moyer-
Guse, 2011). Therefore, it is worth examining
these audiences’ reactions toward fictional me-
dia characters’ transgressions.
Seventh, we used five items from Cohen’s
PSB Scale (2003) to measure PSB in this study.
This PSB Scale was originally used to measure
people’s negative reactions toward the termina-
tion of PSRs with their favorite TV personali-
ties. However, we used the same five items from
the same scale to measure PSB toward both
liked media figures and disliked media figures.
In future research involving PSB toward liked
and disliked media figures’ transgressions, re-
searchers should go beyond simply comparing
the extent of PSB. Rather, they should further
explore the nature and components of PSB to-
ward the liked media figures and disliked media
figures respectively. For example, is it possible
that PSB toward a liked figure’s transgression
includes more surprise, disappointment, and re-
gret while a disliked figure’s transgression may
cause more anger, disgust, and even gloat in-
stead? If so, whether is it the difference of the
nature and the components of PSB that influ-
ences audiences’ forgiveness of their liked and
disliked media figures’ transgressions?
References
Amabile, T. M., Ross, L., & Steinmetz, J. L. (1977).
Social roles, social control, and biases in social-
perception processes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 35, 485– 494. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/0022-3514.35.7.485
Auter, P. J. (1992). TV that talks back: An experimental
validation of a Parasocial Interaction Scale. Journal of
Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 36, 173–181.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159209364165
495REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.7.485
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.7.485
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159209364165
Auter, P. J., & Davis, D. M. (1991). When characters
speak directly to viewers: Breaking the fourth wall in
television. The Journalism Quarterly, 68, 165–171.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/107769909106800117
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The modera-
tor-mediator variable distinction in social psycho-
logical research: Conceptual, strategic, and statis-
tical considerations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Baumeister, R. F., Exline, J. J., & Sommer, K. L.
(1999). The victim role, grudge theory, and two
dimensions of forgiveness. In E. L. Worthington,
Jr. (Ed.), Dimensions of forgiveness: Psychologi-
cal research and theological perspectives (pp. 79–
104). Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation
Press.
Beatty, J. (1970). Forgiveness. American Philosoph-
ical Quarterly, 7, 246–252.
Bennett, M., & Earwaker, D. (1994). Victims’ re-
sponses to apologies: The effects of offender re-
sponsibility and transgression severity. Journal of
Social Psychology, 134, 457– 464.
Boon, S. D., & Sulsky, L. M. (1997). Attributions of
blame and forgiveness: A policy-capturing study.
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12,
19– 44.
Branch, S. E., Wilson, K. M., & Agnew, C. R.
(2013). Committed to Oprah, Homer, or House:
Using the Investment Model to understand paraso-
cial relationships. Psychology of Popular Media
Culture, 2, 96–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0030938
Card, N. A. (2007). ‘I hated her guts!’: Emerging
adults’ recollections of the formation, mainte-
nance, and termination of antipathetic relation-
ships during high school. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 22, 32–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0743558406295783
Cohen, E. (2010). Expectancy violations in relation-
ships with friends and media figures. Communica-
tion Research Reports, 27, 97–111. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/08824091003737836
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
baum.
Cohen, J. (2003). Parasocial breakups: Measuring
individual differences in responses to the dissolu-
tion of parasocial relationships. Mass Communica-
tion and Society, 6, 191–202. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1207/S15327825MCS0602_5
Cohen, J. (2004). Parasocial breakup from favorite
television characters: The role of attachment styles
and relationship intensity. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 21, 187–202. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0265407504041374
Dibble, J. L., Hartmann, T., & Rosaen, S. F. (2015).
Parasocial interaction and parasocial relationship:
Conceptual clarification and a critical assessment
of measures. Human Communication Research,
42, 21– 44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12063
Dibble, J. R., & Rosaen, S. F. (2011). Parasocial
interaction as more than friendship: Evidence for
parasocial interaction with disliked media figures.
Journal of Media Psychology, 23, 122–132. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000044
Eyal, K., & Cohen, J. (2006). When good friends say
goodbye: A parasocial breakup study. Journal of
Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 50, 502–523.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem5003_9
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive disso-
nance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Finkel, E. J., Rusbult, C. E., Kumashiro, M., & Han-
non, P. A. (2002). Dealing with betrayal in close
relationships: Does commitment promote forgive-
ness? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 82, 956–974. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.82.6.956
Forgas, J. P. (1998). On being happy and mistaken:
Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
318–331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75
.2.318
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The corre-
spondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–
38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21
Girard, M., & Mullet, E. (1997). Propensity to for-
give in adolescents, young adults, older adults, and
elderly people. Journal of Adult Development, 4,
209–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02511412
Hartmann, T., & Goldhoorn, C. (2011). Horton and
Wohl revisited: Exploring viewers’ experience of
parasocial interaction. Journal of Communication,
61, 1104–1121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-
2466.2011.01595.x
Hartmann, T., Stuke, D., & Daschmann, G. (2008).
Positive parasocial relationships with drivers affect
suspense in racing sports spectators. Journal of
Media Psychology, 20, 24–34. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1027/1864-1105.20.1.24
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal
relations. New York, NY: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/10628-000
Hess, J. A. (2000). Maintaining nonvoluntary rela-
tionships with disliked partners: An investigation
into the use of distancing behaviors. Human Com-
munication Research, 26, 458– 488. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00765.x
Hoorn, J. F., & Konijn, E. A. (2003). Perceiving and
experiencing fictional characters: An integrative
account. The Japanese Psychological Research,
45, 250–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-5884
.00225
Horton, D., & Wohl, R. R. (1956). Mass communication
and para-social interaction: Observations on intimacy at
496 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/107769909106800117
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030938
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030938
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0743558406295783
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0743558406295783
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824091003737836
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08824091003737836
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327825MCS0602_5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327825MCS0602_5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407504041374
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407504041374
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12063
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000044
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000044
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem5003_9
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.956
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.956
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.318
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.318
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02511412
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01595.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01595.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105.20.1.24
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105.20.1.24
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00765.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00765.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-5884.00225
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-5884.00225
a distance. Psychiatry, 19, 215–229. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/00332747.1956.11023049
Hu, M. (2016). The influence of a scandal on para-
social relationship, parasocial interaction, and
parasocial breakup. Psychology of Popular Media
Culture, 5, 217–231.
“Jennifer Lawrence has the best celebrity reputation,
just as we suspected.” (2014). Retrieved from http://
www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/02/12/jennifer-
lawrence-celebrity-reputation_n_4768307.html
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution
of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
chology, 3, 124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-
1031(67)90034-0
Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution
theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology,
31, 457–501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps
.31.020180.002325
Kelso, G. A., Cohen, M. H., Weber, K. M., Dale,
S. K., Cruise, R. C., & Brody, L. R. (2014). Crit-
ical consciousness, racial and gender discrimina-
tion, and HIV disease markers in African Ameri-
can women with HIV. AIDS and Behavior, 18,
1237–1246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10461-013-
0621-y
Kepplinger, H. M., Geiss, S., & Siebert, S. (2012).
Framing scandals: Cognitive and emotional me-
dia effects. Journal of Communication, 62, 659–
681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012
.01653.x
Konijn, E. A., & Hoorn, J. F. (2005). Some like it bad:
Testing a model for perceiving and experiencing
fictional characters. Media Psychology, 7, 107–144.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0702_1
Lather, J., & Moyer-Guse, E. (2011). How do we
react when our favorite characters are taken away?
An examination of temporary parasocial breakup.
Mass Communication and Society, 14, 196–215.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15205431003668603
Liamputtong, P. (Ed.). (2013). Stigma, discrimina-
tion and living with HIV/AIDS: A cross-cultural
perspective. New York, NY: Springer Science �
Business Media.
McCullough, M. C., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. A.
(2003). Forgiveness, forbearance, and time: The
temporal unfolding of transgression-related inter-
personal motivations. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 540–557.
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J.,
Worthington, E. L., Jr., Brown, S. W., & Hight,
T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close rela-
tionships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measure-
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 75, 1586–1603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0022-3514.75.6.1586
Miller, D., & Vidmar, N. (1981). The social psychol-
ogy of punishment reactions. In S. Lerner & M.
Lerner (Eds.), The justice motive in social behav-
ior (pp. 145–167). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0429-4_8
Newman, L. E. (1987). The quality of mercy: On the
duty to forgive in the Judaic tradition. The Journal
of Religious Ethics, 15, 155–172.
Perse, E. M., & Rubin, R. B. (1989). Attribution in
social and parasocial relationships. Communica-
tion Research, 16, 59–77. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/009365089016001003
Piccirillo, M. S. (1986). On the authenticity of tele-
visual experience: A critical exploration of para-
social closure. Critical Studies in Mass Communi-
cation, 3, 337–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15295038609366658
Reeder, G. D. (1982). Let’s give the fundamental
attribution error another chance. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 43, 341–344.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.341
Regan, D. T., Strauss, E., & Fazio, R. (1974). Liking
and the attribution process. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology, 10, 385–397.
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his
shortcomings. Distortions in the attribution pro-
cess. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
mental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 174–177).
New York, NY: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3
Rubin, A. M., & Perse, E. M. (1987). Audience
activity and soap opera involvement: A uses and
effects investigation. Human Communication Re-
search, 14, 246–292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1468-2958.1987.tb00129.x
Russell, D. (1982). The Causal Dimension Scale: A
measure of how individuals perceive causes. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42,
1137–1145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
.42.6.1137
Sanderson, J., & Emmons, B. (2014). Extending and
withholding forgiveness to Josh Hamilton: Explor-
ing forgiveness within parasocial interaction.
Communication and Sport, 2, 24– 47. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/2167479513482306
Schramm, H., & Wirth, W. (2010). Testing a univer-
sal tool for measuring parasocial interactions
across different situations and media: Findings
from three studies. Journal of Media Psychology,
22, 26–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/
a000004
Simpson, J. A. (1987). The dissolution of romantic
relationships: Factors involved in relationship sta-
bility and emotional distress. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 53, 683– 692. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.683
Tal-Or, N., & Papirman, Y. (2007). The fundamental at-
tribution error in attributing fictional figures’ character-
istics to the actors. Media Psychology, 9, 331–345.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213260701286049
497REACTIONS TO TRANSGRESSIONS BY MEDIA FIGURES
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1956.11023049
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1956.11023049
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/02/12/jennifer-lawrence-celebrity-reputation_n_4768307.html
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/02/12/jennifer-lawrence-celebrity-reputation_n_4768307.html
http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/02/12/jennifer-lawrence-celebrity-reputation_n_4768307.html
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2867%2990034-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2867%2990034-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.002325
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.002325
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10461-013-0621-y
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10461-013-0621-y
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01653.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01653.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0702_1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15205431003668603
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0429-4_8
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365089016001003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365089016001003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15295038609366658
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15295038609366658
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.341
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601%2808%2960357-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601%2808%2960357-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1987.tb00129.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1987.tb00129.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.6.1137
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.6.1137
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167479513482306
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167479513482306
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.683
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.683
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15213260701286049
Tedeschi, J., & Felson, R. (1994). Violence, aggres-
sion, and coercive actions. Washington, DC: APA.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10160-000
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1986). The social
psychology of groups. New Brunswick, NJ: Trans-
action Books. (Original work published 1959)
Thompson, L. Y., Snyder, C. R., Hoffman, L., Mi-
chael, S. T., Rasmussen, H. N., Billings, L. S., . . .
Roberts, D. E. (2005). Dispositional forgiveness of
self, others, and situations. Journal of Personality,
73, 313–360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
6494.2005.00311.x
Tian, Q., & Hoffner, C. (2010). Parasocial interaction
with liked, neutral, and disliked characters on a
popular TV series. Mass Communication & Soci-
ety, 13, 250–269.
Received July 17, 2016
Revision received November 22, 2016
Accepted December 7, 2016 �
Correction to Skopp et al. (2018)
In the article “Positive and Negative Aspects of Facebook Use by Service
Members During Deployment to Afghanistan: Associations With Perceived
Social Support” by Nancy A. Skopp, Cynthia L. Alexander, Tracy Durham, and
Valerie Scott (Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2018, Vol. 7, No. 3, pp.
297–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000123), the scale name Social Media
Use and Integration Scale that appears in the abstract and in the Measures
section should appear instead as Social Media Use Integration Scale.
The online version of this article has been corrected.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000214
498 HU, YOUNG, LIANG, AND GUO
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10160-000
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00311.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00311.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000214
PSB and Media Figures’ Transgressions
Transgressions and Forgiveness
PSI, PSR, and PSB With Disliked Media Figures
Attribution of Transgressions Causes and FAE
Method
Sample
Procedure
Measures
PSR
Severity
PSB
Attribution of causes
Forgiveness
Results
Manipulation Check
Gender Difference
Hypotheses 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5
Hypothesis 6
Discussion
References
Correction to Skopp et al. (2018)
Journals/journal 6
BRIEF REPORT
How People With Serious Mental Illness Use Smartphones, Mobile Apps,
and Social Media
John A. Naslund, Kelly A. Aschbrenner, and Stephen J. Bartels
Dartmouth College
Objective: Research shows that people with serious mental illness are increasingly using mobile devices.
Less is known about how these individuals use their mobile devices or whether they access social media.
We surveyed individuals with serious mental illness to explore their use of these technologies. Method:
Individuals with serious mental illness engaged in lifestyle interventions through community mental
health centers completed a survey about their use of mobile and online technologies. Responses were
compared with data from the general population. Results: Among respondents (n � 70), 93% owned
cellphones, 78% used text messaging, 50% owned smartphones, and 71% used social media such as
Facebook. Most respondents reported daily use of text messaging, mobile apps, and social media.
Technology use was comparable to the general population, though smartphone ownership was lower.
Conclusions and Implications for Practice: These findings can inform future interventions that fully
leverage this group’s use of popular digital technologies.
Keywords: community mental health center, serious mental illness, smartphone, social media, technology
More than 3.5 billion people globally use mobile devices,
about 3 billion access the Internet, and more than 2 billion are
active social media users (Kemp, 2015). Researchers and cli-
nicians are developing cutting edge interventions using emerg-
ing digital, mobile, and social technologies to address health
disparities including the significantly reduced life expectancy
and elevated chronic disease burden impacting individuals liv-
ing with serious mental illness (Naslund, Marsch, McHugo, &
Bartels, 2015; Walker, McGee, & Druss, 2015). Prior studies
have documented increasing use of mobile devices among in-
dividuals living with serious mental illness (Firth et al., 2016),
however little is known about how these individuals actually
use their own mobile devices or whether they access popular
social media such as Facebook.
The success of future technology-based interventions will
depend largely on how the target population of people living
with serious mental illness use and access services through their
mobile devices. It is necessary to understand whether and how
often these individuals use features such as text messaging,
mobile apps, social media, or connecting with others to inform
future interventions that can fully leverage this group’s use of
mobile and online technology. The purpose of this study was to
explore how people living with serious mental illness who
receive services through community mental health centers use
these technologies.
Method
Participants
Participants were age 21 or older and had a serious mental
illness defined by an axis I diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizo-
affective disorder, major depressive disorder, or bipolar disor-
der (based on the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text
revision [DSM–IV]). Participants were excluded if they were
residing in a nursing home or other institution, had cognitive
impairment defined as a Mini Mental Status Exam (Folstein,
Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) score �24, had an active substance
use disorder, and were unable to speak English. Participants
were recruited from three different community mental health
This article was published Online First June 16, 2016.
John A. Naslund, Health Promotion Research Center at Dartmouth, and
The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice, Dartmouth
College; Kelly A. Aschbrenner, Health Promotion Research Center at
Dartmouth, and The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical
Practice, and Department of Psychiatry, Geisel School of Medicine, Dart-
mouth College; Stephen J. Bartels, Health Promotion Research Center at
Dartmouth, The Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Prac-
tice, Department of Psychiatry, and Department of Community and Family
Medicine, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College.
This study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental
Health (R01 MH089811), the Agency for Health care Research and Quality
(K12 HS021695-01), and from the United States Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Re-
search Center (Cooperative Agreement U48 DP005018). The funders had
no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript. Each of the authors contributed to the data
collection, analysis, and preparation of this brief report.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John A.
Naslund, 46 Centerra Parkway, Lebanon, NH 03766. E-mail: john.a
.naslund@gmail.com
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 39, No. 4, 364 –367 1095-158X/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/prj0000207
364
mailto:john.a.naslund@gmail.com
mailto:john.a.naslund@gmail.com
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/prj0000207
centers located in urban areas in New Hampshire to participate
in lifestyle intervention studies. The lifestyle interventions fo-
cused on promoting healthy eating, exercise, and weight loss,
and were delivered within community mental health settings.
Participants completed a mobile health technology survey as
part of their baseline assessment. A trained research interviewer
met with participants in person and administered the surveys in
a community mental health center setting. The mobile health
survey consisted of questions about participants’ use of the
Internet, mobile devices, text messaging, and social media, as
well as specific questions about how they use these different
mobile and online technologies. Participants were compensated
for completing the assessment. Committees for the Protection
of Human Subjects at Dartmouth College and the New Hamp-
shire Department of Health and Human Services approved all
study procedures.
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for participants’ demo-
graphic and clinical characteristics. Survey responses were tab-
ulated and compared to national data available for the general
population published by the Pew Research Center in 2015
(Anderson, 2015; Duggan, Ellison, Lampe, Lenhart, & Madden,
2015; Duggan & Page, 2015). This approach of comparing data
on mobile technology use from a community sample of people
living with serious mental illness and from the general popu-
lation was similarly employed in a recent study (Glick, Druss,
Pina, Lally, & Conde, 2015). Data collection was completed in
2014 and 2015, and data analysis was completed in 2015.
Results
Participants (N � 70) had a mean age of 47.1 years (SD �
12.4), and were mostly female (60%) and predominantly non-
Hispanic white (96%). Most participants (81%) lived indepen-
dently, 17% lived with family, many were never (46%) or
previously (44%) married, 45% had a high school diploma or
less education, and 80% were unemployed. About one quarter
(26%) of participants had a schizophrenia spectrum disorder,
41% had major depressive disorder, and 33% had bipolar dis-
order.
Table 1 highlights characteristics of participants’ use of their
mobile devices and social media, including the type of device
they use, the frequency of use, and who they contact using these
technologies. The majority (93%) of survey respondents re-
ported owning cellphones, 78% used text messaging, 50%
owned smartphones, and 71% used popular social media such as
Facebook. Most participants reported daily use of text messag-
ing, mobile apps or social media. About 30% of participants
with a smartphone or tablet indicated that they had used mobile
apps for health or wellness purposes, and about one quarter of
participants who use social media reported posting (24%) or
searching for health related information (26%) on these popular
platforms. Participants reported mainly using text messaging
and social media to connect with family or friends.
Figure 1 shows that respondents with serious mental illness
engaged in lifestyle interventions through community mental
health centers showed comparable rates of mobile and online
connectivity as the general population. Rates of cellphone own-
ership, and Internet, text messaging, and social media use were
comparable with the general population. Participants living
with serious mental illness reported lower rates of smartphone
ownership (50%) when compared with the general population
(68%).
Discussion
Our findings are consistent with prior surveys and reviews
that have demonstrated trends of increasing mobile device
ownership and digital technology use among people living with
serious mental illness recruited through community mental
health settings (Ben-Zeev, Davis, Kaiser, Krzsos, & Drake,
2013; Firth et al., 2016; Glick et al., 2015; Miller, Stewart,
Schrimsher, Peeples, & Buckley, 2015). A recent study of a
community sample of people living with serious mental illness
found similarly high rates of text messaging (78%), but lower
rates of smartphone ownership (37%; Glick et al., 2015). Al-
though half of our sample reported owning smartphones, this
was considerably lower than smartphone ownership in the gen-
eral population (68%; Anderson, 2015). Research suggests that
this gap in smartphone ownership, which has been largely
attributed to the high costs of these devices, is rapidly closing
as the devices and data plans become more affordable and
widely available (Firth et al., 2016).
Importantly, our study expands on prior surveys by exploring
how this high-risk group uses popular social media such as
Facebook or Twitter among a middle-age sample of adults with
serious mental illness. One recent survey of inpatients and
outpatients with schizophrenia and mean age of 41 years found
that close to half (47%) used social media, of which most (79%)
reported using these websites at least once each week (Miller et
al., 2015). We found that a higher proportion (71%) of our
sample used social media, which may be reflective of the fact
that our respondents were community dwelling and did not have
impaired cognitive functioning. We also found that among
respondents who reported using social media, they used these
websites frequently (87% reported using these websites weekly)
and several reported posting or searching for health related
information. Another recent survey of youth ages 12 to 21 with
serious mental illness recruited from inpatient units and outpa-
tient departments at a New York hospital found that more than
97% were social media users with Facebook as the most popular
(94%), followed by Instagram (61%), Twitter (45%), YouTube
(39%), and Tumblr (36%; Birnbaum, Rizvi, Correll, & Kane,
2015). Our survey also expands on recent studies that have
suggested that people with serious mental illness are increas-
ingly turning to popular social media to connect with others, to
seek advice, and to share their illness experiences (Highton-
Williamson, Priebe, & Giacco, 2015; Naslund, Grande, Asch-
brenner, & Elwyn, 2014). Additionally, characterizing the use
of social media among community samples of people with
serious mental illness is critical to inform efforts to leverage
these online networks for delivering interventions aimed at
promoting the mental and physical wellbeing of people living
with serious mental illness (Naslund, Aschbrenner, Marsch, &
Bartels, 2016).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
365TECHNOLOGY USE BY PEOPLE WITH SERIOUS MENTAL ILLNESS
Limitations
There are several limitations with our study that should be
considered when interpreting these findings. Given the small
sample size and lack of racial or ethnic diversity, our findings
may not be representative of the broader population of people
with serious mental illness receiving services through public
mental health settings. Further, our respondents were recruited
through community mental health centers located in urban areas
from New Hampshire, and therefore the findings may not
generalize to other settings or geographic regions. Lastly, our
survey respondents were enrolled in lifestyle interventions,
suggesting that they were already interested in health and
wellness. Therefore, our respondents may have been more
likely to post or search for health information on social media
or use mobile apps for exercise, diet, or weight loss. Despite
these limitations, our findings offer new insights about how
these individuals use their mobile devices and whether they use
social media, text messaging, and mobile apps.
Conclusions and Implications for Practice
This study contributes to a growing body of evidence highlight-
ing the potential of using smartphone technologies to reach people
living with serious mental illness and to support illness self-
management (Ben-Zeev et al., 2014), symptom monitoring
(Alvarez-Jimenez et al., 2014), and health promotion efforts tar-
geting this high-risk group (Aschbrenner, Naslund, Barre, et al.,
2015; Aschbrenner, Naslund, Shevenell, Mueser, & Bartels, 2015;
Naslund, Aschbrenner, & Bartels, 2016). The successful develop-
Table 1
How People Living With Serious Mental Illness Use Mobile Devices and Social Media
Survey responses Participants, n (%)
Among the 63 (93%) participants who own a cellphone:
Use a cellphone to access the Internet 25 (40%)
Frequency of sending or receiving text messages
Daily 31 (49%)
Weekly 12 (19%)
Less than once per week 6 (10%)
Use text messaging to contact
Family 38 (60%)
Partner or spouse 10 (16%)
Friend 35 (56%)
Among the 41 (59%) participants who own a smartphone or tablet:
Type of mobile device
Android Smartphone 23 (56%)
iPhone 5 (12%)
Tablet 10 (24%)
Other smartphone 3 (8%)
Use mobile device to access apps 39 (95%)
Frequency of using mobile apps
Daily 30 (73%)
Weekly 6 (15%)
Less than once per week 4 (10%)
Use mobile apps to connect with family and friends 25 (61%)
Amount willing to pay for a mobile app
Will not pay, only download free apps 26 (63%)
Will pay only $.99 for an app 2 (5%)
Will pay more than $.99 for an app 12 (29%)
Use of mobile apps for health
Use mobile apps for exercise 12 (29%)
Use mobile apps for diet 14 (34%)
Use mobile apps for weight loss 11 (27%)
Use mobile apps for quitting smoking 1 (2%)
Among the 50 (71%) participants who use social media:
Device used to access social media
Smartphone 24 (48%)
Tablet 16 (32%)
Computer 32 (64%)
Frequency of using social media
Daily 38 (79%)
Weekly 4 (8%)
Less than once per week 6 (13%)
Use social media to connect with
Family 37 (74%)
Partner or spouse 2 (4%)
Friend 42 (84%)
Ever posted personal health information on social media 12 (24%)
Ever searched for health information on social media 13 (26%)
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
366 NASLUND, ASCHBRENNER, AND BARTELS
ment and dissemination of mobile health interventions likely will
be dependent on whether these interventions can be seamlessly
integrated into the patterns of daily mobile device use among the
target group of people with serious mental illness. For example,
mobile health interventions must capitalize on the ways in which
people with serious mental illness use their devices, further high-
lighting the importance of understanding how these individuals use
mobile and online technologies. Our findings can inform efforts
that take full advantage of the different features of these technol-
ogies and could support the design of tailored interventions to
extend the reach and quality of services delivered through com-
munity mental health settings.
References
Alvarez-Jimenez, M., Alcazar-Corcoles, M. A., González-Blanch, C., Ben-
dall, S., McGorry, P. D., & Gleeson, J. F. (2014). Online, social media
and mobile technologies for psychosis treatment: A systematic review
on novel user-led interventions. Schizophrenia Research, 156, 96 –106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.021
Anderson, M. (2015). Technology device ownership: 2015. Retrieved from
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/10/PI_2015-10-29_device-
ownership_FINAL
Aschbrenner, K. A., Naslund, J. A., Barre, L. K., Mueser, K. T., Kinney,
A., & Bartels, S. J. (2015). Peer health coaching for overweight and
obese individuals with serious mental illness: Intervention development
and initial feasibility study. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 5, 277–
284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13142-015-0313-4
Aschbrenner, K. A., Naslund, J. A., Shevenell, M., Mueser, K. T., &
Bartels, S. J. (2015). Feasibility of behavioral weight loss treatment
enhanced with peer support and mobile health technology for individuals
with serious mental illness. Psychiatric Quarterly, 87, 401– 415. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11126-015-9395-x
Ben-Zeev, D., Brenner, C. J., Begale, M., Duffecy, J., Mohr, D. C., &
Mueser, K. T. (2014). Feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary efficacy
of a smartphone intervention for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
40, 1244 –1253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu033
Ben-Zeev, D., Davis, K. E., Kaiser, S., Krzsos, I., & Drake, R. E. (2013).
Mobile technologies among people with serious mental illness: Oppor-
tunities for future services. Administration and Policy in Mental Health
and Mental Health Services Research, 40, 340 –343. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/s10488-012-0424-x
Birnbaum, M. L., Rizvi, A. F., Correll, C. U., & Kane, J. M. (2015). Role
of social media and the Internet in pathways to care for adolescents and
young adults with psychotic disorders and non-psychotic mood disor-
ders. Early Intervention in Psychiatry. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eip.12237
Duggan, M., Ellison, N. B., Lampe, C., Lenhart, A., & Madden, M. (2015).
Social media update 2014. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/
files/2015/01/PI_SocialMediaUpdate20144
Duggan, M., & Page, D. (2015). Mobile messaging and social media 2015.
Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/08/Social-Media-
Update-2015-FINAL2
Firth, J., Cotter, J., Torous, J., Bucci, S., Firth, J. A., & Yung, A. R. (2016).
Mobile phone ownership and endorsement of “mHealth” among people
with psychosis: A meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies. Schizophre-
nia Bulletin, 42, 448 – 455.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental
state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for
the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189 –198. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
Glick, G., Druss, B., Pina, J., Lally, C., & Conde, M. (2015). Use of mobile
technology in a community mental health setting. Journal of Telemedi-
cine and Telecare, 22, 430 – 435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
1357633X15613236
Highton-Williamson, E., Priebe, S., & Giacco, D. (2015). Online social
networking in people with psychosis: A systematic review. International
Journal of Social Psychiatry, 61, 92–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0020764014556392
Kemp, S. (2015). Digital, social and mobile worldwide in 2015. Retrieved
from http://wearesocial.com/uk/special-reports/digital-social-mobile-
worldwide-2015.
Miller, B. J., Stewart, A., Schrimsher, J., Peeples, D., & Buckley, P. F.
(2015). How connected are people with schizophrenia? Cell phone,
computer, email, and social media use. Psychiatry Research, 225, 458 –
463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.067
Naslund, J. A., Aschbrenner, K. A., & Bartels, S. J. (2016). Wearable
devices and smartphones for activity tracking among people with serious
mental illness. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 10, 10 –17. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2016.02.001
Naslund, J. A., Aschbrenner, K. A., Marsch, L. A., & Bartels, S. J. (2016).
The future of mental health care: Peer-to-peer support and social media.
Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25, 113–122. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1017/S2045796015001067
Naslund, J. A., Grande, S. W., Aschbrenner, K. A., & Elwyn, G. (2014).
Naturally occurring peer support through social media: The experiences
of individuals with severe mental illness using YouTube. PLoS ONE, 9,
e110171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110171
Naslund, J. A., Marsch, L. A., McHugo, G. J., & Bartels, S. J. (2015).
Emerging mHealth and eHealth interventions for serious mental illness:
A review of the literature. Journal of Mental Health, 24, 321–332.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09638237.2015.1019054
Walker, E. R., McGee, R. E., & Druss, B. G. (2015). Mortality in mental
disorders and global disease burden implications: A systematic review
and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association Psy-
chiatry, 72, 334 –341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014
.2502
Received January 27, 2016
Revision received May 18, 2016
Accepted May 20, 2016 �
Figure 1. Technology use among people living with serious mental
illness compared with the general population. Values represent proportions
of the entire community sample (n � 70) of people living with serious
mental illness surveyed. National survey data were obtained from the Pew
Research Center reports from 2015 on Internet, smartphone, and social
media use (Anderson, 2015; Duggan et al., 2015; Duggan & Page, 2015).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
367TECHNOLOGY USE BY PEOPLE WITH SERIOUS MENTAL ILLNESS
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.021
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/10/PI_2015-10-29_device-ownership_FINAL
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/10/PI_2015-10-29_device-ownership_FINAL
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13142-015-0313-4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11126-015-9395-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11126-015-9395-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu033
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10488-012-0424-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10488-012-0424-x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eip.12237
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/01/PI_SocialMediaUpdate20144
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/01/PI_SocialMediaUpdate20144
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/08/Social-Media-Update-2015-FINAL2
http://www.pewinternet.org/files/2015/08/Social-Media-Update-2015-FINAL2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956%2875%2990026-6
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956%2875%2990026-6
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1357633X15613236
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1357633X15613236
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0020764014556392
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0020764014556392
http://wearesocial.com/uk/special-reports/digital-social-mobile-worldwide-2015
http://wearesocial.com/uk/special-reports/digital-social-mobile-worldwide-2015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.067
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2016.02.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2016.02.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015001067
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S2045796015001067
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110171
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09638237.2015.1019054
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2502
http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2502
How People With Serious Mental Illness Use Smartphones, Mobile Apps, and Social Media
Method
Participants
Data Analysis
Results
Discussion
Limitations
Conclusions and Implications for Practice
References
Journals/Journal 7
BRIEF REPORT
The Quality of Social Networks Predicts Age-Related Changes in
Cardiovascular Reactivity to Stress
Bert N. Uchino, Robert G. Kent de Grey, and Sierra Cronan
University of Utah
Although existing life span models suggest that positivity in relationships should benefit the health of
older adults, much less is known about how relationships that contain both positive and negative aspects
(i.e., ambivalent ties) might influence age-associated cardiovascular risk. Given the increased interper-
sonal stress associated with ambivalent ties, the SAVI model would predict that older adults might be
more negatively influenced given age-related changes in physiological flexibility. In this study, the
quality of an individual’s social network (i.e., supportive, ambivalent, aversive) was used to predict
cardiovascular reactivity during laboratory stress across a 10-month follow-up period in 108 participants
between the ages 30 to 70. Results revealed evidence that the number of ambivalent network ties
predicted greater increases in diastolic blood pressure reactivity. Importantly, there was an Age �
Ambivalent Ties interaction in which the number of ambivalent ties was related to greater increases in
systolic blood pressure reactivity primarily in older adults. These data are discussed in terms of the health
implications of social networks across the life span.
Keywords: ambivalent ties, cardiovascular reactivity, social support
The quality of one’s social relationships is a robust predictor of
physical health outcomes. In one meta-analysis, supportive rela-
tionships predicted lower mortality rates even when considering
initial health status and geographic region (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, &
Layton, 2010). The link between social support and health shown
in the meta-analysis was comparable with, if not stronger than,
widely accepted risk factors, including smoking and physical ac-
tivity. Importantly, the chronic conditions associated with greater
mortality develop slowly over time. Hence life span relationship
processes may help explain age-associated risk for chronic condi-
tions such as cardiovascular disease (Antonucci, Ajrouch, &
Birditt, 2014; Charles, 2010; Uchino, Ong, Queen, & Kent de
Gray, 2016).
Consistent with epidemiological findings, most major life span
models emphasize the benefits that accrue in the social networks of
older adults. Socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) highlights
the importance of emotional goals as individuals age which lead
older individuals to actively select or prioritize emotionally close
social ties to more peripheral ones (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, &
Charles, 1999). Thus, the social networks of older adults should
contain emotionally positive relationships who would serve as rich
sources of social support. The strength and vulnerability integra-
tion (SAVI) model is a more general perspective built on SST and
explains why older adults often have better well-being and takes
into account both perceptions of time left but also experience
gained over time (Charles, 2010). With experience, individuals
have learned to avoid negative situations and/or de-escalate exist-
ing negativity with close others by using selective strategies, some
of which are activated after the fact (Blanchard-Fields, 2007;
Charles, 2010). By incorporating aspects of SST, the predictions of
SAVI regarding the importance of positive ties for health are
similar.
One important aspect of the SAVI model is that it also high-
lights conditions under which one might expect age-associated
differences in the links between relationship processes and health-
related outcomes. That is, the SAVI model predicts that older
adults may be more negatively influenced by interpersonal stres-
sors given that older adults have less physiological flexibility (e.g.,
stiffer arteries, Charles, 2010; Ferrari, Radaelli, & Centola, 2003).
This is an important point because many close relationships con-
tain both negative and positive aspects and create significant
interpersonal distress (Campo et al., 2009).
Life span work on intergenerational ties (e.g., parents and their
adult offspring) suggests that ambivalence in such relationships is
quite common and grounded in specific interpersonal processes
such as unsolicited advice, personality differences, child rearing,
and past relationship problems (Birditt, Miller, Fingerman, &
Lefkowitz, 2009; Pillemer et al., 2007). More generally, ambiva-
Bert N. Uchino, Robert G. Kent de Grey, and Sierra Cronan, Department
of Psychology and Health Psychology Program, University of Utah.
This research was generously supported by James A. Shannon Director’s
Award 1-R55-AG13968 from the National Institute on Aging. We thank
David Lozano, Daniel Litvack, John T. Cacioppo, Robert Kelsey, and
William Guethlein for their expert technical assistance and for providing us
with copies of their data acquisition and reduction software (i.e., ANS suite
and ENSCOREL).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Bert N.
Uchino, Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 380 S. 1530 E., Rm.
502, Salt Lake City, UT 84112-0251. E-mail: bert.uchino@psych.utah.edu
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Psychology and Aging © 2016 American Psychological Association
2016, Vol. 31, No. 4, 321–326 0882-7974/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000092
321
mailto:bert.uchino@psych.utah.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000092
lent ties appear to exert detrimental influences on health, above
and beyond relationship positivity and negativity, as a result of
increased interpersonal stress and reduced support seeking (Holt-
Lunstad, Uchino, Smith, & Hicks, 2007; Reblin, Uchino, & Smith,
2010). In addition, ambivalent ties are typically close relationships
and the existing positivity makes it more difficult to avoid such
ties, at least compared with aversive relationships (Uchino, Holt-
Lunstad, Uno, & Flinders, 2001). As a result, ambivalent ties are
independent predictors of worse health outcomes including greater
inflammation and coronary artery calcification (Uchino et al.,
2013; Uchino, Smith, & Berg, 2014).
Consistent with the importance of examining positive and neg-
ative network qualities, one relevant study utilized such a compre-
hensive assessment and examined age differences in cardiovascu-
lar reactivity (ages 30 to 70) to a stress reactivity protocol (Uchino
et al., 2001). This cross-sectional study found that both positivity
and ambivalence in network ties predicted cardiovascular reactiv-
ity during stress (Uchino et al., 2001). For instance, a low number
of supportive ties and a high number of ambivalent ties predicted
greater heart rate reactivity as a function of age. However, no study
to date has examined such associations longitudinally. This is
important because social networks may influence older adults for
better or worse thereby influencing their ability to cope with stress
in a laboratory setting over time. Such laboratory stress assess-
ments are thought to reflect how individuals cope with stress in
their daily life and is a predictor of future cardiovascular disease
risk in otherwise healthy populations (Chida & Steptoe, 2010).
The main goal of this study was thus to examine the prediction
of changes in cardiovascular reactivity over a 10-month period via
social network quality and its potential moderation by age. Based
on prior work, it was predicted that the number of supportive and
ambivalent ties would predict changes in cardiovascular reactivity:
a higher number of supportive ties should predict lower cardio-
vascular reactivity over time whereas a higher number of ambiv-
alent ties should predict greater cardiovascular reactivity over
time. Although our prior work suggests more limited influences for
aversive ties, consistent with the SAVI model it was predicted that
the link between aversive and ambivalent ties would be moderated
by age as a higher number of such ties should be related to greater
increases in cardiovascular reactivity primarily in older adults.
Method
Participants
The original study tested 64 men and 69 women between the
ages of 30 and 70 (see Uchino et al., 2001). Approximately equal
numbers of men and women were recruited from each decade
group (e.g., 30 to 39) through advertisements placed in local
newspapers. Individuals were paid $35.00 for approximately 2.5
hours of participation. The following self-reported inclusion crite-
ria were used to select healthy participants: no existing hyperten-
sion, no cardiovascular prescription medication use, no past his-
tory of chronic disease with a cardiovascular component (e.g.,
diabetes), no recent history of psychological disorder (e.g., major
depressive disorder), no tobacco use, and no consumption of more
than 10 alcoholic beverages a week (Cacioppo et al., 1995).
In the follow-up, individuals were rescreened according to the
inclusion criteria listed above and paid $35.00 for approximately
2.5 hours of participation; 108 (81%) of the original sample were
retested on average 10 months later (SD � 1.6, range of 7 to 16
months). The main reasons for attrition were related to our inabil-
ity to contact and in a small minority of cases problems in sched-
uling individuals. Importantly, there were no significant differ-
ences in demographic characteristics between participants who
were lost at the follow-up in comparison to participants who were
retested (see Uchino, Holt-Lunstad, Bloor, & Campo, 2005).
Procedure
Individuals were first recontacted via telephone and rescreened
according to the inclusion criteria detailed above. Qualifying in-
dividuals were scheduled for an appointment and participants’
self-reports were again checked for reliability against the inclusion
criteria. Upon arrival, participants completed an informed consent
document and a demographic background questionnaire. Follow-
ing completion of these questionnaires, the participant’s height and
weight were obtained using a standard medical scale from which
body mass index was calculated (i.e., weight in kg/height2 in
meters).
Participants were then escorted to a separate sound attenuated
room where an occluding cuff of appropriate size was also placed
on the upper left arm. Individuals were seated in a comfortable
chair and instructed to relax for the next 12 min while resting
measures of cardiovascular function were obtained. During the
final 5 min of the resting assessment, cardiovascular assessments
of heart rate, SBP, and DBP were obtained once every 90 seconds.
Following the resting assessments, participants performed a speech
and mental arithmetic protocol (Cacioppo et al., 1995). The same
basic stressors tasks were used for the time two reactivity protocol
with slight changes to minimize habituation (e.g., different speech
topic and different set of serial subtractions). The order of the
stressors was counterbalanced and all verbal instructions were
standardized (see Uchino et al., 2005). Upon completion of both
psychological stressors, participants were debriefed, compensated,
and thanked for their participation. Although impedance measures
of cardiac output, preejection period, and respiratory sinus arrhyth-
mia were collected as part of the larger protocol, these measures
did not elucidate the underlying determinants of blood pressure
and heart rate reported below so they are not considered further.
Assessments
Social Relationships Index (SRI). The SRI was completed at
Time 1 and instructed individuals to list the initials of up to 10
important network members with whom they have contact (Campo
et al., 2009). These network members were then rated in terms of
how helpful and upsetting they were (1 � not at all, 2 � a little,
3 � sometimes, 4 � moderately, 5 � very, 6 � extremely) when
the participant needed emotional, tangible, and informational sup-
port. These helpful and upsetting ratings for aspects of support
have been shown to load on a general positivity and negativity
factor (Campo et al., 2009). For the present study, the internal
consistencies for the SRI helpful and upset ratings were compara-
ble to our pilot work (alphas of .76 to .87). To increase reliability
through aggregation, for each network member an index of posi-
tivity and negativity was calculated by averaging across support
components within helpful and upset ratings.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
322 UCHINO, KENT DE GREY, AND CRONAN
Consistent with our prior work, we operationalized different cate-
gories of social relationships as the number of individuals in one’s
network who were only sources of positivity (i.e., supportive ties),
only sources of negativity (i.e., aversive ties), or sources of both
positivity and negativity (i.e., ambivalent ties). Thus, a socially sup-
portive network member was an individual rated as a “2” or greater on
helpful and only a “1” on upset and an aversive network tie was an
individual rated as only a “1” on helpful and a “2” or greater on upset.
An ambivalent network member was rated as a “2” or greater on both
helpful and upset ratings. In our prior work, these network measures
were temporally stable with significant 3-month test–retest correla-
tions of r � .61 (p � .001) for the number of supportive ties, r � .30
(p � .001) for the number of aversive ties, and r � .68 (p � .001) for
the number of ambivalent ties. The SRI also has good convergent
(e.g., perceived support) and discriminant (e.g., personality) validity
(Campo et al., 2009).
Cardiovascular measures. A Minnesota Impedance Cardio-
graph Model 304B was used to measure heart rate, whereas a Di-
namap Model 8100 monitor (Critikon corporation, Tampa, Florida)
was used to measure blood pressure. The Dinamap used the oscillo-
metric method to estimate blood pressure (see Gorback, Quill, &
Lavine, 1991 for validation). Mean SBP, DBP, and heart rate for each
epoch was averaged across minutes to increase the reliability of these
assessments (Kamarck et al., 1992).
Statistical Analyses
For primary analyses, moderated regression analyses with pre-
dictors centered at the grand mean were used to examine the
separate links between the number of supportive, aversive, and
ambivalent ties at Time 1 and subsequent age-related changes in
cardiovascular reactivity (i.e., task minus baseline). Age was
treated as a continuous factor in all analyses to increase statistical
power. To conduct these tests, the Age � Social Tie cross-product
terms (based on the centered main effects) were entered into the
model after the respective main effects (Aiken & West, 1991).
Main effects of social ties on cardiovascular reactivity were ex-
amined without the cross-product term. Effects sizes for significant
associations were calculated based on f2 (Aiken & West, 1991).
Consistent with prior work, all analyses of Time 2 cardiovascular
reactivity measures controlled for age, sex, changes in body mass
index, Time 1 resting measures of cardiovascular function and
cardiovascular reactivity, as well as Time 2 resting measures of
cardiovascular function. In addition, all analyses statistically con-
trolled for race/ethnicity (White, Other), education, and income.
Simple slope analyses were carried out to examine the form of any
significant Age � Social Tie interactions by testing for the signif-
icance of the slope one standard deviation above and below the
mean for age (Aiken & West, 1991). Finally, ancillary analyses
examined if any significant associations between positive, nega-
tive, or ambivalent network ties were relatively independent of
each other by statistically controlling for the other relationship
categories.
Results
Sample Characteristics
The average age of the follow-up sample was 47.9 (SD � 10.9,
range 30 to 70). Over 84% of the sample was non-Hispanic White.
The median education and yearly income were partial college/
graduate college and $20,000 to $29,000, respectively. Participants
also listed an average of 9.34 important network members (out of
10). The average number of ambivalent ties was 6.1, the average
number of supportive ties was 3.0, and the average number of
aversive ties was 0.2. Finally, as detailed in our prior publication
with this sample, age was related to greater increases in stress
reactivity (e.g., SBP reactivity) over time (see Uchino et al., 2005).
Main Analyses
Given the importance of positive network ties in life span
models, analyses first examined whether the number of supportive
network ties predicted age-related changes in cardiovascular reac-
tivity. In these models, there were no significant main effects of
supportive ties nor any Age � Supportive ties interaction on any
measures of cardiovascular reactivity (see Table 1).1
Analyses focusing on the number of ambivalent ties showed one
main effect as a relatively high number of ambivalent ties at Time
1 predicted greater increases in DBP reactivity over time, b � .41,
95% CI [.10, .73], � � .22, p � .01, f 2 � .06. Importantly, there
was also an Age � Ambivalent Ties interaction for changes in
SBP reactivity, b � .05, 95% CI [.01, .09], � � .16, p � .02, f 2 �
.075.2 These represent small to moderate effect sizes (.02 to .15,
Aiken & West, 1991). As shown in Figure 1, simple slope analyses
for SBP reactivity one standard deviation above and below the
mean for age showed that the number of ambivalent ties predicted
greater increases in reactivity in older (p � .03) but not younger
(p � .24) participants. Consistent with our prior work on aversive
ties, no significant main effects or interactions were found on any
measures of cardiovascular reactivity.
Finally, analyses were run to examine whether the results above
for ambivalent ties were independent of the other categories. First
of all, the number of ambivalent ties remained an independent
predictor of DBP reactivity even when statistically controlling for
the number of supportive ties and aversive ties (ps � .02). The
Age � Ambivalent Ties interaction on SBP reactivity was also still
significant while statistically controlling for the Age � Supportive
Ties and Age � Aversive Ties interactions (ps � .05).
Discussion
The main goal of this study was to examine whether age
moderated the link between relationship quality and cardiovascular
reactivity during stress over time. The SAVI model would predict
that older adults in particular might have problems coping with
interpersonal stress because of age-related decreases in physiolog-
ical flexibility (Charles, 2010). In partial support of this model, the
1 There was a marginally significant Age � Supportive Ties interaction
on changes in heart rate reactivity during acute stress, b � .03, 95% CI
[�.002, .06], � � .12, p � .07. Exploratory simple slope analyses within
age groups found that the only simple effect to approach significance was
within the younger group, where the number of supportive ties predicted a
greater decrease in heart rate reactivity over time (p � .09).
2 There was a marginal Age � Ambivalent Ties interaction on heart rate
reactivity, b � �.02, 95% CI [�.05, .001], � � �.12, p � .07. Exploratory
simple slope analyses showed that the number of ambivalent ties primarily
predicted greater increases in heart rate reactivity in the younger (p � .09)
group.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
323AGE, SOCIAL TIES, AND CARDIOVASCULAR CHANGES
number of ambivalent ties predicted greater increases in DBP
reactivity and greater increases in SBP reactivity over time pri-
marily in older adults. Although the relatively short follow-up
precludes strong inferences about cardiovascular changes, these
findings are potentially important because SBP and DBP reactivity
assessed in laboratory setting are strong predictors of future car-
diovascular risk such as incident hypertension (Chida & Steptoe,
2010).
Most of the prior work in the area emphasizes the importance of
relationship positivity in protecting individuals from adverse
health outcomes (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010). This study provided
little evidence for the benefits of supportive ties in predicting
age-related changes in cardiovascular reactivity. The primacy of
ambivalent ties over supportive ties in our study might be attrib-
utable to a negativity bias in which stimuli which contain negative
properties have more robust associations to physiological out-
comes (Taylor, 1991). Of course, the sample size was relatively
small which resulted in a few marginally significant findings (see
Footnote 1 and 2) so future work would be needed to examine such
associations with greater statistical power.
The SAVI model also highlights the potential problems older
adults might have in coping with negative social ties (Charles,
2010). Such negative influences were not detected in regards to
aversive (negative) ties. However, such ties might be easier to cope
with given that they are less close and hence individuals more
readily discount or ignore them (Uchino et al., 2001). In addition,
older adults are more likely to use selectivity as a coping mecha-
nism and hence avoid such ties in their daily life (Carstensen et al.,
1999). However, the results for ambivalent ties appear consistent
with the model. The SAVI model highlights the potential difficul-
ties older adults have in coping with high arousal situations
(Charles, 2010; also see Labouvie-Vief, 2008). This argument
assumes that interactions with ambivalent ties are relatively high in
arousal compared to interactions with aversive relationships. In-
deed, coping with ambivalent ties appears taxing (Fingerman,
Pitzer, Lefkowitz, Birditt, & Mroczek, 2008) and results in higher
levels of ambulatory blood pressure during everyday life compared
with aversive ties (Holt-Lunstad, Uchino, Smith, Olson-Cerny, &
Nealey-Moore, 2003). This might tax the coping abilities of older
adults who are already showing age-related declines in cardiovas-
cular functioning such that they are less able to cope with stressors
more generally as indexed by our laboratory protocol (Charles,
2010). Future work that directly models important mediators (e.g.,
level of arousal) postulated by existing life span models will be
key to integrating empirical data in this domain.
There are several limitations of this study. One important lim-
itation is that the time frame for the follow-up was relatively short,
so whether these results reflect enduring changes or shorter fluc-
Table 1
Main Results for Moderated Regression Models Predicting Changes in SBP, DBP, and Heart Rate Over the 10-Month Period
Predictor
Systolic blood pressure Diastolic blood pressure Heart rate
b SE p � 95% CI b SE p � 95% CI b SE p � 95% CI
Ambivalent ties
T1 baseline .19 .07 .007 .26 .05, .32 .29 4.28 �.001 .52 .16, .43 .14 .04 �.001 .30 .06, .22
T2 baseline �.09 .07 .204 �.12 �.22, .05 �.29 �4.29 �.001 �.52 �.42, �.15 �.15 .04 �.001 �.32 �.23, �.07
T1 reactivity .73 .08 �.001 .66 .57, .89 .46 5.46 �.001 .48 .29, .62 .60 .05 �.001 .76 .49, .71
Age .18 .06 .004 .23 .06, .30 .03 .65 .515 .06 �.05, .11 .01 .03 .798 .02 �.05, .07
Sex �1.52 1.20 .208 �.09 �3.91, .86 1.09 1.20 .234 .11 �.71, 2.88 1.13 .70 .108 .11 �.25, 2.51
Ethnicity .25 .83 .765 .02 �1.39, 1.89 �.72 �1.20 .234 �.11 �1.92, .47 .91 .48 .060 .13 �.04, 1.87
Income �.26 .34 .442 �.06 �.94, .41 .34 1.40 .166 .12 �.14, .82 �.16 .18 .377 �.06 �.53, .20
Education .61 .46 .184 .09 �.30, 1.52 .37 1.12 .268 .10 �.29, 1.02 .11 .27 .672 .03 �.42, .65
BMI change 8.74 274.94 .975 �.01 �537, 555 139.09 197.26 .483 .06 �253, 531 27.87 151.48 .854 .01 �273, 329
Ambivalent ties T1 .17 .22 .429 .05 �.26, .60 .41 2.63 .010 .22 .10, .73 .04 .12 .729 .02 �.20, .29
Age � T1 Ambivalent ties .05 .02 .022 .16 .01, .09 .01 .59 .554 .05 �.02, .04 �.02 .01 .066 �.13 �.05, .001
Supportive ties
T1 baseline .20 .07 .006 .27 .06, .34 .30 .07 �.001 .53 .16, .44 .15 .04 �.001 .31 .07, .23
T2 baseline �.10 .07 .161 �.13 �.24, .04 �.28 .07 �.001 �.51 �.42, �.15 �.15 .04 �.001 �.32 �.23, �.07
T1 reactivity .75 .08 �.001 .68 .59, .92 .47 .09 �.001 .50 .30, .64 .59 .05 �.001 .75 .49, .70
Age .16 .06 .012 .20 .04, .28 .01 .04 .767 .03 �.07, .09 .01 .03 .688 .03 �.05, .07
Sex �1.41 1.23 .254 �.08 �3.86, 1.03 1.10 .92 .236 .11 �.73, 2.93 1.12 .70 .111 .11 �.26, 2.50
Ethnicity .27 .86 .754 .02 �1.43, 1.97 �.69 .62 .268 �.10 �1.93, .54 .93 .48 .059 .13 �.04, 1.89
Income �.17 .35 .634 �.04 �.85, .52 .39 .24 .110 .14 �.09, .08 �.17 .18 .352 �.06 �.53, .19
Education .55 .48 .256 .08 �.40, 1.50 .38 .34 .277 .10 �.31, 1.06 .03 .28 .906 .01 �.52, .58
BMI change �103.0 290.35 .724 �.03 �679, 473 72.91 207.04 .726 .03 �338, 484 107.0 155.41 .493 .05 �202, 416
Supportive ties T1 �.04 .27 .869 �.01 �.57, .48 �.31 .19 .112 �.14 �.69, .07 .03 .14 .810 .02 �.25, .32
Age � T1 supportive ties �.03 .03 .355 �.07 �.08, .03 �.01 .02 .581 �.05 �.05, .03 .03 .01 .072 .12 �.002, .06
Aversive ties
T1 baseline .20 .07 .004 .28 .07, .34 .28 .07 �.001 .50 .14, .42 .14 .04 �.001 .31 .06, .23
T2 baseline �.11 .07 .097 �.15 �.25, .02 �.26 .07 �.001 �.48 �.40, �.13 �.15 .04 �.001 �.33 �.24, �.07
T1 reactivity .74 .08 �.001 .67 .58, .91 .45 .09 �.001 .48 .28, .62 .57 .05 �.001 .72 .46, .68
Age .16 .06 .012 .21 .04, .29 .01 .04 .808 .02 �.07, .09 .02 .03 .563 .04 �.04, .08
Sex �1.17 1.24 .346 �.07 �3.63, 1.29 1.27 .93 .175 .13 �.58, 3.11 1.18 .72 .105 .12 �.25, 2.60
Ethnicity .11 .84 .898 .01 �1.57, 1.78 �.61 .62 .325 �.09 �1.83, .61 .99 .49 .045 .14 .02, 1.95
Income �.21 .35 .547 �.05 �.91, .48 .31 .25 .223 .11 �.19, .81 �.22 .19 .236 �.08 �.60, .15
Education .43 .46 .358 .07 �.49, 1.35 .28 .34 .412 .07 �.39, .94 .13 .28 .650 .03 �.42, .67
BMI change �21.56 284.21 .940 �.01 �586, 543 180.12 202.66 .376 .08 �222, 582 66.51 154.22 .667 .03 �240, 373
Aversive ties T1 �.61 1.25 .625 �.04 �3.09, 1.86 �.22 .91 .810 �.03 �2.02, 1.58 �.41 .70 .556 �.05 �1.79, .97
Age � T1 Aversive ties �.10 .15 .528 �.05 �.40, .21 �.13 .11 .230 �.13 �.36, .09 �.01 .09 .873 �.01 �.19, .16
Note. Ambivalent, supportive, and aversive ties main effects statistics are based on models without the cross-product term.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
324 UCHINO, KENT DE GREY, AND CRONAN
tuations is unknown. How quickly or slowly cardiovascular reac-
tivity to stress changes over time in older adults and its relevance
to health is difficult to determine as there is little longitudinal work
on the issue (Uchino et al., 2005). However, there is predictive
validity to such assessments at any given point in time as higher
SBP and DBP reactivity to laboratory stress predicts incident
hypertension many years later (Chida & Steptoe, 2010). Future
longitudinal work that follows individuals over a longer period of
time will be needed to test for slower changes that might result in
smaller effect sizes that the present study was underpowered to
detect. In a related point, we did not assess the SRI at both time
points, so whether relationships are changing over this time period
is unknown. Although there is both stability and change in the
relationships of aging adults (Carstensen et al., 1999), ambivalent
ties may be relatively stable as they are important familial ties
(e.g., parents, adult offspring; van Gaalen, Dykstra, & Komter,
2010) and can be maintained as both explicit and implicit relation-
ship representations (Petty, Tormala, Briñol, & Jarvis, 2006).
Nevertheless, this remains an important issue as most prior work
has not examined changes in ambivalent relationships across the
life span. Finally, although the sample was relatively large for
work examining cardiovascular reactivity, future work will be
needed using larger samples sizes to increase confidence in the
pattern of results.
Nevertheless, these data are also among the first evidence link-
ing ambivalent ties to age-related increases in cardiovascular re-
activity over time. Although the associations detected in this study
are closer to a small effect size, it is important to highlight that
greater blood pressure reactivity assessed in similar laboratory
settings are linked to increased cardiovascular disease risk (Chida
& Steptoe, 2010; Uchino, Birmingham, & Berg, 2010). In fact,
these effect sizes are comparable with those found in a meta-
analysis linking increased cardiovascular reactivity to cardiovas-
cular disease (Chida & Steptoe, 2010). Although research has not
yet tested whether intervention-related reductions in stress reac-
tivity can decrease cardiovascular risk over time, these data high-
light the potential importance of future work in the area. To this
point, interventions can be aimed at helping older adults cope with
such ambivalent ties using established (e.g., family or marital
therapy) or potentially unique (e.g., meditation) approaches that
appear successful in reducing biological reactivity (Ditzen, Hahl-
weg, Fehm-Wolfsdorf, & Baucom, 2011; Nyklíček, Mommer-
steeg, Van Beugen, Ramakers, & Van Boxtel, 2013).
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park: Sage.
Antonucci, T. C., Ajrouch, K. J., & Birditt, K. S. (2014). The Convoy
Model: Explaining social relations from a multidisciplinary perspective.
The Gerontologist, 54, 82–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnt118
Birditt, K. S., Miller, L. M., Fingerman, K. L., & Lefkowitz, E. S. (2009).
Tensions in the parent and adult child relationship: Links to solidarity
and ambivalence. Psychology and Aging, 24, 287–295. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0015196
Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 26 –31. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x
Cacioppo, J. T., Malarkey, W. B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Uchino, B. N.,
Sgoutas-Emch, S. A., Sheridan, J. F., . . . Glaser, R. (1995). Heteroge-
neity in neuroendocrine and immune responses to brief psychological
stressors as a function of autonomic cardiac activation. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 57, 154 –164. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pubmed/7792374
Campo, R. A., Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Vaughn, A., Reblin, M., &
Smith, T. W. (2009). The assessment of positivity and negativity in
social networks: The reliability and validity of the social relationships
index. Journal of Community Psychology, 37, 471– 486. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1002/jcop.20308
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time
seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psycholo-
gist, 54, 165–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165
Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integration: A model of
emotional well-being across adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 136,
1068 –1091. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021232
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2010). Greater cardiovascular responses to
laboratory mental stress are associated with poor subsequent cardiovas-
cular risk status: A meta-analysis of prospective evidence. Hypertension,
55, 1026 –1032. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109
.146621
Ditzen, B., Hahlweg, K., Fehm-Wolfsdorf, G., & Baucom, D. (2011).
Assisting couples to develop healthy relationships: Effects of couples
relationship education on cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 597–
607. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.019
Ferrari, A. U., Radaelli, A., & Centola, M. (2003). Invited review: Aging
and the cardiovascular system. Journal of Applied Physiology, 95, 2591–
2597. http://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00601.2003
Fingerman, K. L., Pitzer, L., Lefkowitz, E. S., Birditt, K. S., & Mroczek,
D. (2008). Ambivalent relationship qualities between adults and their
parents: Implications for the well-being of both parties. The Journals of
Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63,
362–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.P362
Gorback, M. S., Quill, T. J., & Lavine, M. L. (1991). The relative accu-
racies of two automated noninvasive arterial pressure measurement
devices. Journal of Clinical Monitoring, 7, 13–22. http://www.ncbi.nlm
.nih.gov/pubmed/1999694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01617893
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships
and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7,
e1000316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316
Holt-Lunstad, J., Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., & Hicks, A. (2007). On the
importance of relationship quality: The impact of ambivalence in friend-
8
10
12
14
16
18
20
Low Amb. Ties High Amb. Ties
Young Older
S
B
P
R
ea
ct
iv
ity
(m
m
H
g)
Figure 1. Predicted changes in SBP reactivity over the 10-month period
one standard deviation above and below the mean for age and the number
of ambivalent ties.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
325AGE, SOCIAL TIES, AND CARDIOVASCULAR CHANGES
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnt118
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015196
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015196
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7792374
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7792374
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20308
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20308
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021232
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.146621
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.146621
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.019
http://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00601.2003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.P362
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1999694
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1999694
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01617893
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316
ships on cardiovascular functioning. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33,
278 –290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02879910
Holt-Lunstad, J., Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., Olson-Cerny, C., & Nealey-
Moore, J. B. (2003). Social relationships and ambulatory blood pressure:
Structural and qualitative predictors of cardiovascular function during
everyday social interactions. Health Psychology, 22, 388 –397. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.388
Kamarck, T. W., Jennings, J. R., Debski, T. T., Glickman-Weiss, E.,
Johnson, P. S., Eddy, M. J., & Manuck, S. B. (1992). Reliable measures
of behaviorally-evoked cardiovascular reactivity from a PC-based test
battery: Results from student and community samples. Psychophysiol-
ogy, 29, 17–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02006.x
Labouvie-Vief, G. (2008). Dynamic integration theory: Emotion, cogni-
tion, and equilibrium in later life. In V. Bengtson, M. Silverstein, N.
Putney, & D. Gans (Eds.), Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 277–293).
New York, NY: Springer.
Nyklíček, I., Mommersteeg, P. M. C., Van Beugen, S., Ramakers, C., &
Van Boxtel, G. J. (2013). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and phys-
iological activity during acute stress: A randomized controlled trial.
Health Psychology, 32, 1110 –1113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032200
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (2006). Implicit
ambivalence from attitude change: An exploration of the PAST model.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 21– 41. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.21
Pillemer, K., Suitor, J. J., Mock, S. E., Sabir, M., Pardo, T. B., & Sechrist,
J. (2007). Capturing the complexity of intergenerational relations: Ex-
ploring ambivalence within later-life families. Journal of Social Issues,
63, 775–791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00536.x
Reblin, M., Uchino, B. N., & Smith, T. W. (2010). Provider and recipient
factors that may moderate the effectiveness of received support: Exam-
ining the effects of relationship quality and expectations for support on
behavioral and cardiovascular reactions. Journal of Behavioral Medi-
cine, 33, 423– 431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9270-z
Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events:
The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110,
67– 85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.67
Uchino, B. N., Birmingham, W., & Berg, C. A. (2010). Are older adults
less or more physiologically reactive? A meta-analysis of age-related
differences in cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory tasks. Journals of
Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65,
154 –162. http://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp127
Uchino, B. N., Bosch, J. A., Smith, T. W., Carlisle, M., Birmingham, W.,
Bowen, K. S., . . . O’Hartaigh, B. (2013). Relationships and cardiovas-
cular risk: Perceived spousal ambivalence in specific relationship con-
texts and its links to inflammation. Health Psychology, 32, 1067–1075.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033515
Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Bloor, L. E., & Campo, R. A. (2005).
Aging and cardiovascular reactivity to stress: Longitudinal evidence for
changes in stress reactivity. Psychology and Aging, 20, 134 –143. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.134
Uchino, B. N., Holt-Lunstad, J., Uno, D., & Flinders, J. B. (2001). Heter-
ogeneity in the social networks of young and older adults: Prediction of
mental health and cardiovascular reactivity during acute stress. Journal
of Behavioral Medicine, 24, 361–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:
1010634902498
Uchino, B. N., Ong, A., Queen, T. L., & Kent de Grey, R. G. (2016).
Theories of social support in health and aging. In R. Bengtson & V.
Settersten (Eds.), Handbook of theories of aging (3rd. ed.). New York,
NY: Springer.
Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., & Berg, C. A. (2014). Spousal relationship
quality and cardiovascular risk: Dyadic perceptions of relationship am-
bivalence are associated with coronary-artery calcification. Psychologi-
cal Science, 25, 1037–1042. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/09567
97613520015
van Gaalen, R. I., Dykstra, P., & Komter, A. E. (2010). Where is the exit?
Intergenerational ambivalence and relationship quality in high contact
ties. Journal of Aging Studies, 24, 105–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.jaging.2008.10.006
Received September 4, 2015
Revision received March 30, 2016
Accepted April 5, 2016 �
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
326 UCHINO, KENT DE GREY, AND CRONAN
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02879910
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.388
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.4.388
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02006.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032200
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.21
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.21
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00536.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-010-9270-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.67
http://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp127
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033515
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.134
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.134
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010634902498
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010634902498
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613520015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613520015
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2008.10.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaging.2008.10.006
The Quality of Social Networks Predicts Age-Related Changes in Cardiovascular Reactivity to Stress
Method
Participants
Procedure
Assessments
Social Relationships Index (SRI)
Cardiovascular measures
Statistical Analyses
Results
Sample Characteristics
Main Analyses
Discussion
References
Journals/journal 8
BRIEF REPORTS
Aging and Social Satisfaction: Offsetting Positive and Negative Effects
William von Hippel
University of Queensland
Julie D. Henry and Diana Matovic
University of New South Wales
Social satisfaction in late adulthood originates from competing sources. Older adults tend to be more
positive and less negative than younger adults, but social contact and working memory often decrease
with age, both of which might limit older adults’ social functioning. In the current study of younger and
older adults, these socially facilitative vs. socially debilitative changes were found to underlie stasis in
social satisfaction. These findings show that the lack of an overall effect for age can mask competing
changes in social functioning in late adulthood, as the sources of social satisfaction might change even
if the outcome does not.
Keywords: social satisfaction, social activities, working memory, hassles, uplifts
Social satisfaction in late adulthood originates from competing
sources. On the one hand, life experiences and the wisdom that
accompanies them can lead to more harmonious social relation-
ships. Thus, socioemotional models of aging propose that social
functioning in late adulthood does not follow the same course of
decline as cognitive and biological aging (Antonucci, 2001;
Charles & Carstensen, 2007). Evidence also suggests that older
adults are biased toward positive information (Carstensen &
Mikels, 2005; Gross et al., 1997; but see Grühn, Smith, & Baltes,
2005). This bias might reflect motivational shifts driven by time
perspective that lead to prioritization of emotion-related goals
(Charles & Carstensen, 2007). This bias might also be a mecha-
nism that explains older adults’ generally more positive approach
to conflict resolution (Blanchard-Fields, 2007; Carstensen, Gott-
man, & Levenson, 1995) as well as their reports of fewer unde-
sirable daily events (Almeida & Horn, 2004) and less interpersonal
tension (Birditt, Fingerman, & Almeida, 2005).
Competing with these facilitative effects, however, are other
changes in late adulthood that are likely to have a detrimental
impact on social functioning and on consequent social satisfaction.
First and foremost, although older adults appear to intentionally
cull their peripheral social partners to focus on closer and more
meaningful relationships (Charles & Carstensen, 2007; Lansford,
Sherman, & Antonucci, 1998), they also suffer unintended social
losses brought about by poor health, retirement, mobility con-
straints, widowhood, and so on, that can lead to reduced social
satisfaction (Jang, Mortimer, Haley, & Borenstein Graves, 2004;
Pinquart & Sörensen, 2001; van Tilburg, 1998). Thus, although
intentional reductions in social network size are theorized not to
cause decreased social satisfaction, unintended reductions in social
contact have the potential to do so.
Compounding the effects of reduced social contact, cognitive
losses associated with normal adult aging might also affect older
adults’ social functioning by limiting their ability to negotiate
complex social relationships. Indeed, social reasoning skills im-
pose substantial demands on various aspects of cognitive function-
ing, such as mental flexibility and inhibitory control (German &
Hehman, 2006), and there is considerable evidence that links
cognitive decline to reduced social functioning in older adulthood.
For example, cognitive deficits have been linked to socially insen-
sitive behaviors in the context of normal adult aging, such as
increased difficulty in taking another’s perspective (Bailey &
Henry, in press), off-target and verbose speech (Pushkar et al.,
2000), and prejudicial and other socially inappropriate comments
(von Hippel & Dunlop, 2005; von Hippel, Silver, & Lynch, 2000).
Whether older adults show increased or decreased social satis-
faction, or if indeed they show any change at all, depends on the
relative magnitude of these socially facilitating versus socially
debilitating changes. This lack of reliable change might mask
important and competing changes in social functioning in late
adulthood, as the sources of social satisfaction might change even
if the outcome does not (cf. Kunzmann, Little, & Smith, 2000).
The goal of this article is to test this possibility.
In service of this goal, we examined several social and cognitive
factors that might account for competing changes in social satis-
faction in later life. To assess social factors that might impact
social satisfaction, we asked older and younger adults to indicate
how much time they spend alone and how frequently they engage
in various social activities. To assess cognitive factors that might
impact social satisfaction, we asked older and younger adults to
complete measures of working memory and inhibitory control.
Based on previous research, we expected older adults to spend
more time alone, to engage in fewer social activities, and to have
decreased working memory capacity, and we expected these age
differences to have a negative impact on social satisfaction. We
also expected older adults to show increased cognitive disinhibi-
tion compared to younger adults, but it was unclear whether this
William von Hippel, School of Psychology, University of Queensland,
St. Lucia, Australia; Julie D. Henry and Diana Matovic, School of Psy-
chology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.
This research was supported by Australian Research Council Grant
DP0774268.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to William
von Hippel, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia
QLD 4072, Australia. E-mail: billvh@psy.uq.edu.au
Psychology and Aging Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 23, No. 2, 435– 439 0882-7974/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.435
435
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
effect would impact social satisfaction. On one hand, poorer cognitive
inhibition can be associated with social disinhibition (see Pushkar et
al., 2000; von Hippel & Dunlop, 2005; von Hippel & Gonsalkorale,
2005; von Hippel et al., 2000) and thus might predict reduced social
satisfaction if social disinhibition is causing relationship problems for
older adults. On the other hand, inhibitory control is unlikely to play
as broad of a role in social functioning as does working memory (see
Feldman Barrett, Tugade, & Engle, 2004) and by itself may not be a
significant predictor of social satisfaction.
On the basis of the age-related positivity effect (Carstensen &
Mikels, 2005), however, we expected older adults to experience
their social events as more positive and less negative than younger
adults. As a consequence, once the suppressing effects of time
alone, reduced social activity levels, and reduced working memory
were controlled, we expected this increased positivity to lead older
adults to experience greater social satisfaction than younger adults.
Thus, we expected older adults who did not spend a great deal of
time alone, who maintained high social activity levels, and who
retained good working memory to experience greater social satis-
faction than younger adults. Furthermore, if increased emphasis on
the positive (or decreased emphasis on the negative; Grühn et al.,
2005) leads to enhanced social satisfaction, then older adults
should experience more uplifts and fewer hassles from their daily
activities and relationships than should younger adults. This effect,
in turn, should mediate the increased residual social satisfaction
that emerges with age after controlling for time alone, decreased
social activity levels, and reduced working memory.
Method
Participants
Thirty-eight younger adults aged 18 –30 years (M � 23.8, SD �
3.2; 26 female) and 40 older adults aged 66 –91 years (M � 74.4,
SD � 7.5; 25 female) participated in this study. Participants were
paid $20 Australian (�US$16). Older and younger adults had
similar levels of education, with average responses from both
groups that ranged between some university study and completed
university degree, �2(7, N � 78) � 7.23, p � .40. Participants
were community-dwelling, and older and younger adults were
recruited in a similar manner from social clubs, apartment com-
plexes, and churches in the Sydney metropolitan area. All older
adults had normal mental status as indexed by the Mini-Mental
State Exam (MMSE; range � 27–30, M � 29.40, SD � 0.87;
Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975).
Procedure
Participants first completed a brief demographic form on which
they indicated their age, gender, and education level, and then they
completed several measures unrelated to the current research.
Next, because measures of working memory can be fatiguing and
sometimes disheartening for older participants and thus these
measures have the potential to influence subsequent measures,
participants completed either a working memory measure followed
by a Hassles and Uplifts Scale (DeLongis, Folkman, & Lazarus,
1988) followed by a Stroop test or a Stroop test followed by a
Hassles and Uplifts Scale followed by a working memory measure.
The working memory measure was adopted from Daneman and
Carpenter (1980). The measure required participants to read a
series of sentences aloud and then to recall the last word of each
sentence. Participants began by reading three sets of two sentences
each, followed by three sets of three sentences, and they worked
their way up to three sets of five sentences. Working memory was
scored as the total number of final words that participants recalled,
summed across the series of different lengths. A shortened form of
the Hassles and Uplifts Scale was used to assess the degree to
which daily experiences and relationships were experienced as
hassles and uplifts. Participants rated a series of 43 items (e.g.,
family-related obligations, friends, and recreation) on the degree to
which the items were experienced as hassles and uplifts over the
last week, with responses to both questions provided on 4-point
scales ranging from not at all (0) to a great deal (3). Participants
were also given the option to indicate that an item was not
applicable. For the Stroop test, participants first named aloud the
ink colors of 42 color blocks and then named aloud the ink colors
of 42 color words whose letters were printed in a color inconsistent
with most of the word meanings (e.g., red printed in green ink).
The reading times for the page of 42 color blocks and the page of
42 color words were measured with a stopwatch, and Stroop
Color–Word Interference scores were computed as the difference
in time taken to read color words and color blocks divided by the
time taken to read color blocks.
Next, participants were asked to indicate how many hours each
day they spend alone. They then completed a slightly modified
version of the Prosocial subscale of the Social Functioning Scale
(SFS–P; Birchwood, Smith, Cochrane, Wetton, & Copestake,
1990). The standard version of this measure assesses level of
participation in 22 different social activities (e.g., being visited
by/visiting relatives, eating out), but in the present study a 23rd
activity (volunteering) was added, as this was considered to be a
social activity in which older adults were particularly likely to be
engaged. Participants were asked to provide an estimate of the
frequency with which they had engaged in each activity in the last
month. The SFS–P assesses involvement in activities that one
generally does in the company of others (e.g., being visited by or
visiting relatives, eating out), as opposed to activities one may be
more likely to do alone (e.g., reading, gardening, or knitting).
Responses to the 23 items were averaged to form a total score, with
higher scores indicative of more active social functioning—the
range in the current sample was 0.23 to 12.52. After participants
completed the SFS–P, they responded to the single item “How
satisfied are you with your social life?” on a scale that ranged from
0 (very dissatisfied) to 10 (very satisfied).
Results
The first step in the analyses was to examine age differences in
the primary measures to assess whether the expected age differ-
ences emerged. Consistent with prior research, a significant mul-
tivariate analysis of variance, F(5, 70) � 15.72, p � .001, followed
up by univariate analyses of variance revealed that older adults
spent more hours per day alone (M � 7.04, SD � 4.55) than did
younger adults (M � 3.36, SD � 2.30), F(1, 75) � 19.95, MSE �
13.09, p � .001. The scores of older adults on the SFS–P, � � .78,
indicated that they engaged in fewer social activities during the
prior month (M � 1.99, SD � 1.42) than did younger adults (M �
4.04, SD � 2.46), F(1, 76) � 20.50, MSE � 3.99, p � .001. Older
adults showed greater Stroop interference (M � 1.17, SD � 0.45)
than did younger adults (M � 0.82, SD � 0.30), F(1, 75) � 15.53,
436 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
MSE � 0.15, p � .001, and older adults recalled fewer of the
words on the working memory task (M � 27.96, SD � 6.53) than
did younger adults (M � 33.32, SD � 4.19), F(1, 76) � 18.39,
MSE � 30.42, p � .001. Despite these age-related deficits, older
adults showed high levels of social satisfaction (M � 7.85, SD �
1.99) that were comparable to those of younger adults, (M � 7.78,
SD � 1.98), F(1, 76) � 0.02, MSE � 3.93, p � .85.
The Hassles and Uplifts Scale was then examined to see if
younger and older adults responded to the same items with the
same frequencies. This analysis revealed that some of the items
were differentially likely to be chosen as not applicable by older or
younger adults (e.g., items that referred to children were not
applicable to most younger adults, whereas items that referred to
parents were not applicable to most older adults). To ensure that
the analyses were not unduly influenced by age differences in the
likelihood of a participant’s responses to particular items, we
created subscales that included only the 23 items that older and
younger adults were equally—and greater than 75%—likely to
respond to the item, � � .70 for hassles, � � .82 for uplifts.
Analyses of these subscales revealed that older adults reported
fewer hassles (M � 0.55, SD � 0.38) than did younger adults
(M � 0.88, SD � 0.41), F(1, 76) � 13.63, MSE � 0.16, p � .001;
but older adults reported no differences in uplifts (M � 1.54, SD �
0.43) compared to younger adults (M � 1.46, SD � 0.49), F(1,
76) � 0.64, MSE � 0.21, p � .40. It should be noted that analyses
of the remaining 20 items for which there were differential re-
sponses by age group revealed that older adults again reported
fewer hassles, (M � 0.29, SD � 0.33), than did younger adults
(M � 0.92, SD � 0.51), F(1, 76) � 42.21, MSE � 0.18, p � .001,
and in this case greater uplifts, (M � 1.70, SD � 0.56) than
younger adults (M � 1.24, SD � 0.53), F(1, 76) � 14.25, MSE �
0.30, p � .001.
The analyses thus far had suggested that older adults were
neither more nor less satisfied with their social relationships than
were younger adults, but as noted above, the absence of an age
effect might mask underlying competing effects. That is, a sup-
pression effect might have emerged whereby age led to positive
effects on social satisfaction via one mechanism and to negative
effects on social satisfaction via a different mechanism. To test this
possibility, we estimated a regression-based causal model follow-
ing the procedures outlined in Baron and Kenny (1986). The
correlation coefficients for all of the variables in the model, and
also for education and MMSE, are presented in Table 1.
In the first step of the regression analysis, social satisfaction was
regressed on age and gender (with male coded as 0 and female
coded as 1) because social satisfaction correlated with gender (see
Table 1).1 As shown in Figure 1, and consistent with the analysis
of variance results presented above, age had no direct effect on
social satisfaction. In the second step of the analysis, social satis-
faction was regressed on age, time spent alone, social activity
levels, working memory, and Stroop performance. After control-
ling for these four factors, age exerted a substantial and positive
direct effect on social satisfaction (see Figure 1). In the third step
of the analysis, self-reported hassles and uplifts were included as
additional mediators between age and social satisfaction (i.e.,
social satisfaction was regressed on age, time spent alone, social
activity levels, working memory, Stroop performance, and self-
reported hassles and uplifts). This analysis revealed that self-
reported uplifts but not hassles partially mediated the residual
effect of age on social satisfaction. In the final step of the analysis,
social activity levels were included as a mediator between age and
self-reported hassles and uplifts (i.e., hassles and uplifts were
regressed on age and social activity levels). This analysis revealed
the presence of an additional suppression effect, whereby the direct
effect of age on self-reported uplifts was not significant until social
activity levels were included in the analysis. Thus, it seems that
decreased social activity levels with age also accounted for the fact
that no age differences emerged in self-reported uplifts. Older
adults who did not experience reduced social activity levels re-
ported more uplifts than younger adults (see Figure 1).
To determine whether these mediational pathways were signif-
icant, we computed indirect effects from unstandardized regression
weights with 10,000 bootstrap resamples to obtain accurate con-
fidence limits by following the syntax provided by Preacher and
Hayes (2007). Three sets of indirect effects and associated confi-
dence intervals were computed in analyses that controlled for the
effect of gender on social satisfaction. First, analyses revealed that
increased time alone, decreased social activity levels, and de-
1 The results are essentially unchanged when gender is not included in
the analyses.
Table 1
Correlations Between Demographic Variables and Indices of Psychosocial Functioning
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Age —
2. Time alone .43*** —
3. SFS–P �.43*** �.39*** —
4. Working memory �.49*** �.28* .05 —
5. Stroop .47*** .19 �.09 �.43*** —
6. Uplifts .10 .07 .41*** �.16 .00 —
7. Hassles �.42*** �.25* .33** .23* �.26* .17 —
8. MMSE (n � 40) �.20 �.19 �.01 .55*** �.02 �.26 .23 —
9. Gender �.12 �.02 �.03 .03 �.16 .07 .10 .13 —
10. Education .15 .00 .05 .17 �.14 .05 .08 .09 �.14 —
11. Social satisfaction .06 �.28* .26* .11 �.01 .40*** .09 .15 .30** �.01 —
Note. Correlations of gender and education were calculated with Spearman’s rho. All other correlations were calculated with Pearson’s r. SFS–P � Social
Functioning Scale—Prosocial; Stroop � Stroop Color–Word Interference Test; MMSE � Mini-Mental State Exam.
* p � .05. ** p � .01. *** p � .001.
437BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
creased working memory all significantly mediated the negative
effect of aging on social satisfaction (indirect effect via time
alone � �.008, SE � .004, 95% CI � �.018, �.001; indirect
effect via social activity levels � �.011, SE � .004, 95% CI �
�.022, �.004; indirect effect via working memory � �.009,
SE � .005, 95% CI � �.022, �.001). Consistent with the lack of
a relationship between the Stroop and social satisfaction, Stroop
performance was not a significant mediator in this analysis (indi-
rect effect � �.001, SE � .004, 95% CI � �.009, .008). Second,
analyses revealed that when self-reported uplifts and hassles were
added to the model, uplifts significantly mediated the residual
positive effect of aging on social satisfaction (indirect effect �
.010, SE � .006, 95% CI � .001, .023), but hassles did not
(indirect effect � .000, SE � .002, 95% CI � �.003, .006). Third,
analyses revealed that uplifts also significantly mediated the pos-
itive effect of social activity levels on social satisfaction (indirect
effect � .117, SE � .060, 95% CI � .016, .251). Finally, when
these mediational analyses were conducted with the full Hassles
and Uplifts Scale or with only those items in the Hassles and
Uplifts Scale that were explicitly social in nature, the results were
equivalent to those depicted in Figure 1.
Discussion
The results of this study provide a picture of aging in which
social costs are offset by social gains. A community-based sample
of older adults showed neither greater nor lesser social satisfaction
than a community-based sample of younger adults. Additionally,
older adults in this sample reported neither greater nor fewer
uplifts from the experiences that they had in common with younger
adults. Yet this apparent stability in social experience masked
underlying counter-currents whereby age-related losses suppressed
the effect of age-related gains. On the loss ledger, older adults
spent more time alone, engaged in fewer social activities, and had
poorer working memory than did younger adults. All three of these
factors played a mediating role in decreased social satisfaction among
older adults. When these factors were included in the model, aging
was associated with residual increases in social satisfaction and self-
reported uplifts. Furthermore, the residual increase in social satisfac-
tion was itself partially mediated by the residual increase in uplifts.
That is, once age-related decreases in social activity levels were
controlled, older adults reported greater social satisfaction than did
younger adults, which was partially accounted for by the degree to
which older adults also reported increased uplifts.
Results also revealed that older adults considered their daily
activities to be less of a hassle than did younger adults. This age
effect on self-reported hassles appears to be of lesser importance
than the effect on uplifts, however, as uplifts and not hassles
played a mediating role in increased residual social satisfaction
among older adults. Thus, it seems to be the case that increased
positivity but not decreased negativity played a role in maintaining
social satisfaction in late adulthood. The fact that age had a
significant residual effect on social satisfaction after self-reported
hassles and uplifts had been accounted for, however, suggests that
other age-related changes also lead to increased social satisfaction.
One possibility is that other aspects of increased positivity and
decreased negativity are important determinants of increased so-
cial satisfaction with age, as it is highly unlikely that hassles and
uplifts capture the full experience of increased positivity and
decreased negativity with age. Additionally, a variety of other
factors might be at play in the residual increase in social satisfac-
tion that emerges in late adulthood. For example, increased social
satisfaction might derive from changes in anger management strat-
egies (Phillips, Henry, Hosie, & Milne, 2006, in press) or perhaps
from older adults’ tendency to make increased use of reappraisal to
regulate their emotions (Gross et al., 1997; John & Gross, 2004).
Future research might attempt to gain a more complete picture of
how all of these factors contribute simultaneously to change and
stability in social satisfaction across the lifespan.
The fact that the current study is cross-sectional and reliant on
self-report limits the causal conclusions that can be drawn. Clearly,
longitudinal work with a larger sample and multiple behavioral or
physiological indicators would represent an important supplement
Age
Time
Alone
Social
Activities
Working
Memory
Satisfac w/
Social Life
-.32**
.25*
.43**
-.42**
-.49***
.48**
.35**
Gender
.27**
.09
Panel A
Age
Time
Alone
Social
Activities
Working
Memory
Hassles
Uplifts
Satisfac w/
Social Life.35*
-.32**
.16
.31*
.25*
-.03
.43**
-.42**
-.49***
.34**
.55***
-.42***
.10
Gender
.27**
Panel B
Figure 1. Mediational models of the effect of age on time spent alone,
social activity levels, working memory, hassles, uplifts, and social satis-
faction are depicted. Path coefficients are standardized beta weights. In
Panel A, two direct effects of age on social satisfaction are included in the
model. The coefficient above the path is from a model that did not include
any mediators. The coefficient below the path is from a model that included
time spent alone, social activity levels, working memory, and Stroop
performance as mediators. Stroop performance is not depicted because it
was not a significant mediator. In Panel B, two direct effects are included
in the model for the impact of age on uplifts. The coefficient above the path
is from a model that did not include social activity levels as a mediator. The
coefficient below the path is from a model that included social activity as
a mediator. The nonsignificant path from social activity levels to hassles is
not depicted for the sake of expositional clarity. Satisfac w/ � satisfaction
with. * p � .05. ** p � .01. *** p � .001.
438 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
to the current findings. Additionally, it should be noted that the
order of the questions might have increased the impact of time
spent alone and social activity levels on social satisfaction. That is,
because these items were assessed directly before social satisfac-
tion was measured, their momentary accessibility might have
increased their impact on this overall judgment (Strack, Martin, &
Schwarz, 1988). Although future research should counterbalance
these questions to ensure that time spent alone and social activity
levels predict social satisfaction independent of question order, it
should be noted that prior research has found that time spent alone
and social activity levels are related to various indices of well-
being that include social and life satisfaction (Hawkley et al.,
2008; Iecovich et al., 2004; Pinquart & Sörensen, 2001, 2003;
Routasalo, Savikko, Tilvis, Strandberg, & Pitkälä, 2006).
References
Almeida, D. M., & Horn, M. C. (2004). Is daily life more stressful during
middle adulthood? In C. D. Ryff & R. C. Kessler (Eds.), A portrait of
midlife in the United States (pp. 425– 451). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Antonucci, T. C. (2001). Social relations: An examination of social net-
works, social support, and sense of control. In J. E. Birren & K. W.
Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp.
427– 453). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bailey, P. E. & Henry, J. D. (in press). Growing less empathic with age:
Disinhibition of the self-perspective. Journal of Gerontology: Psycho-
logical Sciences.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51, 1173–1182.
Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Cochrane, R., Wetton, S., & Copestake, S.
(1990). The Social Functioning Scale: The development and validation
of a new scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 853– 859.
Birditt, K. S., Fingerman, K. L., & Almeida, D. M. (2005). Age differences
in exposure and reactions to interpersonal tensions: A daily diary study.
Psychology and Aging, 20, 330 –340.
Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An
adult developmental perspective. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 16, 26 –31.
Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotional
behavior in long-term marriage. Psychology and Aging, 10, 140 –149.
Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion
and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 14, 117–121.
Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2007). Emotion regulation and aging.
In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 307–327).
New York: Guilford Press.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working
memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,
19, 450 – 466.
DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily
stress on health and mood: Psychological and social resources as medi-
ators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 486 – 495.
Feldman Barrett, L., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual
differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of
mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553–573.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental
State: A practical method for grading cognitive state of patients for the
clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 12, 189 –198.
German, T. P., & Hehman, J. A. (2006). Representational and executive
selection resources in ‘theory of mind’: Evidence from compromised
belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition, 101, 129 –152.
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Skorpen, C. G., &
Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and
control. Psychology and Aging, 12, 590 –599.
Grühn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring
memory for positive material: Evidence from a heterogeneity–
homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychology
and Aging, 20, 579 –588.
Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, R. A., &
Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Who are the lonely people II: The Chicago health,
aging, and social relations study. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Iecovich, E., Barasch, M., Mirsky, J., Kaufman, R., Avgar, A., & Kol-
Fogelson, A. (2004). Social support networks and loneliness among
elderly Jews in Russia and Ukraine. Journal of Marriage and Family,
66, 306 –317.
Jang, Y., Mortimer, J. A., Haley, W. E., & Borenstein Graves, A. R.
(2004). The role of social engagement in life satisfaction: Its significance
among older individuals with disease and disability. Journal of Applied
Gerontology, 23, 266 –278.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regu-
lation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span de-
velopment. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334.
Kunzmann, U., Little, T. D., & Smith, J. (2000). Is age-related stability of
subjective well-being a paradox? Cross-sectional and longitudinal evi-
dence from the Berlin aging study. Psychology and Aging, 15, 511–526.
Lansford, J. E., Sherman, A. M., & Antonucci, T. C. (1998). Satisfaction
with social networks: An examination of socioemotional selectivity
theory across cohorts. Psychology and Aging, 13, 544 –552.
Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., Hosie, J., & Milne, A. B. (2006). Age, anger
regulation and well-being. Aging & Mental Health, 10, 250 –256.
Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., Hosie, J. A., & Milne, A. B. (in press). Adult
aging and the experience, expression and regulation of negative affect.
Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences.
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2001). Influences of loneliness in older adults:
A meta-analysis. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23, 245–266.
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2003). Risk factors for loneliness in adulthood
and old age: A meta-analysis. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in Psychol-
ogy Research, Volume 19 (pp. 111–143). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Asymptotic and resampling
strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in simple and
multiple mediator models. Manuscript under review.
Pushkar, D., Basevitz, P., Arbuckle, T., Nohara-LeClair, M., Lapidus, S.,
& Peled, M. (2000). Social behavior and off-target verbosity in elderly
people. Psychology and Aging, 15, 361–374.
Routasalo, P. E., Savikko, N., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., & Pitkälä,
K. H. (2006). Social contacts and their relationship to loneliness among
aged people: A population-based study. Gerontology, 52, 181–187.
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communi-
cation: Social determinants of information use in judgments of life
satisfaction. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 429 – 442.
van Tilburg, T. (1998, November). Losing and gaining in old age: Changes
in personal network size and social support in a four-year longitudinal
study. Journal of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and
Social Sciences, 53B, S313–S323.
von Hippel, W., & Dunlop, S. M. (2005). Aging, inhibition, and social
inappropriateness. Psychology and Aging, 20, 519 –523.
von Hippel, W., & Gonsalkorale, K. (2005). “That is bloody revolting!”
Inhibitory control of thoughts better left unsaid. Psychological Science,
16, 497–500.
von Hippel, W., Silver, L. A., & Lynch, M. E. (2000). Stereotyping against
your will: The role of inhibitory ability in stereotyping and prejudice among
the elderly. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 523–532.
Received June 20, 2007
Revision received January 11, 2008
Accepted January 21, 2008 �
439BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
Journals/journal 9
Psychology and Aging
I W T . V o f . r " - '12. No. 3. 433-443
Copyright 1997 by the Americ rt Psychological Association, Inc.
0882-7974/97/S3.0C
Everyday Functioning and Successful Aging: The Impact of Resources
Margret M. Baltes and Frieder R. Lang
Free University Berlin
The goal of this article is to examine differential aging in everyday functioning between resource-
rich and resource-poor older adults. Four groups of older adults were identified on the basis of 2
distinct resource factors: a Sensorimotor-Cognitive factor and a Social-Personality factor. The
resource-richest group consisted of those participants who were above the median in both factors;
those falling below the median in both factors comprised the resource-poorest group; and 2 additional
groups consisted of older adults who were above the median in either 1 of the 2 factors. At the level
of mean differences, the 4 groups differed in the length of the waking day, the.variability in activities,
the frequency of intellectual-cultural and social-relational activities, and resting times. Considering
age differences there are more and larger negative age effects in the resource-poorest group than in
the resource-richest one. The metamodel of selective optimization with compensation is used to
interpret the findings.
Effective functioning in everyday life is a developmental task
of old age, yielding autonomy and independent living. Losses
in different life domains threaten everyday functioning and, thus,
require adaptation. Effective mastery of this developmental task
is an integral part of successful aging (M. Baltes & Carstensen,
1996; P. Baltes & M. Baltes, 1990; Marsiske, Lang, Baltes, &
Baltes, 1995). How well individuals can adapt to functional
loss depends on the availability of resources in the sensorimotor,
cognitive, personality, and social domains of functioning. This
is not to say that individuals who are rich in resources will not
experience functional loss at all, but they will begin with loss
experiences at higher levels of functioning (see Lindenberger &
The present study is part of the Berlin Aging Study (BASE), which
has been conducted by the interdisciplinary working group Aging and
Societal Development (AGE) in cooperation with institutes and research
units of the Free University Berlin, the Humboldt University of Berlin,
and the Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education in
Berlin, Germany. AGE was constituted in 1987 by the former Academy
of Sciences of Berlin (West) and has been continued since 1994 by the
Berlin-Brandenburg Academy of Sciences (BBAW). BASE was finan-
cially supported between 1989 and 1991 by the German Ministry of
Research and Technology (No. TA O i l + 13 TA O i l / A ) ; since 1992
financial support has been received from the German Ministry of Family,
Seniors, Women, and Youth. Primary responsibility for BASE is shared
by Paul B. Baltes, Karl Ulrich Mayer (Max Planck Institute for Human
Development and Education), Hanfried Helmchen (Free University
Berlin), and Elisabeth Steinhagen-Thiessen (Humboldt University Ber-
lin). This steering committee is joined by a central project coordinator,
R. Nuthmann (Max Planck Institute for Human Development and
Education).
We thank Fredda Blanchard-Fields, Chris Hertzog, Ulman Lindenber-
ger, Heiner Meier, and Michael Marsiske for their valuable comments
on a version of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to either
Margret M. Baltes or Frieder R. Lang, Research Unit Psychological
Gerontology, Department of Gerontopsychiatry, Free University Berlin,
Nussbaumallee 38, 14050 Berlin, Germany. Electronic mail may be sent
via Internet to baltesma@zedat.fu-berlin.de.
Baltes, 1997). In other words, the aging process might reflect
the same slope of differences or decline in functioning, but
functional deterioration will manifest itself later because still-
available resources may be used to compensate and optimize
with or select from in the face of real or anticipated losses.
Consequently, negative aging effects might be less pronounced
in individuals who are rich in resources. In summary, we argue
that sensorimotor, cognitive, personality, and social resources
have imminent importance for successful aging because they
facilitate the interplay between three adaptive processes: selec-
tion, compensation, and optimization; the more resources a per-
son has, the easier it is to anticipate, confront, and adapt to
aging losses.
Typically, in gerontology, aging theories have put forth differ-
ent criteria or outcomes that characterize and indicate successful
aging (M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; P. Baltes & M. Baltes,
1990). For instance, subjective well-being or life satisfaction
has played a key role as an indicator of successful aging (e.g.,
Rowe & Kahn, 1987). This criterion for evaluating modes of
aging well was most notably applied in the disengagement-
activity debate. The disengagement theory (Gumming & Henry,
1961) posits a gradual and mutual withdrawal between the indi-
vidual and society in old age that represents a symbolic prepara-
tion for death. This model views success as acceptance of and
reconciliation with the loss of power endemic in old age. Some-
what similarly, Buhler's (1933) five-stage model views success-
ful aging as the realization and acceptance of age-related physi-
cal limitations and the reconciliation of conflicting goals. In
contrast, activity theory (Havighurst & Albrecht, 1953; Maddox,
1963) argues that social withdrawal in old age is not natural,
but that it is imposed by traditional societal practices such as
retirement and intractable age-related problems, like poor
health. According to activity theory, successful aging demands
the maintenance of activity, replacement of lost roles with new
ones, and involvement in society and interpersonal relationships.
The more recent continuity theory (Atchley, 1989) suggests that
a continuation of previous life patterns best ensures adaptation
and success in late life.
433
434 BALTES AND LANG
Another set of criteria has sprung from the emphasis on per-
sonality growth. In this sense, for Jung (1931,1934), successful
aging involves life review, psychological interiorization, and
expansion beyond gender constraints toward full humanity and
wisdom. Erikson's stage model (1959, 1968, 1982; Erikson,
Erikson, & Kivnick, 1986), arguably the most popular model
of adult personality development, suggests that the task of old
age is acceptance and resolution of one's life. Described as a
crisis between ego integrity and despair, the challenge in old
age is to accept oneself and one's deeds, both good and bad, in
order to obtain psychological peace and acceptance of death.
Theoretical models of positive mental health have also contrib-
uted to the discussion of successful aging criteria. For example,
models of self-actualization (Maslow, 1968), the healthy per-
sonality (Jahoda, 1958), and the trusting, meaningful life (Rog-
ers, 1961) all tacitly suggest that successful aging is the realiza-
tion of one's full human potential. In this context of personality
and positive mental health models, the more recent work by
Ryff (1982, 1989a, 1989b) is perhaps the most ambitious. Ryff
has proposed an integrative model of successful aging that is
based on developmental, clinical, and mental health criteria. Her
model includes six dimensions of positive functioning: self-
acceptance, positive relations with others, autonomy, environ-
mental mastery, purpose in life, and personal growth.
In summary, when considering these criteria of successful
aging, most require a high level of functioning that in turn
requires the availability of resources in the biological, psycho-
logical, and the social domains of functioning. In fact, although
not a criterion in the above-indicated models, cognitive function-
ing as an important general-resource factor supporting success-
ful aging has been an argument in life span psychology. Specifi-
cally, recent data point to the importance of sensorimotor func-
tioning as a resource (Lindenberger & Baltes, 1994). In
conclusion, both objective and subjective resources help main-
tain and optimize functioning and should cover as broad a con-
tent base as possible.
In this sense, the purpose of the present article is to examine
the interrelationship of a number of resources necessary for the
attainment of high functioning, such as sensorimotor, cognitive,
personality, and social resources, their effect on everyday func-
tioning as well as their relationship to negative age differences
in everyday functioning. Our hypothesis states that participants
scoring high on resources will show less age differences in
everyday functioning when compared with people who score
low. This means that successful aging will manifest itself in the
reduction of the magnitude of cross-sectional age differences in
everyday functioning. Consequently, the question to be answered
in this article is the following: Are age differences in everyday
functioning less pronounced in older people who have many
resources (score high on a variety of resource indicators) avail-
able? To answer this question, first, resource-rich versus re-
source-poor older adults are identified on a total of 11 indicators
from four domains of resources; namely, sensorimotor, cogni-
tive, personality, and social resources. Second, to examine the
effects of these resources on everyday functioning, 21 indicators
of everyday functioning, which were assessed by the activities
reported in a Yesterday Interview (YI) by the older participants,
are inspected. Third, we analyze age differences in these re-
source effects on everyday functioning. Existing age-differential
effects are discussed using the metamodel of selective optimiza-
tion with compensation (M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; P. B.
Baltes, 1993; P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes, 1990; Marsiske et
al., 1995).
Method
Participants
In the Berlin Aging Study (BASE), 516 community-dwelling and
institutionalized West Berlin residents age 70-103 years (M = 84.9
years, SD = 8.7) took part in an intensive 14-session interview schedule,
the content of which addressed four disciplines: psychology, sociology,
psychiatry, and internal medicine (see P. B. Baltes, Mayer, Helmchen, &
Steinhagen-Thiessen, 1993). The sessions were distributed over 4.5
months (SD = 1.9). Participants were identified through probability
sampling from the local registration office (in Germany each citizen
must he registered) and stratified by age and sex. Twenty-seven percent
of those contacted took part in all 14 sessions of BASE. In an analysis
of selectivity and representativity of this final BASE sample compared
with the original sample of 1,908 participants, Lindenberger, Gilberg,
Potter, Little, and Baltes (1996) did not find any substantial evidence
for sample biases. However, there was indication that the sample of 516
had a lower mortality rate after 1 year of testing, 5% as compared with
14% in the general population, and all other variables inspected exhibited
the expected, albeit nonsignificant, direction in the differences (see Lin-
denberger et al., 1996). There was also no indication that covariances
and variances of variables in the final sample differed from a sample
of participants who have completed an intake assessment of basic vari-
ables from all disciplines (i.e., 49% of the parent sample; see Lindenber-
ger et al., 1996).
Of the 516 participants, 86% lived in the community and 14% lived
in long-term care institutions (i.e., sheltered housing, nursing homes,
and hospitals for the chronically ill), and 21% were diagnosed with
dementia according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (3rd ed.. rev.; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) crite-
ria. Twenty-nine percent of the participants were married, 55% were
widowed, and 15% were divorced or have never been married. On the
average, participants had 10.8 years of education (SD - 2.4): 54% had
8 to 10 years, 40% had between 10 and 13 years, and 7% had more
than 13 years.
Measures
Everyday functioning. The YI (Moss & Lawton. 1982; see also M.
Baltes, Wahl, & Schmid-Furstoss, 1990), an instrument that provides a
minute-to-minute reconstruction of the sequence, duration, frequency,
the geographical and social context of activities engaged in during the
day preceding the interview, was used to assess everyday functioning.
The YI was part of the seventh session in the intensive data protocol
and was conducted by trained interviewers. The interviewer asks the
participant first to recount all activities in the sequence of their occur-
rence during the preceding day from waking up to falling asleep. Once
activities are recounted, the interviewer starts anew with the first one
reported and asks for the duration of each activity mentioned before
including where and with whom they occurred. All reported activities
were recorded as verbatim as much as possible. Trained coders coded
these activities into 44 activity codes (see M. M. Baltes, Maas. Wilms, &
Borchelt, 1996). Kappa values of intercoder reliability were above .80
(see M. M. Baltes, Mayr, Borchelt, Maas, & Wilms, 1993). On average,
participants reported 28 (SD — 7.3) activities during an average waking
day of 968.5 rain (SD = 109.7).
In the present article these 44 activity categories were summarized
into eight classes of activities differentiating between self-care or basic
EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING AND SUCCESSFUL AGING 435
activities, those related to household chores, social engagement, physical
leisure, intellectual-cultural leisure, television viewing, and resting and
sleeping. Table 1 presents these eight activity classes. Information about
the social context was classified along 3 categories into time spent
alone, time spent with family members, and time spent with others.
Furthermore, we included the variability or variety in activities indicating
how many different activity types the older participants engaged in and
the length of the waking day as additional indicators of everyday func-
tioning. In all, a total of 21 parameters of everyday functioning was
examined.
There is information about satisfactory reliability measures from Law-
ton and colleagues (Moss & Lawton, 1982). The YI appears to be
robust; that is, participants with diagnosis of mild dementia are able to
reconstruct their previous day at least on the basis efface validity. Only.
31 or 6% of interviews had to be excluded because of too many missing
values. Of these 31 people, 27 (87%) had a diagnosis of dementia. This
attrition in the sample explains why all subsequent analyses and tables
contain a sample of 485 instead of 516.
Resources indicators. For biological resources we used sensorimotor
indicators instead of more direct health indicators such as medical diag-
noses because of two reasons. First, there is a high interrelation between
sensorimotor functioning and physical health in BASE (see Steinhagen-
Thiessen & Borchelt, 1996). Second, sensorimotor functioning has been
shown in BASE to have a significant effect on a number of domains,
such as cognitive functioning, well-being, and self-care (Lindenberger &
Bakes, 1994; Marsiske, Klumb, & Baltes, 1997).
1. Sensorimotor resources are indicated by three constructs: visual
acuity, auditory acuity, and balance-gait. Visual acuity consists of a
composite construct of three measurements, (a) Distance visual acuity
was assessed binocularly with a reading table placed at least 2.5 m from
the participant. Close visual acuity was assessed separately (b) for the
left eye and (c) for the right eye with a reading table placed at reading
distance. Auditory acuity was assessed with a Bosch ST-20-1 pure-tone
audiometer by using headphones at the participant's residence or in the
clinic of a university medical school (for a detailed description of the
measures for visual acuity and auditory acuity, see Lindenberger &
Baltes, 1994). For the assessment of balance-gait, participants were
asked to perform a 360° turn as fast as they could without risking a fall.
The number of steps needed to complete the circle was counted (for a
detailed description of this measure, see M. M. Baltes et al., 1993).
For psychosocial resources, cognitive, personality, and social re-
sources are included. The cognitive resources included are closely re-
lated to the concept of general intelligence (Lindenberger, Mayr, &
Kliegl, 1993). As personality resources we used two measures that
reflect protective factors (i.e., resources) rather than risk factors. For
social resources we used information gathered with the social relation-
ship questionnaire in BASE, which is in part based on the circle diagram
(Kahn & Antonucci, 1980; Lang, 1996; Lang & Carstensen, 1994).
2. Cognitive resources relate to a measure of reasoning consisting of
three tests: Figural Analogies, Letter Series, and Practical Problems
(Lindenberger et al., 1993). All tests were assessed within the cognitive
battery of the BASE study with a Macintosh SE30 personal computer
system equipped with a Micro Touch Systems touch-sensitive screen for
stimulus presentation and data collection (for a detailed description of
the cognitive battery used in BASE, see Lindenberger et al., 1993).
3. Personality resources relate to the personality dimension of extra-
version and goal strength. The personality construct of extraversion con-
sists of a 6-item German adaptation (Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1990; see
also Smith & Baltes, 1993) of the Extraversion subscale of the NEO-
Personality Questionnaire (Costa & McCrae, 1985). Participants rated
the degree to which each of the items described themselves on a 5-point
scale. Internal consistency was indicated by an alpha coefficient of .64.
Goal strength, the second personality resource, represented the average
of the perceived investment in 10 goal domains (e.g., health, hobbies,
friends, family, and death and dying; Staudinger, Smith, & Freund,
1992).
4. Social resources are indicated with three measures concerning the
availability of social and relational resources, (a) Perceived social sup-
port in the network is measured as the number of different instances of
social support received during the past 3 months (e.g., being cheered
up or being supported in times of sorrow; Lang, 1996). (b) Role variety
in the social network reflected the availability of different role relation-
ships. If an older person mentioned, for instance, the presence of a
grandchild in the social network, this was scored as an instance for the
role of grandparent. For a detailed description of the social network
measure used in the BASE study, see also Lang and Carstensen (1994;
Lang, 1996); The third indicator was assessed by the sociologists in
BASE and referred to social status, (c) As an indicator of social status,
occupational status was assessed with the Wegener Prestige Scale (Weg-
ener, 1988). Married and widowed women received scores reflecting
the occupational status of their husbands (i.e., if the husband's status
was higher than that of the wife). For unmarried and single women, their
own occupational status was determined by Iheir own last occupation.
Overview of Statistical Procedures
To explore mechanisms of late-life adaptivity, such as selection, opti-
mization, and compensation, a comparison of age-related differences
in everyday functioning among resource-rich and resource-poor adults
ultimately requires a longitudinal approach. However, first insights can
also be gained from cross-sectional analysis, especially when the focus
is on the comparison of age differences in some dependent variable
Table 1
Description of Eight Classes of Everyday Activities
Activity Description
Self-care
Household chores/housekeeping
Physical leisure
Intellectual-cultural leisure
Television viewing
Social engagement
Resting
Sleeping
Basic activities of daily living (e.g., getting up, toileting, and eating)
Instrumental activities of daily living (e.g., shopping, cleaning banking, and getting medical care)
Activities that include any kind of physical efforts or demands (e.g., exercising, gardening, walking, and
traveling)
Activities refering to intellectual efforts or cultural interest (e.g., visiting the museum, painting, visiting the
theater, writing, reading, playing, listening to radio or music, and religious activities)
Separate leisure category
Activities that explicitly engage social partners (e.g., talking to others, visiting others, telephoning, helping
others, and political activities)
Passive phases during the day (not sleeping)
Sleeping during daytime
436 BALTES AND LANG
between resource-defined groups of older adults (e.g., Hertzog, 1985).
We present four steps of data analysis. In Step 1, four major domains
of resources were differentiated. These were sensorimotor, cognitive,
personality, and social resources that were assessed with 11 criteria,
with 3 criteria in each domain except for only 2 in the personality
domain (see p. 435). Unit-weighted composites for each domain of
resource indicators were aggregated (i.e., factor scores) as a result of
four independent exploratory factor analyses. Table 2 displays the factor
loadings, communalities, and the total explained variances for each of
the four resource factors.
In Step 2, these four domains of resources were analyzed to detect
groups of resource-rich and resource-poor older adults. Because these
resources showed differential age correlations, an exploratory factor
analysis on the four domains of resources was conducted with age par-
tialed out. These residualized factors represent resources not directly
associated with age. Thus, partialing age allowed us to identify relatively
age-heterogeneous groups for further age comparisons concerning every-
day functioning. Two factors emerged: a Sensorimotor-Cognitive re-
source factor and a Social-Personality resource factor. In Step 3, these
two resulting age-residualized resource factors were dichotomized to
identify four resource-defined groups of older adults. Given the quality
of our measurement variables in terms of validity and reliability, a me-
dian-split approach is justified and reduces the complexity of further
tests for interaction effects between age and resources, despite the known
problems of such procedures (for a discussion of this problem, see, for
example. Maxwell & Delaney, 1993; McClelland & Judd, 1993). In
Step 4, differential age effects on parameters of everyday functioning
between the four groups of older adults were compared by using hierar-
chical regression analyses.
Results
Exploratory Factor Analyses on Resource Indicators
First, to examine the relationship between the four resource
domains—sensorimotor, cognitive, personality, and social inte-
gration—four exploratory factor analyses were conducted on
the subsets of the 11 success criteria (3 criteria in each domain
save for only 2 in the personality domain). Table 2 shows the
factor loadings for each resource factor. As Table 2 also shows,
most of the four resource indicators were correlated with age.
Sensorimotor resources were highly negatively related to age
(r — -.73, p < .001), cognitive resources were moderately
negatively related (r = —.51, p < .001), personality resources
were only mildly correlated with age (r = -.23, p < .001),
and social integration resources did not show any significant
age association (r = -.09, ns). Next, to be able to make age
comparisons between resource-rich and resource-poor adults,
age was partialed from the four resource factors by using age-
residualized factor scores. Remember that partialing out age
allowed us to extract in a second step relatively age-heteroge-
neous factors, thus making further age comparisons between
resource-rich versus resource-poor older adults possible.
Second, an exploratory factor analysis with oblique rotation
was conducted on the age-residualized resource indicators, re-
vealing two relatively distinct factors: one capturing the variance
of sensorimotor and cognitive resources, and one capturing the
variance of the social and personality resources. The factor anal-
ysis accounted for 66% of the total variance (see Table 3).
The Sensorimotor-Cognitive factor correlated mildly with the
Social-Personality factor (r = .22).
Third, to identify groups of resource-rich versus resource-
poor participants, the Sensorimotor-Cognitive and Social-Per-
sonality factor scores were dichotomized by using a median
split resulting in four groups of participants: (a) 95 participants
were above the median on both resource factors (resource-rich
group), (b) 140 participants were above the median in the
Sensorimotor-Cognitive resource factor but not in Social-Per-
sonality resources, (c) 146 participants were above the median
in the Social-Personality resource factor but not in the Sensori-
motor-Cognitive factor, and (d) 104 participants were below
the median on both resource factors (resource-poor group).
Table 4 gives an overview of some of the central descriptors of
the four resource-defined groups of older adults.
As a consequence of having partialed out age earlier, the four
groups did not necessarily differ in respect to age. However,
Table 2
Four Exploratory Factor Analyses on Unit-Weighted Resource Indicators Within Four
Domains: Factor Loadings, Communalities, Explained Variances, and Age Correlations
Factor analyses Factor loading h2 Total explained variance (%) Age correlation
Sensorimotor resources
Auditory acuity
Visual acuity
Gait
Cognitive resources
Figural Analogies test
Letter Series tesl
Practical Problems test
Personality resources
Extraversion
Goal strength
Social resources
Role variety of network
Received social support
Occupational status
.78
.80
.78
.86
.88
.86
.80
.80
.76
.75
.53
62
.61
.65
.60
75
.74
.77
.75
63
.64
.64
47
.57
.56
.28
-.73***
-.51***
-.23***
-.09
Note. N = 516. A2 represents factor communality.
***p < .00!.
EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING AND SUCCESSFUL AGING 437
Table 3
Results of an Exploratory Factor Analysis With Age-
Residualized Resource Factors
Factor loadings
Resource factors
Sensoriraotor
Cognitive
Social
Personality
Total explained
variance (%)
Factor 1:
Sensorimotor-
Cognitive
.80
.81
.27
-.16
4J.1
Factor 2:
Personality -
Social
-.03
.03
.64
.90
24.7
Factor
communality
(A2)
.63
.68
.68
.77
groups differed significantly in respect to marital status, living
arrangement, and dementia; no gender difference was found.
Compared with all other groups, participants below the median
in both factors (resource-poor group) were more likely not to
be married, to live in an institution, and to be demented. Note
that the 146 participants above the median in the Social -Person-
ality factor only were also more likely to be demented than
those in the other two groups; namely, the participanls above
the median in both factors (resource-rich group) as well as
those above the median in the Sensorimotor-Cognitive factor
only.
Before investigating the effect of Age x Group interaction
effects on everyday functioning, correlations between age and
the two resource factors within each of the four groups had to
be checked. Differential correlations within the groups could
account for differential age correlations between the four
groups. With two exceptions, no significant correlations be-
tween age and the resource factors were found within the four
groups. The Sensorimotor-Cognitive factor was positively cor-
related with age within the resource-poor group (below the
median in both factors; r = .34, p < .01) as well as in the group
above the median in the Personality-Social resource factor only
(r = .19, p < .01). These correlations were not significant in
the two other groups (—.02 within the resource-rich group and
—.11 in the group above the median in sensorimotor-cognitive
resources only).
Description of Everyday Functioning in the Four
Resource-Defined Groups
Table 5 displays means and standard deviations of the indica-
tors of everyday functioning in the four groups of older adults
as well as the total explained variances and significance of
group comparisons. As can be seen in Table 5, groups differed
significantly in respect to the length of the waking day, the
variability of activities, the frequencies of intellectual and social
activities, and resting or contemplation during the day. Group
differences explained only small portions of the total variances
with respect to the frequencies of housekeeping activities and
physical leisure activities and to the duration of resting phases.
Most of these observed group differences were related to
differences between the resource-poor group and the other three
groups. The resource-poor group reported shorter days, a
smaller variety of activities, fewer physical and intellectual-
cultural activities, fewer social activities, and more as well as
longer resting times during the day. When excluding the re-
source-poor group, none of the reported differences, save the
one for frequency of social activities, remained significant. The
older adults above the median in only sensorimotor-cognitive
resources reported also fewer social activities than the resource-
rich older adults (3% of total explained variance; p < .01).
When contrasting the resource-rich versus the resource-poor
group, the first one reported a greater variety of activities—
housekeeping, physical, intellectual, and social-relational—
more television viewing, and less resting or passive times during
Table 4
Descriptors of the Four Resource-Defined Groups
Descriptor
Age
M
SD
Women (%)
Married {%)
Widowed (%)
Never married-divorced (%)
Demented (%)
Living in institutions (%)
Sensorimotor-cognitive resources
T-value mean
SD
Social-personality resources
T-value mean
SD
Resource
rich Sensorimotor-cognitive
(H = 95) resources (n = 140)
84.1
9.2
42.1
43.2
46.3
10.5
5.3
2.1
58.6
6.1
56.7
5.7
84.2
8.8
43.6
27.9
55.0
17.1
7.9
8.6
58.8
6.7
42.0
6.8
Social —personality
resources
(n - 146)
85.0
7.9
54.1
34.2
53.4
12.3
24.0
11.6
41.9
6.6
59.1
6.2
Resource
poor
(n = 104) F(3, 481) x2<3, N = 485)
84.6 0.3
8.7 0.3
52.9 0.6
21.2
59.6 14.0*
19.2
29.8 34.8***
23.1 3.4***
43.4 268.7***
5.3
43.0 254.0***
6.4
Note, T values represent a T transformation.
*p< .05. ***p < .001.
438 BALTES AND LANG
Table 5
Means of Everyday Activity Parameters for the Four Resource-Defined Groups
Sensorimotor—
cognitive
Resource rich
Activity parameters
Length of waking day (min)
Time spent alone (min)
Time spent with family (min)
Time spent with others (min)
Variability of activities (T values)
Frequency
Self-care
Household chores
Physical leisure
Intellectual-cultural leisure
Television viewing
Social engagement
Resting
Sleeping during daytime
Weighted duration61
Self-care
Household chores
Physical leisure
Intellectual -cultural leisure
Television viewing
Social engagement
Resting
Sleeping during daytime
(n
M
975
590
169
123
53.1
7.8
5.5
4.0
3.5
1.8
2.0
1.6
0.5
19.5
24.9
26.5
55.1
89.6
32.7
43.5
35.1
= 95)
SD
96
311
234
147
9.1
1.7
3.8
3.5
1.8
1.3
1.8
1.4
0.6
6.7
18.4
24.2
34.1
67.7
33.3
47.2
47.2
resources
(« =
M
991
611
219
132
50.9
8.0
5.4
3.9
3.2
1.7
1.5
2.1
0.5
18.7
25.1
24.0
51.1
93.1
35.2
42.9
38.6
140)
SD
102
340
289
197
9.4
1.9
3.2
3.3
2.0
1.2
1.4
2.1
0.6
5.9
16.5
23.0
32.3
67.8
40.4
42.0
55.7
Social -
personality
resources
(» =
M
972
580
235
153
51.0
8.0
5.3
3.4
2.9
1.8
1.8
2.3
0.6
19.3
24.8
23.7
47.8
87.4
30.1
48.0
41.8
146)
SD
107
334
308
227
9.7
1.8
3.4
3.2
2.1
1.3
1.8
2.0
0.7
6.3
16.6
29.4
32.6
67.1
39.3
38.7
53.9
Resource poor
(n
M
928
603
249
149
44.4
7.7
4.1
2.7
2.3
1.4
1.2
2.6
0.5
19.5
22.4
23.1
51.8
82.6
34.8
60.1
36.1
= 104)
SD
125
310
292
222
10
1.9
3.7
2.8
2.0
1.2
1.5
2.2
0.8
7.0
23.3
33.5
42.6
81.6
59.6
45.2
51.6
«•
4.3***
0.2
1.6
0.5
9.3***
0.3
2.1*
2.3»
44* *t
1.6
3.8**»
2.8**
0.3
0.3
0.3
0.2
0.5
0.3
0.2
2.3*
0.2
Note. T value represents a T transformation.
a R2 X 100 indicates the proportion of explained variance in the respective category. Significance relates to F tests of univariate analyses of variance.
b Weighted duration of activities relates to the average duration of one single activity in the respective activity category (in minutes).
*p<.Q5. **p < .01. ***p<.001.
the day. There were no differences between these two groups
in respect to frequency of self-care and sleeping during the day.
Overall, everyday functioning of resource-rich older adults as
compared with resource-poor older adults is characterized by
more activity in different activity domains.
Age Differences in Everyday Functioning
To test for differential age effects on everyday functioning
among the resource-poor and resource-rich groups of older
adults, we compared the regression coefficients of age on the
indicators of everyday functioning. Table 6 provides the differ-
ential age correlations within all four resource-defined groups.
AX2 in the right column of Table 6 indicates the size of effects
(percentage of explained variance) and significance of the dif-
ferential slopes when comparing the resource-rich and the re-
source-poor groups of older adults,
Age differences bet\veen the resource-rich and the resource-
poor groups. There were consistent differences between the
resource-rich and the resource-poor groups with respect to the
correlations between everyday activities and age. First, in com-
parison to resource-poor older adults, resource-rich older adults
had fewer negative age differences. Second, age correlations
were significantly reduced in the resource-rich group compared
with the resource-poor group with respect to six variables:
length of day, variety of activities, time spent alone, number and
duration of housekeeping activities, and duration of intellectual -
cultural activities. Figure 1 displays the scatterplots for the sig-
nificant differential age trends in the resource-rich and the re-
source-poor groups on length of waking day (Figure la), vari-
ability of activities (Figure I b ) , and frequency of social-rela-
tional activities (Figure Ic).
In addition, there were also differences between both groups
in respect to correlations between age and duration indicators
of everyday activities. Figure 2 displays the scatterplots for the
significant differential age trends in the resource-rich and the
resource-poor groups on duration of time spent alone (Figure
2a), duration of housekeeping activities (Figure 2b), and dura-
tion of intellectual activities (Figure 2c).
Not included in Figures 1 and 2 is the interaction effect of
Age X Group on time spent with others (cf. Table 6). Most of
this effect was related to the fact that with increasing age the
resource-poor older adults were more likely to live in an institu-
tion. That is, very old (above 85 years) compared with young-
old ( below 85 years) older people in the resource-poor group
reported more time spent with professional helpers and room-
mates. When helpers were excluded from the analysis, the effect
disappears. In contrast, such a relationship with age was not
found among the resource-rich older adults.
Age differences between all four resource groups. Differen-
tial age effects on everyday functioning between the resource-
EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING AND SUCCESSFUL AGING 439
Table 6
Age Effects for Everyday Activities Within Each Group: The Resource Rich, the Sensorimotor-Cognitive Resources Only, the
Social-Personality Resources Only, and the Resource Poor
Regression coefficients of age on everyday activities within
Activity parameters
Length of waking day (min)
Time spent alone (min)
Time spent with family (min)
Time spent with others (min)
Variability of activities
Frequency
Self-care
Household chores
Physical leisure
Intellectual -cultural leisure
Television viewing
Social engagement
Resting
Sleeping during daytime
Weighted duration
Self-care
Household chores
Physical leisure
Intellectual-cultural leisure
Television viewing
Social engagement
Resting
Sleeping during daytime
Resource rich
(n = 95)
-.23*
.16
-.17
-.10
-.23*
.09
-.15
-.17
-.28**
-.11
-.34*
.24*
.27**
.17
-.06
-.11
.19
.01
-.17
.12
.29**
Sensorimotor -cognitive
resources (n = 140)
-.19*
.03
-.02
-.14
-.35**
.02
-.19*
-.41**
-.31**
-.20*
-.13
.38**
.16
.15
.02
-.11
-.11
.02
-.05
.29**
.15
Social-personality
resources
(„ = 146)
-.02
.09
-.28**
.16
-.35**
-.06
-.19**
-.20*
-.25*
-.20*
-.13
.30
.09
.24*
-.12
-.17*
-.08
-.07
-.06
.31**
.09
groups
Resource poor
(n = 104)
-.48**
-.19*
-.22*
.23*
-.44**
-.02
-.41**
-.28**
-.22*
-.35**
-.03
.16
.05
.22*
-.30**
-.06
-.19*
-.19*
-.03
.36**
.04
Resource rich vs.
poor group A/?2"
3.2*
3.0*
0.0
3.1*
1.4*
0.3
2.1*
0.1
0.0
1.3
2.6*
0.0
0.8
0.1
1.8*
0.0
3.7*
1.2
0.1
1.4
1.2
a AW2 X 100 indicates the increase of explained variance in the respective category accounted for by the Age X Group interaction effect.
* ; > < .05. * * p < . 0 1 .
rich and the resource-poor groups were also present in compari-
sons of these two groups with each of the two one-factor-only
resource groups (Table 6). First, comparison of the resource-
poor group with the sensoricognitive group revealed signifi-
cantly different age associations with respect to length of waking
day (AR2 = 3.1', p < .05), time spent with others (A/{2 =
3.5, p < .01), and duration as well as frequency of household-
related activities (A/?2 = 3.4, p < .01, respectively, 1.8, p <
.05). When comparing the social—personality group with the
resource-poor group significantly different age associations
were apparent in the length of waking day (AS2 = 5.4, p <
.01) and time spent alone (AR2 = 1.8, p < .05).
Second, when comparing the resource-rich group with the
sensoricognitive group, significant Age X Group interaction ef-
fects on everyday functioning were found with respect to dura-
tion of intellectual activities (AW 2 = 2.4, p < .05), frequency
of social-relational activities (AR2 = 1.7, p < .05), and dura-
tion of passive times (AR2 = 1.5, p < .05). Comparison with
the social-personality group revealed significant Age X Group
interaction effects with regard to time spent with others (AR 2
= 1.6, p < .05) and duration of intellectual activities (AR2 =
1.8, p < .05). This pattern of findings underscores that both
social-personality resources as well as sensoricognitive re-
sources contribute to the pattern of age differences in everyday
functioning found for the resource-rich group. Being rich in
only one of the two resource factors alone could not account
for the pattern of findings reported above.
Discussion
In this research we tested the assumption that the resource-
rich participants exhibit less negative age trends than the
resource-poor participants. In this sense, resources would
have protective or buffering functions against negative aging
effects on everyday functioning. This buffering or protective
function can be explained with the model of selective optimi-
zation with compensation (SOC; P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes,
1990).
On the basis of the SOC model, we argue that resource-rich
people will age more successfully because they can make more
frequent use of the three processes—selection, optimization,
and compensation—and thereby delay decline. In the context
of aging, selection is defined as actively or passively reducing
the number of goals and domains in order to free and conserve
energy and motivation for more important goals or to select new
goals in the service of new developmental tasks (e.g., awareness
of one's own finitude); compensation is defined as searching
for and using alternate means to reach a goal once old means
are lost or blocked. Optimization is defined as the refinement
of means and resources that are necessary to reach a goal and
to excel in selected domains, thereby maximizing the quantity
and quality of one's life (for extensive discussions of the pro-
cesses see M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; P. B. Baltes & M. M.
' AR, is reported after multiplying by 100.
440 BALTES AND LANG
Baltes, 1990; Marsiske et al., 1995). Accordingly, the model
treats the success criteria as resources or protective factors (Rut-
ter, 1987; Staudinger, Marsiske, & Baltes, 1995):
The more I have in terms of resources the better I can engage in
selection, compensation, and optimization strategies to replace or
readjust goals, the more likely will I age successfully—that is,
reach those goals that 1 have selected as important for me—and
the less I will experience aging decline.
In a cross-sectional design, we used a factor analytic ap-
proach, first, to describe the relationship between the 11 re-
source indicators that resulted in four domains of resources—
sensorimotor, cognitive, personality, and social resources—and,
second, to identify groups differing in resources. In the factor
analytic procedure, age was partialed out to allow the detection
of differential age effects in the identified groups. Two age-
residualized factors emerged; a Sensorimotor-Cognitive re-
sources factor and a Personality-Social resources factor. Using
a median-split procedure, we identified four groups: a resource-
rich group identified as above the median in both factors, and
a resource-poor group identified as below the median in both
factors. These two groups were considered to represent the most
successful and the least successful aging adults. Two additional
groups were identified as possessing resources in only one of
the factors; that is, those that were above the median in either
one of the two resource factors. As was shown in the data, these
groups clearly fall in between the most and least successful
aging group with regard to everyday functioning.
The findings permit some general and specific statements. In
general, comparing the mean level of the 21 indicators of every-
day functioning, the resource-rich older adults (i.e., the most
successful aging adults) exhibited a higher level of activity. This
higher level refers almost exclusively to higher frequency scores
and to greater variety in activities. No differences in activity
level were found, however, with regard to self-care activities,
television watching, and sleeping during the daytime. Although
there was a significant difference between the groups as to length
of waking day, there were no significant differences between
the groups when looking at the duration or time spent in activi-
ties. There was a difference, however, in resting time that was
longer in the resource-poor group than the other groups.
At first glance, it might seem surprising that neither frequency
nor duration of self-care activities differed significantly between
the four groups. Several explanations come to mind. First of
all, there is a finite number of self-care activities that can and
need to be accomplished per day. Usually you dress yourself or
undress yourself once a day. Second, most of these self-care
activities are highly routinized and automatized and thus might
be the least or the last affected by aging losses. Third, significant
differences in time spent with self-care across age groups (see
Figure 2) suggested that when difficulties do arise in the execu-
tion of these activities, help and assistance is forthcoming.
Therefore, time spent with these activities would not increase,
if anything perhaps even decrease.
In summary, looking at group differences, older people with
only few resources seem to be less active and engaged in less
different activities. This is true particularly for the least success-
ful aging adults—the resource-poor group. Most of the signifi-
cant differences disappeared when the resource-poor group was
deleted from the comparison. This seems to signify the follow-
ing : the more resources the better, at least with regard to every-
day functioning.
When inspecting the age effects on parameters of daily func-
tioning we found first that age was a risk factor in all four
groups: \bung older people did better than the older people in
all groups. As expected, however, the most successful aging,
resource-rich group showed fewer and less dramatic age effects
when compared with the resource-poor group. The negative
difference with regard to variability in activities between young
and old older people in both groups can be interpreted as the
result of selection strategies that help concentrate efforts and
energies on those goals that are most important to the older
person.
Significant Group X Age interaction effects related to the
length of the waking day, variety of activities, frequency and
duration of household activities, and duration of intellectual
activities, all of which showed significantly greater negative age
effects in the resource-poor group. Only with regard to time
spent alone or time spent with others and frequency of social -
relational activities did the resource-rich group show larger neg-
ative age effects. This discrepancy can be explained by selection
and compensation processes. We have already alluded to the
assistance by others; that is, professional helpers, which explains
the finding that the resource-poor, least successful aging older
adults exhibited less negative age differences with regard to
time spent in social-relational activities than the resource-rich,
most successful adults (see Figure 2 ) . Unable to perform basic
activities any longer, the resource-poor, least successful aging
older adults became dependent on others to execute those activi-
ties and thus spend more time more often in the presence of
others (M. M. Baltes, 1995). They did compensate for losses
in respect to self-care activities with the help of others.
In contrast, the negative age differences in social-relational
activities for the most successful aging group could be a selec-
tion effect as postulated, for instance, by the socioemotional
selectivity theory by Carstensen (1991, 1993). According to a
series of studies by Carstensen (1993) one would expect nega-
tive age differences in social-relational activities that reflect a
preference for emotionally meaningful or gratifying interactions
with very close social partners, whereas interactions with other
less close social partners are reduced (Lang & Carstensen, 1994;
Lang, Staudinger, & Carstensen. 1996).
Furthermore, we found within the resource-rich group that the
young-old older adults spent less time in intellectual—cultural
activities than the old-old older adults. This could be the product
of either optimizing or compensating strategies. That is, the very
old resource-rich older adults might compensate in that they
allow themselves more time to execute these tasks or they might
optimize intellectual performance and, therefore, put more effort
and time into these tasks. When considering, however, that the
Sensoricognitive-factor-only group did not show such a positive
difference between young and old, optimization seems to be the
more fitting interpretation. Similarly, among the resource-rich
older adults, the old old showed increasingly more and longer
sleeping periods than the young old. Within SOC, the resource-
rich old old use sleeping to revitalize, and thus sleeping becomes
a compensatory strategy for a lowered energy level.
In general, these findings support our hypothesis that re-
EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING AND SUCCESSFUL AGING 441
Length of Waking Day
O O • •
89 94
Age Cohort
99 104
(b)
80 ~
60 -
50 -
40 -
30 -
20
O Resource
Rich (O)
69 74 79 84 89 94
Age Cohort
99 104
(c)
10 -|
Frequency of Social Activities
Resource
Poor (•)
Resource
Rich (O)
74 79 84 89 94
Age Cohort
99 104
Figure !. Significant age differences in three indicators of everyday
functioning—length of waking day, variability of activities, and fre-
quency of social activities—shown separately for the resource-rich
group and for the resource-poor group.
sources help maximize the quantity and quality of life in old and
very old age. Resources provide fertile ground for the interplay
between the three processes of selection, optimization, and com-
pensation and thereby buffer and reduce the magnitude of cross-
sectional age differences in the resource-rich group. In sum-
mary, the cross-sectional findings speak for the assumption that
resource-rich older adults when compared with resource-poor
older adults will demonstrate more investment of time and effort
in selected domains of everyday functioning. Among resource-
poor adults, everyday functioning seems to be characterized by
a general concentration on basic levels of everyday competence.
(a)
Duration of Time Alone
J
1200-
1000-
800-
600-
400-
200-
0
Resource
Rich (O)£L5£g
Resource
(b)
69 74 79 84 89 94
Age Cohort
99 104
Duration of Housekeeping Activities
£ 90-1 O
70 -
50 -
Resource
Rich (O)
Resource
Poor(.)
(C)
69 74 79 84 89 94 99 104
Age Cohort
Duration of Intellectual Activities
74 79 84 89 94 99
Age Cohort
104
Figure 2. Significant age differences in three indicators of everyday
functioning—duration of time alone, duration of household chores-
housekeeping, and duration of intellectual activities—shown separately
for the resource-rich group and the resource-poor group.
442 BALTES AND LANG
These specific interpretations as well as the general relationship
between protective factors and their buffering effect toward de-
cline or negative aging changes will be validated with longitudi-
nal data that will be available for the Berlin Aging Study in the
near future.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Atchley, R. C. (1989). A continuity theory of normal aging. Gerontolo-
gist, 29, 183-190.
Baltes, M. M. (1995). Dependency in old age: Gains and losses. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 14-19.
Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1996). The process of successful
aging. Ageing and Society, 16, 397—422.
flaltes, M. M., Maas, I., Wilms, H.-U, & Borchelt, M. (1996). Alltags-
kompetenz im Alter: Theoretische Oberlegungen und empirische Be-
funde [Everyday competence in old age: Theoretical considerations
and empirical findings]. In K. U. Mayer & P. B. Baltes (Eds,), Die
Berliner Altersstudie (pp. 525-542). Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Baltes, M. M., Mayr, U., Borchelt, M., Maas, I., & Wilms, H.-U. (1993).
Everyday competence in old and very old age: An inter-disciplinary
perspective. Ageing and Society, 13, 657-680.
Baltes, M. M., Wahl, H. W, & Schmid-Furstoss, U. (1990). The daily
life of elderly Germans: Activity patterns, personal control, and func-
tional health. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 45,
P173-P179.
Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. Gemmolo-
gist, 33, 580-594.
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on
successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensa-
tion. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Per-
spectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1-34). New Tfork: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Baltes, P. B., Mayer, K. U., Helmchen, H., & Steinhagen-Thiessen, E.
(1993). The Berlin Aging Study (BASE): Overview and design. Age-
ing and Society, 13, 483-515.
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, R (1990). Comparing explanatory and con-
firmatory factor analysis: A study on the five-factor model of personal-
ity. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 515-524.
Biihler, C. (1933). Der menschliche Lebenslauf als psychologisches
Problem [The human life course as psychological problem]. Leipzig,
Germany: Hirzel.
Carstensen, L. L. (1991). Socioemotional selectivity theory: Social ac-
tivity in life-span context. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriat-
rics, 11, 195-217.
Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life
span: A theory of socio-emotional selectivity. In J. Jacobs (Ed.),
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 209-254). Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press.
Costa, P. T, & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO-Personality Inventory:
Manual Form S and Form R. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Cumming, E., & Henry, W. E. (1961). Growing old: The process of
disengagement. New 'tork: Basic Books.
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues
(Monograph 1). New York: International University Press.
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Life cycle. In D. L. Sills (Ed.), International
encyclopedia of the social sciences (pp. 286-292). New ^fork: Mac-
millan/Free Press.
Erikson, E. H. (1982). The life cycle completed. New York: Norton.
Erikson, E. H., Erikson, J. M., & Kivnick, H. (1986). Vital involvement
in old age: The experience of old age in our time. London: Norton.
Havighurst, R. J., & Albrecht, R. (1953). Older people. New York:
Longmans.
Hertzog, C. (1985). An individual differences perspective. Research on
Aging, 7, 7-45.
Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New
York: Basic Books.
Jung, C. G. (1931). Die Lebenswende [The life transition]. In C. G.
Jung (Ed.), Seelenprobleme der Gegenwarl (pp. 248-274). Zurich,
Switzerland: Rascher.
Jung, C. G. (1934). Seele und Tod [Psyche and death]. In C. G. Jung
(Ed.), Wirklichkeit der Seele (pp. 212-230). Zurich, Switzerland:
Rascher.
Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convoys over the life course:
Attachment, roles and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim,
Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 254-283). New
York: Academic Press.
Lang, F. R. (1996). Social support relationships of parents and nonpar-
ents in old and very old age. In H. Mollenkopf (Ed.), Elderly people
in industrialised societies (pp. 41-51). Berlin: Edition Sigma.
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (1994). Close emotional relationships
in late life: Further support for proactive aging in the social domain.
Psychology and Aging, 9, 315-324.
Lang, F. R., Staudinger, U. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1996). Socioemo-
tional selectivity in late life: How personality and social context do
(and do not) make a difference. Unpublished manuscript, Free Univer-
sity Berlin.
Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intel-
ligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging, 9,
339-355.
Lindenberger, U, & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning in old
and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study.
Psychology and Aging, 12, 395-409.
Lindenberger, U., Gilberg, R., Potter, U., Little, T. D., & Baltes, P. B.
(1996). Stichproblenselektivitat und Generalisierbarkeit der Ergeb-
nisse in der Berliner Altersstudie [Sample selectivity and generaliz-
ability of the results in the Berlin Aging Study]. In K. U. Mayer &
P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Die Berliner Altersstudie (pp. 85-108). Berlin:
Akademie Verlag.
Lindenberger, U., Mayr, U, & Kliegl, R. (1993). Speed and intelligence
in old age. Psychology and Aging, 8, 207-220.
Maddox, G. L. (1963). Activity and morale: A longitudinal study of
selected elderly subjects. Social Forces, 42, 195-204.
Marsiske, M., Klumb, P. L., & Baltes, M. M. (1997). Everyday activity
patterns and sensory functioning in old age. Psychology and Aging,
12, 444-457.
Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1995). Selec-
tive optimization with compensation: Life-span perspectives on suc-
cessful human development. In R. Dixon & L. Backman (Eds.), Psy-
chological compensation: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp.
35-79). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). Princeton,
NJ: Van Nostrand.
Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (1993). Bivariate median splits and
spurious statistical significance. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 181-
190.
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of
detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin.
1/4, 376-390.
Moss, M., & Lawton, M. P. (1982). Time budgets of older people: A
window on four lifestyles. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 576-582.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rowe, J. W, & Kahn. R. L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and successful.
Science, 237, 143-149.
Rutter, M. (1987). Resilience in the face of adversity. Protective factors
EVERYDAY FUNCTIONING AND SUCCESSFUL AGING 443
and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry,
147, 598-611.
Ryff, C. D. (1982). Successful aging: A developmental approach. Ger-
onlologisl, 22, 209-214.
Ryff, C. D. (1989a). In the eye of the beholder: Views of psychological
well-being among middle-aged and older adults. Psychology and
Aging, 4, 195-210.
Ryff, C. D. (I989b). Beyond Ponce de Leon and life satisfaction: New
directions in quest of successful aging. International Journal of Be-
havioral Development, 12, 35-55.
Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1993). Differential psychological ageing:
Profiles of the old and very old. Ageing and Society, 13, 551-587.
Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and
reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of develop-
ment across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.),
Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, pp. 801-847). New York:
Wiley.
Staudinger, U. M., Smith, J., & Freund, A. (1992, November). The goal
system: A facet of the resilient self in old age? Paper presented at the
45th annual scientific meeting of the Gerontological Society of
America, Washington, DC.
Steinhagen-Thiessen, E., & Borchelt, M. (1996). Morbiditat, Medika-
tion und Funktionalitat im Alter [Morbidity, medication, and func-
tional capacity in old age]. In K. U. Mayer & P. B. Baltes (Eds.),
Die Berliner Altersstudie (pp. 151-183). Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Wegener, B. (1988). Kritik des Prestiges [Critique of prestige], Opla-
den, Germany: Westdeutscher Verlag.
Received June 26, 1996
Revision received January 10, 1997
Accepted January 10, 1997 •
Sternberg Appointed Editor of Contemporary Psychology
(APA Review of Books), 1999-2004
The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association
announces the appointment of Robert J. Sternberg, Yale University, as editor of Contempo-
rary Psychology (APA Review of Books) for a 6-year term beginning in 1999.
Sternberg, at the request of the Publications and Communications Board, as well as many
readers, will be embarking on a program to make the journal more timely, more interesting,
and more relevant to psychologists during his editor-elect year in 1998. Some of the changes
envisioned include fewer but longer and more thoughtful reviews of books, reviews only of
"new" books (with a few noteworthy exceptions), comparative textbook reviews at strategic
times of the year, and changes in publication frequency and pricing. Steinberg welcomes
suggestions for improving the journal and serving reader needs.
E-mail: sterobj@yalevm.cis.yale.edu
Publishers should note that books should not be sent to Sternberg. Publishers should
continue to send two copies of books to be considered for review plus any notices of
publication to
PsycINFO Services Department, APA
Attn: Contemporary Psychology Processing
750 First Street, NE
Washington, DC 20002-4242
(Please note that all reviews are written by invitation.)
We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.
Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.
Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.
Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.
Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.
Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.
We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.
Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.
You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.
Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.
Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.
You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.
You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.
Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.
We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.
We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.
We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.
Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!
Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality
Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.
We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.
We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.
We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.
We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.