Psychology

Remember, reactions are the amount of material that will fit on a one page, double-spaced, typed document. Part of the exercise is for you to get right to your point and justify it briefly. Sources are open, but I’d prefer some element of empirical research. So, if you see something in the newspaper and want to react to it, try to track down the original research. Or, find some research that supports or refutes the information in the newspaper and discuss that. Show me that you’re thinking, include some cognitive stuff, and read some of the primary literature and I will be pleased.

What will get me excited about a reaction paper:

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Psychology
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

· React based on something else you’ve learned in the class (“when we discussed language, you said…but this article said…” or “here’s another example of…”). Bring things together in a new and interesting way.

· React based on something you know about your area of psychology that relates to Cognitive Psychology.

· How does this idea lead to new research questions?

· Make me say “this person is insane, but that’s a really cool idea.” Explore absurd places to take the research.

What won’t get me excited about a reaction paper:

· “This article was really easy/hard to read/understand.”

· A personal anecdote; overturning data with an anecdote

· “There were only five participants in the study which seems like too few.” I don’t want a showboating critique, talk to me about ideas.

· Two pages of summary followed by “I really liked this article.”

· A “reflection.” In fact, calling it a reflection report will piss me off.

Cog. Psych WWR #1

In class last week the Chinese Room argument was brought up in reference to how symbols are grounded. I had never heard of the Chinese Room or John Searle so I wrote it down to investigate after class. Further into the discussion the analogy of trying to discern symbols with more symbols struck an immediate cord with me. My six year old daughter had recently finished reading a beginners chapter book on Helen Keller and in the story it tells of this momentous moment where Helen finally understood that the hand movements she was making equated to a symbol for the water she was feeling. In the book Helen poignantly describes this as her soul’s birthday. My little one had a lot of questions about what that meant and I fumblingly try to convey the meaning. Finally I asked her to close her eyes and cover her ears tight while I made movements in her hand. I think in some small way she was able to appreciate what it would be like to have communicate that way.

In an effort to try and learn more about that moment I pursued more information about the Chinese Room and Helen Keller. Indeed I found an article published in Minds & Machines in 2006 titled “How Helen Keller used syntactic semantics to escape from a Chinese Room” by William J. Rapaport. He posits that computers can learn natural language through syntax semantics which he says is how Helen Keller came to know language. In a dictionary analogy Rapaport clearly identifies the circular process of defining word with another. At some point you must have understanding or meaning of a word to get out of this loop. He calls this a closed loop. Our minds work in much the same way per Rapport “More significantly, our brain is just such a closed system: all information that we get from the external world, along with all of our thoughts, concepts, etc., is represented in a single system of neuron firings. Any description of that real neural network, by itself, is a purely syntactic one” (Rapport, 2006).

Ultimately, Rapport explains that Helen had some semantic correspondence because she had her own rudimentary version of signs and ways of communication before her teacher Ann Sullivan arrived. While this article certainly helped me to understand how it was Helen was able to make such an extraordinary leap in understand I was not able to see how this could be applied to computers. That

could be due to my own lack of understanding rather than fault in the author’s logic. There is another article out there titled Helen Keller was not in a Chinese Room. I intend to read that as well to see if I can get anything further of the subject

Research

Why Can’t We Be More
Idiographic in Our Research?
David H. Barlow1 and Matthew K. Nock2

1
Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston University, and

2
Harvard University

ABSTRACT—Most psychological scientists make inferences

about the relations among variables of interest by

comparing aggregated data from groups of individuals.

Although this method is unarguably a useful one that will

continue to yield scientific advances, important limitations

exist regarding the efficiency and flexibility of such de-

signs, as well as with the generality of obtained results.

Idiographic research strategies, which focus on the

intensive study of individual organisms over time, offer a

proficient and flexible alternative to group comparison

designs; however, they are rarely taught in graduate

training programs and are seldom used by psychological

scientists. We highlight some of the unique strengths of

idiographic methods, such as single case experimental

designs, and suggest that psychological science will prog-

ress most efficiently with an increased use of such methods

in both laboratory and clinical settings.

Edward Tolman said to Gordon Allport ‘‘I know I should be more

idiographic in my research, but I just don’t know how to be,’’ to

which Allport replied, ‘‘Let’s learn!’’ (Allport, 1962, p. 414).

This sentiment was based on the fact that, whether it’s a labo-

ratory rat or a patient in the clinic with a psychological disorder,

it is the individual organism that is the principle unit of analysis

in the science of psychology. The intensive study of the

individual is associated with a hallowed tradition in scientific

psychology. Indeed, the founders of experimental psychology

including Fechner, Wundt, Ebbinghaus, and Pavlov studied

individual organisms with scientific approaches that would be

considered internally valid, and they strengthened these find-

ings (and began to establish generality) through replication in

other organisms (see Barlow, Nock, & Hersen, 2008).

This scientific strategy, which is fully capable of establishing

causal relations among variables, came to be known as the

idiographic approach. Gordon Allport, in his area of social

psychology, argued eloquently that the science of psychology

should attend to the uniqueness of the individual organism

(Allport, 1962). Routed deep in the structural school of

psychology, this approach also was popular in more applied

branches in psychology in the middle of the last century.

Perhaps the biggest champion of an idiographic approach in

clinical settings was Shapiro, who was advocating a scientific

approach to the study of individuals with psychopathology as

early as 1951 (e.g., Shapiro 1961, 1966). The idiographic

approach perhaps reached its zenith in psychological science

with the work of B.F. Skinner. In a famous quote, Skinner (1966)

noted: ‘‘. . . instead of studying a thousand rats for one hour each

or a hundred rats for ten hours each the investigator is more

likely to study one rat for a thousand hours’’ (p. 21). Thus,

Skinner and his colleagues in the animal laboratories are largely

credited with developing and refining an experimental

idiographic approach that came to be known as the experimental

analysis of behavior.

This idiographic approach represents a true scientific

undertaking, as independent variables are manipulated in the

context of carefully measured and repeatedly assessed depen-

dent variables. This is in contrast to the alternative nomothetic

experimental strategy, in which the researcher looks to assemble

relatively large groups of individual organisms and, in the most

straightforward application, examines the average response of

the group to the introduction of some manipulation compared

with the response to well-construed control conditions. The

major differences between the idiographic and nomothetic

traditions are, of course, approaches to intersubject variability

and the generality of findings. As variability is often consider-

able among organisms, the task of any psychological scientist is

to discover functional relations among independent variables

over and above the welter of environmental and biological

variables influencing the organism at any given point in time. A

nomothetic approach makes an implicit assumption that much of

Address correspondence to David H. Barlow, Center for Anxiety and
Related Disorders, Boston University, 648 Beacon Street, 6th Floor,
Boston, MA 02215; e-mail: dhbarlow@bu.edu.

P E R S P E C T I V E S O N P S Y C H O L O G I C A L S C I E N C E

Volume 4—Number 1 19Copyright r 2009 Association for Psychological Science

this variability is intrinsic to the organism and uses sophisti-

cated data analytic procedures to look for reliable effects over

and above this ‘‘error.’’ Significant effects are then assumed to be

more or less generalizable based on the number of individuals

included in the experimental group and the representativeness

of the population of such individuals (i.e., the use of random

sampling).

Of course, random sampling is seldom achieved in psycho-

logical research where, indeed, the goal is more often to strive

for homogeneous samples in which the generality of findings can

be very limited. Sidman (1960) made the following point a

number of years ago when discussing approaches to variability:

The rationale for statistical immobilization of unwanted variables

is based on the assumed random nature of such variables. In a

large group of subjects, the reasoning goes, the uncontrolled factor

will change the behavior of some subjects in one direction and will

affect the remaining subjects in the opposite away. When the data

are averaged over all the subjects, the effects of the uncontrolled

variables are presumed to add algebraically to zero. The composite

data are then regarded as though they were representative of one

ideal subject who had never been exposed to the uncontrolled

variables at all. (p. 162)

Addressing the issue of the generality of findings, Sidman

wrote the following:

Tracking down sources of variability is then a primary technique

for establishing generality. Generality and variability are basically

antithetical concepts. If there are major undiscovered sources of

variability in a given set of data, any attempt to achieve subject or

principle generality is likely to fail. Every time we discover and

achieve control of a factor that contributes to variability, we

increase the likelihood that our data will be reproducible with new

subjects and in different situations. Experience has taught us that

precision of control leads to more extensive generalization of data.

(p. 152)

Although the use of the idiographic approach led to signifi-

cant advances in the earliest days of laboratory-based experi-

mental psychology, as well as during early translations of

findings from psychological science to clinical applications in

the middle of the last century, it is clear that the nomothetic

strategy has become a dominant method to establish both

internal and external validity over the past few decades (Kazdin,

2003; Nock, Janis, & Wedig, 2008). One reason for this devel-

opment in applied settings was the beginning of funding of large

randomized clinical trials (RCTs) by the National Institutes of

Health.

Many such studies require 10 or more years and many

millions of dollars to perform one treatment trial. For instance,

the National Institutes of Mental Health (NIMH) funded

the treatment of depression collaborative research program

(Elkin et al., 1989). This study, which took 13 years to finish

(1977–1990), was reminiscent of earlier efforts such as the

Cambridge Somerville Youth Study conducted from 1935

through 1951, which divided delinquent boys into two groups—

one treatment group and one group that received ‘‘treatment as

usual’’ (McCord, 1978). The fact that there were no effects at 5,

10, 20, or 30 years did much to discourage efforts of this

type for at least the next 30 years. In fact, results from the NIMH

depression collaborative trial were not particularly revealing

either, as no significant differences existed among treatment

and comparison groups at any point in time. Nevertheless,

this trial provoked useful comment and a great deal of

controversy about strategic issues and the potential for

improvement in the methodology of RCTs. These trials have

improved to the point where they have become ‘‘the

gold standard’’ for establishing causal relations between inde-

pendent and dependant variables more generally, and data

emanating from these trials have deep influences on health care

practices (Barlow, 2004).

But is something still lacking? Scientifically, relying on a

relatively small group of researchers requiring enormous

amounts of time and resources to perform a single treatment trial

can be seen as an inefficient method of advancing knowledge. In

applied clinical settings, clinicians often question the applica-

bility of findings from RCTs to individuals seen in typical

clinical settings. In other words, there is a strong perception

that problems exist in generalizing a nomothetic result to an

idiographic situation. The variety of forms that these arguments

take are often cast as specific objections to RCT methodology,

and these arguments have been detailed numerous times in

the past decade (e.g., Persons & Silberschatz, 1998; Westen,

Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004).

Rather than simply critiquing nomothetic methodologies, can

we enrich these methodologies with a complementary focus on

the individual? The fact is that we have a good idea of how to be

more idiographic in our research. Although most psychological

researchers have been trained in group comparisons designs and

have relied primarily on them, exciting advances have been

made in the use of idiographic methodologies, such as the

single-case experimental design (see Barlow et al., 2008).

The flexibility and efficiency of these designs make them ideally

suited for use by psychological scientists, clinicians, and

students alike, given that they require relatively little time and

few resources and subjects and yet they can provide strong

evidence of causal relations between variables.

The time now seems right to put more emphasis on idiographic

strategies that can be integrated in a healthy way into existing

nomothetic research approaches in both clinical and basic

science settings. In clinical science, having established the

effectiveness of a particular independent variable (e.g., an in-

tervention for a specific form of psychopathology), one could

then carry on with more idiographic efforts tracking down

sources of intersubject variability and isolating factors respon-

sible for this variability (Kazdin & Nock, 2003; Nock, 2007).

Necessary alterations in the intervention protocols to effectively

20 Volume 4—Number 1

Why Can’t We Be More Idiographic in Our Research?

address variability could then be tested, once again idiograph-

ically, and incorporated into these treatments. Researchers in

basic science laboratories could undertake similar strategies

and avoid tolerating large error terms. Thus, all of psychological

science, both basic and applied, would benefit.

REFERENCES

Allport, G.D. (1962). The general and the unique in psychological

science. Journal of Personality, 30, 405–422.
Barlow, D.H. (2004). Psychological treatments. American Psychologist,

59, 869–878.
Barlow, D.H., Nock, M.K., & Hersen, M. (2008). Single case experi-

mental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change (3rd ed.).
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Elkin, I., Shea, M.T., Watkins, J.T., Imber, S.D., Sotsky, S.M., Collins,

J.F., et al. (1989). National Institute of Mental Health Treatment

of Depression Collaborative Research Program: General

effectiveness of treatments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46,
971–982, 983.

Kazdin, A.E. (2003). Research design in clinical psychology (4th ed.).
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Kazdin, A.E., & Nock, M.K. (2003). Delineating mechanisms of

change in child and adolescent therapy: Methodological issues

and research recommendations. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 44, 1116–1129.

McCord, J. (1978). A thirty-year follow-up treatment effects. American
Psychologist, 33, 284–289.

Nock, M.K. (2007). Conceptual and design essentials for evaluating

mechanisms of change. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Research, 31, 4S–12S.

Nock, M.K., Janis, I.B., & Wedig, M.M. (2008). Research design. In

A.M. Nezu & M. Nezu (Eds.), Evidence-based outcome research: A
practical guide to conducting randomized controlled trials for
psychosocial interventions (pp. 201–218). New York: Oxford
University Press.

Persons, J.B., & Silberschatz, G. (1998). Are results of randomized

controlled trials useful to psychotherapists? Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 66, 126–135.

Shapiro, M.B. (1961). The single case in fundamental clinical psy-

chological research. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 34,
255–263.

Shapiro, M.B. (1966). The single case in clinical psychological

research. Journal of General Psychology, 74, 3–23.
Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluating experi-

mental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books.
Skinner, B.F. (1966). Operant behavior. In W.K. Honig (Ed.), Operant

behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 12–32). New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Westen, D., Novotny, C.M., & Thompson-Brenner, H. (2004). The

empirical status of empirically supported psychotherapies:

Assumptions, findings, and reporting in controlled clinical trials.

Psychological Bulletin, 130, 631–663.

Volume 4—Number 1 21

David H. Barlow and Matthew K. Nock

What Will You Get?

We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

Premium Quality

Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

Experienced Writers

Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

On-Time Delivery

Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

24/7 Customer Support

Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

Complete Confidentiality

Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

Authentic Sources

We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

Moneyback Guarantee

Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

Order Tracking

You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

image

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

Areas of Expertise

Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

image

Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

Preferred Writer

Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

Grammar Check Report

Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

One Page Summary

You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

Plagiarism Report

You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

Free Features $66FREE

  • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
  • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
  • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
  • Paper Formatting $05FREE
  • Cover Page $05FREE
  • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
  • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
  • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
  • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
image

Our Services

Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

  • On-time Delivery
  • 24/7 Order Tracking
  • Access to Authentic Sources
Academic Writing

We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

Professional Editing

We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

Thorough Proofreading

We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

image

Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

Check Out Our Sample Work

Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

Categories
All samples
Essay (any type)
Essay (any type)
The Value of a Nursing Degree
Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
Nursing
2
View this sample

It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

0+

Happy Clients

0+

Words Written This Week

0+

Ongoing Orders

0%

Customer Satisfaction Rate
image

Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

image

We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

  • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
  • Customized writing as per your needs.

We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

  • Proactive analysis of your writing.
  • Active communication to understand requirements.
image
image

We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

  • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
  • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
Place an Order Start Chat Now
image

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy