For this discussion, complete the following:
Design strategies include the following themes:
Data collection and fieldwork strategies include the following themes:
Analysis strategies include the following themes:
Reference for the article in the attachment
Kirrane, M., Breen, M., & O’Connor, C. (2018). A qualitative investigation of the origins of excessive work behaviour. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 91(2), 235-260. doi:10.1111/joop.12203
Persistent link to where to article was retrieved
http://web.b.ebscohost.com.library.capella.edu/ehost/detail/detail?vid=0&sid=18d121e7-6b24-418f-ae67-6c9a10fde1d6%40pdc-v-sessmgr06&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWhvc3QtbGl2ZSZzY29wZT1zaXRl#AN=129427260&db=bth
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology (2018), 91,
235
–260
© 2018 The British Psychological Society
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com
A qualitative investigation of the origins of
excessive work behaviour
Melrona Kirrane
1
* , Marianne Breen
2
and Cli�odhna O’Connor3
1
Dublin City University Business School, Ireland
2
Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
3
University College Dublin, Ireland
Studies of workers who engage in excessive work behaviour continue to attract the
attention of researchers. Most research in this field adheres to quantitative methodolo-
gies aligned to the addiction or trait paradigms and largely focuses on correlates and
consequences of such behaviour. However, within this literature, empirically based
understandings of the factors that propel individuals to engage in excessive work patterns
are sparse. Resting on socio-cultural theories of work, we adopt a novel approach to this
field of enquiry and examine the genesis of excessive working using a qualitative
methodology. We use discourse analysis to comparatively explore data from a sample of
twenty-eight workers comprising excessive and non-excessive workers. Our study
identified the roles of family of origin, educational experience, and professional norms as
clear drivers of excessive work patterns. Data to support the dominant addiction and trait
paradigms within this research domain were equivocal. Lifestyle decision-making
differentiated the comparison group from the excessive workers. We discuss our
findings with reference to theories of workaholism and consider their implications for the
evolution of this field.
Practitioner points
� Organizational culture can strongly influence the emergence of excessive work patterns among
employees. Human resource professionals and organizational leaders are in a position to intervene in
the development and support of work cultures that are conducive to effective work patterns
� Employee selection and assessment procedures should be sufficiently in-depth to gather relevant
information into the personal backgrounds of applicants
� Employee development initiatives should take account of learned work orientations to ensure the
effectiveness of interventions.
The globalized post-industrial society is characterized by a 24-hour economy (Granter,
McCann, & Boyle, 2015) and has led to the normalization of intensive work (Worrall,
Mather & Cooper, 2016). As research suggests figures of 10 per cent and more of the
working population engage in these lifestyles (Andreassen et al., 2014; Sussman, Lisha, &
Griffiths, 2011), understanding the genesis of these types of work practices is now an
important endeavour. Intensive working is commonly captured by the term ‘worka-
holism’ which initially arose to describe the mindset of individuals most deeply involved in
*Correspondence should be addressed to Melrona Kirrane, Dublin City University Business School, Collins Avenue, Dublin 9,
Ireland (email: melrona.kirrane@dcu.ie).
DOI:10.1111/joop.12203
235
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4831-9411
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4831-9411
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4831-9411
work-focused lifestyles (Oates, 1971). Over the years, the terminology used to describe
such practices has broadened to include work addiction (Robinson, 1998) excessive
overwork (Andreassen, 2013), obsessive passion for work (Vallerand, Paquet, Philippe, &
Charest, 2010), heavy work investment (Golden, 2014; Snir & Harpaz, 2012), work
craving (Wojdylo, Baumann, & Karlsson, 2016), and work over-involvement (Lehr, Koch,
& Hillert, 2010)
1
Most studies of workaholism to date are quantitative investigations of correlates and
consequences of workaholism. One of the strongest outcomes of such work has been the
positioningoftherootofsuchworkingpatternssquarelywithintheindividualworker(van
Wijhe, Schaufeli & Peeters, 2010). However, work patterns are acknowledged to emerge
from an interactive process that occurs between the individual and their environment
(Osipow, 1990). While theorists have signalled the important role of socio-cultural
processes in understanding intensive work patterns (Mazzetti, Schaufeli, & Guglielmi,
2014; Porter, 1996; Snir & Harpaz, 2006, 2012), field studies within this domain remain
disappointingly limited (Sussman, 2012). In this study, we build on socio-cultural theory
(SCT) which highlights dynamic and situation-specific elements of the individual that
together lead to vocational outcomes (Bandura, 1999). Taking a qualitative approach, we
exploretheautobiographicalaccountsofthegenesisofexcessiveworkingpatternsamong
a sample of excessive workers. We compare their accounts with those of a comparison
group of non-excessive workers drawn from the same context. In this way, we provide a
solid foundation for understanding the intense career pathways of such workers.
Theoretical background to workaholism research
Research in the field of workaholism has been dominated by the addiction model and the
trait theory approach (Sussman, 2012). The addiction model considers the phenomenon
to be an irresistible inner drive to work excessively hard (Andreassen & Pallesen, 2016),
and it is described as a progressive, compulsive, potentially fatal disease (Porter, 1996;
Robinson, 1998). Despite the absence of evidence that excessive working shares
psychophysiological characteristics of established definitions of addiction (McMillan, O’
Marsh, & Brady, 2001; Porter, 1996) and its exclusion from the DSM-5 (American
Psychiatric Association, 2013), many researchers continue to draw on the addiction
model of workaholism as a conceptual framework for their work (Andreassen, Griffiths,
Hetland, & Pallesen, 2012; Griffiths, 2011). Such studies typically measure work addiction
quantitatively, and although some recent promising additions have been made
(Andreassen et al., 2012; Schaufeli, Shimazu, & Taris, 2009), the most widely used
measure, the Work Addiction Risk Test (Robinson, Post, & J. Khakee, 1992), is not
regarded as rigorous, rendering research based on it vulnerable to criticisms (Andreassen
et al., 2012; Bowler, Patel, Bowler, & Methe, 2012; Flowers & Robinson, 2002; Sussman,
2012).
A further theoretical paradigm deployed widely in this field is the trait theory
approach. This perspective construes excessive working, associated with traits such as
neuroticism, conscientiousness, narcissism, and perfectionism (Andreassen et al., 2012;
Clark, Lelchook, & Taylor, 2010) as a manifestation of a ‘stable individual difference
characteristic’ (Burke, 2004, p. 421) comprising the psychological dimensions of high
1
For the sake of parsimony and consistency with previous literature, the term ‘workaholism’ will be used in this article, but should
not be taken to necessarily imply agreement with the addiction model of these work
patterns.
236 Melrona Kirrane et al.
work involvement, high drive, and low work enjoyment (Spence & Robbins, 1992).
Although this model has been criticized for its lack of conceptual rigour (Harpaz & Snir,
2003; Robinson, 2001; Scott, Moore, & Miceli, 1997), considerable research continues to
rely on it as a platform for investigation (Burke, Matthiesen, & Pallesen, 2006; Clark et al.,
2010). Unfortunately, resultant isolated correlations have not led to the development of a
coherent theoretical framework (Harpaz & Snir, 2003; Kanai, Wakabayashi, & Fling, 1996;
McMillan, Brady, O’Driscoll, & Marsh, 2002; Mudrack & Naughton, 2001).
While these two theoretical perspectives have driven research streams which have
provided information on the correlates and consequences of intensive work practices
(Baruch, 2011; Giannini & Scabia, 2014; Ng, Sorensen, & Feldman, 2007; Robinson, 2013;
Sussman, 2012), each shows distinct weaknesses and leaves the question of the aetiology
of workaholism empirically unanswered (Quinones & Griffiths, 2015; Spurk, Hirschi, &
Kauffeld, 2016). Moreover, these approaches are characterized by positioning worka-
holism entirely within the individual. Holding some promise of greater refinement of the
genesis of excessive work patterns are studies that explore the contribution of other
factors to this behaviour. These include unsatisfied needs (Burke, 2004; van Beek, Taris, &
Schaufeli, 2011), cognitions (Graves, Ruderman, Ohlott & Weber, 2012), social learning
(Burke, 2001), family dynamics (Chamberlin & Zhang, 2009; Robinson, 2013), and
organizational culture and climate (Keller, Spurk, Baumeler, & Hirschi, 2016; Johnstone &
Johnston, 2005; Mazzetti et al., 2014). In general, such elements have been treated as
peripheral within the dominant research paradigms, and the causal influence of some
have, at times, been explicitly denied (e.g., Robinson, 1998). Although the importance of
these issues has been highlighted (McMillan, O Driscoll, & Burke, 2003), they remain
underexplored in empirical work and their role in the phenomenon of excessive work
patterns remains tentative (Andreassen, 2014; McMillan et al., 2003; van Wijhe et al.,
2010).
Socio-cultural factors and the construal of workaholism
Applying a socio-cultural perspective to understanding the origin of workaholism
represents a rich starting point in research on excessive working patterns. The socio-
cultural approach to understanding behaviour which recognizes the role of norms,
customs, and values of the general population has demonstrated that work norms,
attitudes, and practices are influenced by multiple layers of socio-cultural factors (Kanai &
Wakabayashi, 2004; Lantolf, 2000). At the broadest level is national culture which has a
singular effect on how people construe themselves at work (Brewer & Chen, 2007; Gahan
& Abeysekera, 2009; Triandis, 1990). This effect is perhaps best illustrated by the
phenomenon known as ‘karoshi’, a term coined by Sugisawa and Uehata (1998) to refer to
the particular Japanese phenomenon of death or permanent disability caused by
cardiovascular problems, mediated by excessive work and stress. In Japan, work is
regarded as an element of living in that one is supposed to live in accordance with the
order of society (Ishiyama & Kitayama, 1994; Kanai & Wakabayashi, 2004). Psycho-social
factors such as a social value system that exhorts perseverance and the concept of
‘ganbaru’, which means to suffer in silence and to endure difficulties, are regarded as
perpetuating the syndrome (Meek, 1999, 2004). Considering these features of Japanese
cultural life fosters a deeper understanding and appreciation of the phenomenon of
karoshi and underscores the impact of socio-cultural factors in approaches to work.
A second element of the socio-cultural landscape that has a significant impact on work
behaviours is the familial context (Lawson, Crouter, & McHale, 2015; Piotrowski &
The origins of excessive work behaviour 237
Vodanovich, 2006; Robinson, 2000). The family of origin influences work behaviours as
values, norms, and expectations for achievement are transferred and internalized via
parent–child relations (Schaie & Willis, 1996). This process is well explained by the
expectancy-value theory of achievement (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). The family an
individual creates themselves as a socio-cultural feature also significantly influences
workplace behaviour (Janoski & Wilson, 1995). Involvement in multiple roles causes ‘spill
over’ which effects behaviour and actions of individuals in both contexts (Arnett, 2014;
Livingston, 2014; Wayne, Casper, Matthews, & Allen, 2013).
Educational systems are an integral feature of the socio-cultural landscape and their
influence on workplace behaviours (Billett, 1998; Konkola, Tuomi-Gr€ohn, Lambert, &
Ludvigsen, 2007), are emphasized in Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological model of human
development. By introducing pupils to notions of achievement and authority, coping and
time management skills, this social system provides the intellectual and social skills that
children will use to perform roles within the adult world (Haycock, Hart, & Irvin, 1991;
Tomlinson, 2013). In essence, school educates students on how to become fully
functioning and productive members of society and fosters the development of
appropriate work attitudes and habits deemed important for the continued development
of the social world (Goodlad, 1984; Kourilsky & Walstad, 1998).
Finally, organizational norms of behaviour are a well-established feature of the socio-
cultural environment (Rousseau, 2005; Schein, 1985; Schneider, Ehrhart, & Macey, 2013).
Research has established the potent effects of such norms on workplace behaviour
(Hogan & Coote, 2014; Lee & Yu, 2004), and organizations go to great lengths in fostering
the development of performance-enhancing workplace cultures (O’Reilly, Caldwell,
Chatman, & Doerr, 2014). Taking all these factors together, this literature aptly
demonstrates that to fully understand the origin of excessive work patterns, there is
value to be gained from immersing the study of such behaviour within its socio-cultural
context.
Researching workaholism
According to the epistemology of social constructionism, human knowledge does not
result from individuals’ direct perception of ‘brute reality’, but rather is co-constructed in
social interaction and always mediated by language, interpretations, and values (Berger &
Luckmann, 1996; Potter, 1996). As such, equally important as what does cause the
behaviour patterns termed ‘workaholism’ is what people believe causes it, because the
latter will guide how people manage their own career-related behaviour. To date, this
remains unchartered territory in the empirical literature.
To research workaholism as a discursive construction rather than the predetermined,
yet controversial ‘thing’ pursued in other studies, there is valued to be had in exploring the
insights alternative methodologies may provide. Qualitative methods are ideally suited to
tap the naturalistic, everyday language through which this form of behaviour is
constructed in social interaction. Thus, we pose the following question in an attempt
to address this vacuum: How do people account for the origin of their working patterns?
Method
We position our study within the philosophical orientation of social constructionism
(Neimeyer, 1993), emphasizing the subjective experiences of actors’ ‘lifeworlds’
238 Melrona Kirrane et al.
(Husserl, 1969; Schutz, 1972). Paying close attention to the language used, we apply
discourse analysis techniques to our data (Antaki, 1994; Billig, 1997; Harvey, Turnquist, &
Agostinelli, 1988), looking beyond the surface of the sentence to identify the pragmatic
social functions that the utterance achieves (Silverman, 2001). We present the data in raw
form to accommodate an expansive interpretation of the participants’ perspectives
(Johnson & Waterfield, 2004; Wimpenny & Gass, 2000).
Sampling
Two sampling techniques were used in this study. In the first instance, we deployed a
theory-based sampling process, targeting a sample on the basis of their potential
manifestation of our theoretical construct. For this purpose, we concentrated on
members of Workaholics Anonymous (WA), which is a social network specifically
targeted at self-selected workaholics.
The global WA headquarters (based in the United States) agreed to email details about
the study to its members, and a notice requesting participants for the project was placed in
the WA monthly newsletter. To achieve generalizability (Mason, 2010), we also used a
purposive sampling strategy which involves using prior research and informed ‘hunches’
to identify the segments of the population likely to hold a unique perspective on the
research topic and directly recruiting from these groups (Bauer & Aarts, 2000). Certain
occupational fields, such as financial services, are known for their demanding workloads
(European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions, [EFILWC],
2015). To recruit participants for our study, 110 companies were contacted from the
database of an International Financial Services Centre. Human resource specialists of 72
companies (65%) agreed to disseminate to their employees an invitation from the
researchers to participate in a study on work patterns. Due to this recruitment strategy, it
was impossible to calculate the response rate, as the number of people who received our
invitation was unknown. However, our aim was not to attain a statistically representative
dataset but to provide an in-depth account of the range of ideas present and examine what
underlies and justifies them (Gaskell, 2000; Patton, 2002).
Measure
Machlowitz’s (1980) measure of working patterns was administered via email in the
invitation to participate in the study. The intent of this element of the research process
was not to reify these individuals as ‘workaholics’, but to purposively select people
who indicated that they exemplify characteristics of the construct of ‘workaholism’.
There are 10 items in this measure; a sample item is ‘Do you dread retirement?’
Deployed in a number of studies (Doerfler & Kammer, 1986; Greenberg, 2002; Kilburg,
Nathan, & Thoreson, 1986), with items derived from empirical work rather than a
priori theoretical assumptions, each behaviourally based item on this measure has a
‘yes/no’ response option whereby ‘yes’ responses warrant one point, and ‘no’
responses warrant zero points. A score above eight points is deemed to represent
workaholic behaviour (Machlowitz, 1980). A total of 146 people responded to the
questionnaire, 22 (15%) of whom were identified as workaholics by meeting the cut-off
point established by Machlowitz (1980). This figure is within the range of international
norms regarding the prevalence of workaholism (Doerfler & Kammer, 1986; Freimuth,
Waddell, Stannard, Kelley, & Kipper, 2008; Sussman, 2012). Respondents who agreed
and were available to be interviewed about their work patterns formed this subsample
The origins of excessive work behaviour 239
of the study. In order to fully understand the particular conceptions of the origins of
excessive working, a comparative sample was generated by interviewing willing
respondents who did not meet the criteria for ‘workaholism’ according to Machlowitz
(1980). This afforded the opportunity for the research question to be richly explored
and extensively examined according to the tenets of SCT. The sum of the research
strategies deployed ensured ontological integration of the nature of social life was
achieved (Guarino, 1997).
Sample
The sample ultimately consisted of twelve workaholics, four of whom were WA members,
and sixteen comparison group members. This sample size is acceptable for discourse
analytic studies and is well within the ranges identified by Charmaz (2006), Bertaux
(1981), Morse (2000) and Mason (2010). Of the workaholic sample, three were female
(two members of WA and one general population workaholics [GPW]) and the sample
was aged between 32 and 57 years with an average age of 46 years. Ten of this sample
were married/partnered and job titles included management consultant (5), investment
banker (3), IT consultant (2), journalist (1), and medical doctor (1). Of the comparison
group, five were female and the average age was 47 years. Eleven of this group were
married/partnered, three were divorced, and two were single. Job titles included
management consultant (11), financial services/banking (3), and IT consultant (2).
Procedure
Semi-structured interviews were conducted with each participant (See Appendix). The
interview began with appropriate ‘warm-up’ questions (Arksey & Knight, 1999) and then
proceeded to explore participants’ conceptions on the evolution of their working lives
with the question: ‘What do you think has influenced your work pattern?’ The interview
schedule was employed flexibly to facilitate responsiveness to discursive pathways
introduced by the participant (Gaskell, 2000) and to accommodate issues pertinent to
participants (Gioia, Corley, & Hamilton, 2013).
The researcher did not use the word ‘workaholic’ at any point in the process, and the
neutrality criterion (Guba & Lincoln, 1982) was met by the researcher being aware of, and
critical of vocalizations in the research process. Interviews took place either in private
offices at the participants’ workplaces or nearby convenient spaces and lasted between 60
and 90 min. As WA members were all based in the United States, interviews were
conducted with them by telephone. Interviews were recorded and transcribed verbatim
(O’Connell & Kowal, 1995; Potter & Wetherell, 1987). Each hour of interview data took
approximately ten hours to transcribe.
Data analysis
The analysis followed the discursive action model (Edwards & Potter, 1992), and the
interpretative strategy was informed by the three major foundations of discourse analysis,
namely construction, function, and variability (Potter & Wetherell, 1987). A battery of
discursive features was compiled to aid analysis (Edwards & Potter, 1992; Gee, 1999;
Wetherell, Taylor, & Yates, 2001). Following Guest, Bunce, and Johnson (2006), two
coders separately analysed the data from five interviews. Coding patterns were compared
and a 96% code agreement rating was established (Armstrong, Gosling, Weinman, &
240 Melrona Kirrane et al.
Marteau, 1997). A codebook was then developed using a standard iterative process
(MacQueen, McLellan, Kay, & Milstein, 1998). Codes were refined while reading the
remaining transcripts to accommodate emerging patterns and finally inputted into the
Nvivo software program to facilitate analysis. The analysis met the criteria of trustwor-
thiness (Bowen, 2009; Guba & Lincoln, 1982) by ensuring data credibility, transferability,
dependability, and confirmability using the audit trail, coding checks, and peer debriefing.
Trustworthiness was further reinforced by ensuring all interpretations were supported by
raw data and accompanied by representative verbatim extracts (Speer & Potter, 2000).
The criterion of soundness (Potter & Wetherell, 1987) was satisfied by our presentation of
analysed texts and demonstration of routes to conclusions. This documentation of
procedures enabled accountability to be examined and the confirmability of claims to be
established (Parker, 2002). In addition, only plausible and insightful analyses were
included (Phillips & Hardy, 2002) and it was ensured that all arguments fitted together in
order to provide a coherent reading of the data (Wood & Kroger, 2000). The study thereby
fulfilled the warranting criteria for discourse analysis research (Antaki, Billig, Edwards, &
Potter, 2003; Edwards, 2005).
Results
The data are presented according to the major rift in workaholism literature,
focussing first on the role of internal/dispositional factors, followed by data on the
significance of socio-cultural factors. Findings are displayed according to subgroup
membership (Workaholics [WA members and general population workaholics
{GPW}] and comparison group members (C)). The table below presents a summary
of the findings (Table 1).
Workaholics
a) Internal/dispositional antecedents of excessive work patterns
Overview
Uniform explanations of the internal causes of excessive working by workaholics were
absent from the data. Instead, accounts fell into three primary categories: addiction,
personal choice, and the influence of personal characteristics. WA members invoked
addiction as its primary cause, whereas personal agency was the strongest factor reported
in the data of
GPW.
Detail
i Addiction: WA1 explained her working patterns as a consequence of the hormone
adrenaline, which was defined as an addictive substance:
I am an adrenaline junkie, basically is what I am [WA1]
WA1 constructed a fundamental self-identity as an addict (or in slang terms, ‘junkie’).
This construal of workaholism as an addiction positioned the problem completely within
the self. The label of being an addict was applied without any more detailed construals of
addictive behaviours, symptoms, or signs. Another WA member spoke of his work
patterns using the register of addiction by explicitly comparing work to drugs:
The origins of excessive work behaviour 241
I had what in the programme we call “my stash”. Some people have a stash of drugs, I had a
stash of projects and activities that were never-ending [WA3]
ii Choice: On the other hand, GPW constructed their working style as an active,
volitional choice, and regarded their chosen lifestyle in positive terms. For example,
GPW3 stated:
I like being able to get up at six o’clock in the morning and being able to put in a Fourteen-hour
day [GPW3]
For GPW2, working long hours was positioned as a strategic move rather than an
addictive force. It was not a reward in itself but directed at future benefits, which were
assembled in monetary terms.
I never sacrifice things and invest myself in something unless there’s a pay off or
compensation for it somewhere down the road [GPW2]
iii Trait/disposition: Perfectionism was constructed as a driver of behaviour among
workaholics although the emphasis attributed to it differed between participants from
WA and those from the GPW subgroup. For instance, WA4 stated:
There’s this whole pattern I call “the three P’s”. It’s perfectionism, which leads to paralysis
which leads to procrastination. So perfectionism drives a lot of things. [WA4]
Assembling this chain reaction of events as a ‘pattern’ established it as a general law of
behaviour. This interviewee positioned himself in a powerless stance in relation to
perfectionism, which was afforded agency by installing it as the grammatical subject (e.g.,
‘perfectionism drives’). Perfectionism was also compiled as a behavioural factor among
GPW.
However, it was discussed in less absolute terms:
I’m a bit of a perfectionist. Other people here say that I am one but I don’t know if it is true.
Once I’m satisfied, and once it’s good enough for me, then I’ll move on to the next thing. But
there’s a certain point at which too much perfection gets in your way [GPW8]
This participant stated that others classified him as a perfectionist but that he did not
fully identify with this characterization. He equated perfectionism with an inefficient
Table 1. Summary of findings
Explanatory
mechanisms of
work behaviour Workaholics
Comparison group
Internal factors 1. Addiction (WA)
2. Perfectionism (WA)
3. Personal choice (GPW)
4. Perfection strivings (GPW)
1. Personal maturation
2. Boundary management
3. Value-driven choice
Socio-cultural
factors
5. Stressful family of origin dynamics
6. Intenseeducational norms
7. Pervasive organizational norms
8. National culture
1. Proactive adjustment
2. Supportive family of origin
3. Created family
4. Constructive educational experience
5. Alternating work norms
242 Melrona Kirrane et al.
inability to ‘move on’. While he demanded high standards, he claimed that his ability to
reach satisfaction with a completed task made him, at most, ‘a bit of’ a perfectionist.
b) Socio-cultural attributions for work patterns
Overview
A rich body of socio-cultural data emerged pertaining to the influence of family
background, educational history, organizational/work context, and cultural context on
work behaviours. These elements speak strongly to the role of environmental factors in
encouraging the development of certain work behaviours.
Detail
i Family background: No reference was made to the role of created family in the
development of excessive work patterns – family of origin was invoked instead. For
example:
So even from an early age I was working. My father was a holy terror for work, work, work,
work. He’d kick me out of bed at seven o’clock on a Saturday morning – that was the way I was
brought up. I would always have worked [GPW7]
This participant presented himselfas working demanding hours from an early age. This
was positioned as not due to his own nature or personality, but rather due to his father’s
influence. The participant presented himself as agentless in determining the amount of
work he did as a child by employing verbs that situate him in a passive position. Being
‘kicked out of bed’ established his father’s control over his activity. His father’s work ethic
was couched in negative terms, and the home context was cast as creating his lifelong
working behaviour through the extreme case formulation ‘I would always have worked’.
An inevitability of the development of excessive behaviour emerged in the data from WA3:
Both my parents are nicotine and coffee addicts. I just grew up in a very disturbed home. My
mom has got a lot of issues like anxiety and my dad’s a little more on the control side of things.
So between the two of them, it’s like I’m constitutionally wired for addiction. [WA3]
WA3 used the ‘addict’ term to categorize his parents. This categorization was
construed as a statement of fact through the lack of hedging. The modifier ‘just’ was
employed to simplify the construction of his family context as disorderly. Terms such as
‘issues’ and ‘anxiety’ established his mother as psychologically unstable and built up this
extract as a legitimate fact.
As a result, he regarded himself as being inevitably addictive in his behaviour thus
legitimizing his construction of his work addiction as being created in the family home.
WA1 explained her work behaviours as developing in childhood in response to the
low-status position she believed she held within her family unit:
Both us girls – it was perfectly clear that we were second rate. I was called the runt of the litter,
the cowardly one, and my way out of that was that I was clever and I did well at school. That
was the one area where I got some approval from my father. [WA1]
This participant linked her current work orientations to her childhood desire to escape
from paternal taunting and her humble familial status. The clarity of her standing within
the family was built up through the use of the adjective ‘perfectly’. Thus, trying to gain
The origins of excessive work behaviour 243
approval from the family context was positioned as causing her to develop excessive work
patterns.
ii Education: Educational experiences were also construed as factors that influenced
work practices. GPW1 offered:
I always worked very hard at school. I used to get just two or three hours of sleep at night. . . so
yes I would say that my school environment has influenced my current work pattern. [GPW1]
This participant construed herself as consistently being a hard-working student,
through the extreme case formulation of ‘always’. The negative effects of working hard
were constructed as leading to physiological costs of not sleeping which were built up
through specific temporal details. These details functioned to construe a fresh perceptual
memory. Working hard and sleeping for a fraction of the night were established as her
typical behaviour. GPW4 described the evolution of intensive work patterns as emerging
as a result of his time in university:
I worked very hard at university. Iwas there on a running scholarship so Iwas getting up at five
thirty in the morning, training with the team, going to an eight thirty class, coming back and
studying for a couple of hours, training until six then going to a job. So I’d been working very
hard for a number of years. [GPW4]
iii Professional norms: Performance expectations were also invoked as leading to
excessive work patterns. WA2 said:
My experience in the corporate world is that workaholism is extremely insidious within it. It is
a disease that is rewarded in this culture. [WA2]
This participant positioned workaholism as an illness that was incentivized in her work
experience. The global term (‘corporate world’) was employed to construct the effect of
the broader context which suggested that these challenges transcended any one
individual. This perspective received further endorsement from WA4:
This is an incredibly workaholic place to work. 9 a.m. to 1 a.m. the following morning is not
unusual. You’re expected to put your private life second to your work so I don’t suppose that
has helped in my pattern of workaholism. [WA4]
The prominence of the ‘you’ personal pronoun externalized workaholism from the
participant and situated it within the work context. Prioritizing work over one’s private
life was assembled as being a requirement of the job. The role-talk of GPW6 also
normalized and externalized this behaviour to the work environment:
I work in an investment bank and it is an intense regime. Everybody else, your peer group, is
doing the same number of hours so you don’t really see it as a problem [GPW6]
iv Culture: Differences in cultural context were also positioned as leading to different
work patterns.
For example:
In America they want people to succeed and excel – it’s a great system and so I’ve sort of
adopted that mentality [GPW5]
GPW5 described himself as flexibly aligning himself with this ethos, although he
hedged this statement with the modifier ‘sort of’. National culture was thus depicted as an
issue of the mind, affecting work behaviour by instilling a worldview or ‘mentality’.
244 Melrona Kirrane et al.
Comparison group
a) Internal/dispositional antecedents of excessive work patterns
Overview
The most notable feature in this data was the role of personal agency with respect to work
behaviours. Further elements identified as explanatory features of work practices
included commitment to retaining work–nonwork boundaries and significant life
incidents which led to a re-ordering of priorities.
Detail
i Personal decision-making: The issue of proactive choice regarding work patterns
emerged in the data from this subgroup. The manner in which this was done included
implementing time boundaries as described by C12:
I do my work from nine until six and then get out and do something else with my life. [C12]
Another strategy involved acting on the basis of valued goals. For example:
I decided to come off the seniority treadmill because they really do expect your heart and your
soul and your life to get anywhere. [C5]
Evident in the prominence of first-person phrases such as ‘I decided’, this participant
was constructed as having a strong sense of agency in relation to her work pattern. The
intolerance of below-par performance was established through extreme case formula-
tions such as ‘heart, soul, life’. The prominence of the ‘you’ personal pronoun further
established this as a law external to this particular context (Potter, 1996). Members of this
subgroup also construed their work pattern as emerging from rational decision-making.
For example:
I’m inclined to overwork by nature but I took a decision that I wasn’t going to do it. [C4]
This participant constructed altering work patterns as overriding natural tendencies.
This was worked up through the verb ‘took’ which positioned her as being in control of
her work pattern. The prominence of the ‘I’ personal pronoun constructed a strong sense
of agency. The absence of hedging assembled this extract as a statement of fact (Edwards
& Potter, 1992). Adjusting habitualized work patterns was construed as requiring some
achievable effort:
You have to be determined, to stick to and once you do that, you get back into it. It’s all about
habit, really. I didn’t find it too difficult. [C6]
ii Perseverance: This latter quote from C6 also demonstrates how being committed to
the new work behaviour was worked up as a necessary requirement, given the
habitual nature of work patterns. The prominence of the general ‘you’ pronoun
distanced this construal from his situation and thus positioned it as a universal law
(Edwards & Potter, 1992).
iii Proactive adjustment: Personal tragedy was also worked up to explain work
behaviours. C1 said:
The origins of excessive work behaviour 245
I have a sister who died about eleven years ago and almost the last thing she said to me was to
‘stop doing what I have done’, which was work too hard. That had a big impact on me, a big
impact. [C1]
The effect of this event on C1 was constructed as a physical impression through
the repetition of the phrase ‘a big impact’. The authenticity of this account
was assembled through the temporal details and the direct speech quote from her
sister.
iv Maturation: Timely personal development was invoked to explain work behaviours
in a number of ways. Some participants referred to age-related changes in personality
and preferences:
I’m just not as ambitious as I was when I was younger. [C4]
The manner in which ambition manifested itself was constructed as subject to
variation, and dependent on age rather than it being a central feature of her personhood.
Similarly, C3 described her approach to work as having changed over time:
I used to get hooked on the adrenaline of crisis management like lots of people but I prefer a
more planned approach. [C3]
This behavioural dynamic was generalized to the majority of people which
constructed her argument as less extreme. However, she established herself as getting
tired of this approach and a sense of agency was worked up through the prominence of
the ‘I’ personal pronoun.
v Available resources: Approaches to work were also attributed to energy resources
rather than personality characteristics:
My dog had puppies – they’d be fast asleep for hours and then they go mad and then their
energy levels drop – so I think I’m a bit like that. I like the excitement or the buzz of a deadline
so I don’t get bored but I get exhausted from it. [C9]
The movement from the analogy of the puppies to this participant’s work pattern was
established through the switch from ‘they’ to the ‘I’ personal pronoun. The excitement of
working against the clock was positioned as a way of working that this participant found
enjoyable but tiring.
b) Socio-cultural attributions for work patterns
Overview
A broad range of socio-cultural factors were supplied to further explain work practices,
including family environment, educational experience, and organizational norms.
Detail
i Family background: Both family of origin and created family were worked up as
influencing work patterns. With reference to the former, C2 said:
There was never any pressure put on us at home to go to third level, but there was this kind of
unwritten understanding between myself and my parents that you work to the best of your
ability. [C2]
246 Melrona Kirrane et al.
In this extract, performance expectations were positioned as an implicit agreement
between C2 and her parents. There was a switch from the ‘I’ personal pronoun to the
‘you’ pronoun which distanced her working behaviour from this agreement and
established it as an external law (Potter, 1996). Features of the created family were also
identified as effecting
work patterns:
I think it is easier to put in more hours when you don’t have a family. I don’t ever see myself
working any longer than nine to five or half five. [C5]
Working long hours and having a family were construed as difficult and
problematic to combine. C5 switched from the ‘I’ personal pronoun to the ‘you’
pronoun which functioned to distance this statement from her personal philosophy
and constructed these contextual conditions as fact (Edwards & Potter, 1992).
Another participant described how features of their created family led to an alteration
in work patterns:
I just have better things to be doing just now with my son – he is twenty months now and I just
want to spend time with him and spend time with just the family at home. [C7]
In this extract, engagement in family activities was positioned as more desirable than
work. The modifier ‘just’ was repeated four times which functioned to attribute the
change in the work style to a single aspect of his life, namely fatherhood. A further family
feature cited as influencing work patterns was marital disharmony. C10 said:
I used to work long hours to escape my relationship with my wife. I think that was a symptom
of a marriage breaking down, more than anything else. [C10]
C10 positioned his excessive work patterns as a relief from an unsatisfactory home life.
The extreme case formulation, ‘more than anything else’, positioned this as an accurate
interpretation of his behaviour. Medical terminology (‘a symptom of’) was used to
construct excessive working as being a reaction to and indicative of a failing marriage.
ii Educational experiences: Participants construed lessons learned in the school
environment as core to their approach to work:
My school environment taught me to be fairly disciplined and taught me how to work hard. I
suppose you could say discipline and hard work – that’s what school taught me to bring to the
job. [C11]
Working hard was positioned as an activity that was learned, through the repetition of
the verb ‘taught’. Discipline and hard work were construed as being learnt prior to
entering the workplace.
iii Organizational norms: Respondents reported on the role of different norms across
diverse work environments and their corresponding adaptation to them. For example,
C13 said:
I always change the way I work depending on the job I’m in. I think that’s just the way you
evolve through your jobs. [C13]
This approach to work was expanded beyond his personal circumstances through the
use of the generic terms ‘you’ and ‘your’. These terms externalized this construal from her
specific case and constructed it as a universal tendency (Potter, 1996). Particular work
The origins of excessive work behaviour 247
roles and departments within organizations were also construed as leading to specific
work patterns. C14 proffered:
Oh I worked very long hours there. It was eight o’clock in the morning to eleven o’clock at
night, five days a week. It was very heavy going . . .but I was trying to get my feet under the
table properly and trying to understand what was going on. [C14]
The length of working hours was emphasized and tied into a particular workplace.
The reason for working a 75-hr week was clearly constructed as a necessary
requirement for becoming familiar with a new job. This was built up through the verbs
‘struggling’ and ‘trying’, as well as the idiomatic phrases ‘to get on top of things’ and
‘get my feet under the table properly’. However, he was construed as being in control
of his work behaviour through the personal pronouns ‘I’ (Edwards & Potter, 1992).
Geographic location and national culture were also construed as relevant in relation to
work patterns:
Some of the American companies expect their employees to work crazy hours, but that’s not
for me. My company is British-based and it’s not expected. Japanese and American firms have
such a different work ethic. [C8]
National culture was positioned as leading to an obligation to work long hours
which was departicularized from individuals to pervasive expectations about work
practices.
Epilogue
In presenting this analysis, it is important not to establish a false dichotomy between
internal and external modes of explanation, as the two frequently interacted in
participants’ discourses to form complex, multifaceted accounts. For instance, while
speaking about the societal factors assembled to explain excessive working practices,
WA4 stipulated that acknowledging these influences did not amount to a renunciation of
personal responsibility:
Actually I think the problem is in me. I don’t blame society – it’s up to me to regulate my
behaviour. It’s not them outside. Why should anybody outside help me regulate my own
behaviour? I’m the one that has to do it. I don’t blame society for my workaholism, I take full
responsibility for it. I’m not a victim [WA4]
Another example of the interweaving of social context and individual causality lay in
the commentary of WA3:
My dad’s a work addict. I think he has that going on back in his ancestral realm. I think there’s
like an inter-generational heritage of addiction in my family. I would consider one of my sisters
to be work-addicted. So for me, I feel like it’s a mixture. I was probably both genetically and
psychologically predisposed for the disease and then I was around people who also had that
behaviour. [WA3]
WA3 unambiguously categorized his parents as addicts and a long-standing history of
family addiction was clearly constructed. By classifying his sister as a work addict, he
emphasized the causality of the common family environment. Genetic and social
explanations for his addictive tendencies were not mutually exclusive, but they co-existed
in one coherent account.
248 Melrona Kirrane et al.
Discussion
This paper makes an original contribution to the workaholism literature, expanding it
theoretically, conceptually, methodologically and practically. The novel employment of a
qualitative social constructionist approach allowed the research to move beyond a reified
view of workaholism to empirically explore its genesis as a socio-cultural construct. Our
inclusion of a comparison group of workers provided a further opportunity to identify the
role of socio-cultural factors in development of excessive work patterns. Together these
approaches allowed the study identify rich and novel insights into how those who work
excessively explain, justify, and account for their behaviour
Data from the workaholic group revealed that they understood the origin of their work
patterns to include many socio-cultural features such as family background, educational
experience, and professional norms. While the role of family systems (Robinson, 2001;
Stevens-Smith, 1994) and professional norms (Mazzetti et al., 2014) have been identified
in the workaholism literature, our findings elaborate the complex influential pathways of
these factors. Notably, our identification of the impact of the educational environment on
the development of work patterns is a new addition to this domain of enquiry and signals a
new trajectory for research in this domain. We encourage researchers to consider Wigfield
and Eccles’ (2000) expectancy-value theory of achievement as a vehicle to further explore
the role of educational experiences in the development of work behaviours. The related
theoretical frameworks of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1999), and locus of control (Rotter,
1966), along with theories focused on reasons for engaging with certain activities such as
flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 2014), self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2010), and
self-worth theory (Covington, 1998) may also be helpful frameworks to guide future
research in this area.
The personality trait of perfectionism was also referenced in the data of workaholics.
While this is consistent with certain findings from the trait theory literature (e.g., Clark
et al., 2010; Liang & Chu, 2009), other documented personality links (e.g., the ‘Big Five’
[Andreassen, Hetland, & Pallesen, 2010; Burke et al., 2006; ]) were not intuited by this
sample. This suggests that there is value to be had in taking an expansive consideration of
the role of traits in the emergence of workaholism. The role of addiction also appeared in
the findings and the addiction framework has long been invoked to explain workaholism
(Andreassen et al., 2012; Griffiths, 2011). However, only those who were members of WA
referenced addiction, all of whom had previously attended other addiction programmes.
As such, their identity as addicts extended beyond the work environment to capture a
pervasive mode of relating to the world (Cain, 1991; Carnes, Murray, & Charpentier,
2005). It is interesting to note that these individuals did not mention related facets of
addiction such as withdrawal symptoms, increased tolerance, brownouts, or blackouts
(Walters, & Gilbert, 2000). In these data, perhaps the status of ‘work addiction’ may have
been more rhetorical than ontological.
Addiction was not referenced by any other workaholic in the study despite all meeting
the same criteria for categorization as a workaholic. Instead, GPW evinced personal
choice, the antithesis of addiction, as an explanation for their excessive work patterns.
This choice led to need satisfaction in terms of a sense of accomplishment and achieving
valued rewards, outcomes aligned with Vallerand et al.’s (2010) concept of harmonious
passion. While van Beek et al. (2011) identified ‘engaged workaholics’ among random
users of an Internet site, our study confirms that controlled and autonomous motivation
indeed drives behaviour of excessive workers (Van den Broeck et al., 2011). Given the
differences in explanations of excessive work patterns from workaholics, diverging
The origins of excessive work behaviour 249
according to membership and non-membership of WA, our data query the explanation of
workaholism provided by the addiction paradigms.
While the comparison group name-checked many of the same factors as explanations
for their work patterns, certain differences were notable in their accounts such as their
inclusion of created family as an influencing feature of their work patterns. Most
interesting was the broad articulation of personal agency in conforming to or rejecting
socio-cultural features of work environments such as professional norms (Bandura, 1999;
Savickas, 2008). Explicit in their descriptions were decisions to abandon previous
excessive work patterns, begging the question whether the research had uncovered
‘former workaholics’. While studies have identified routes to recovery from workaholic
behaviours (Bakker, Demerouti, Oerlemans, & Sonnentag, 2013), our findings suggest
that supporting personal agency in this regard may be a productive avenue of future
research.
An important finding of this research is the identification of the multidimensional
nature of lay explanations of workaholism. Traditionally, attribution theory positions
internal and external attributions as oppositional: Heider (1958) proposed a ‘hydraulic’
relation between internal and external attribution, such that the more there is of one, the
less of the other. However, research examining explanations offered spontaneously in
natural conversation suggests that people understand the world in terms of ‘intuitive
interactionism’: single-cause explanations in terms of either internal or external factors
are rare (Antaki, 1994). This is borne out in the current data. For instance, accounts of
workaholism were at one point attributed to the internal trait of perfectionism, but were
then also attributed to the influence of parenting and schooling. Similarly, research on lay
understandings of substance addiction confirms that people invoke multiple explanatory
factors (including biology, character, emotion, the social environment, learning, and drug
properties) and explicitly site the cause of addiction in the interactions between them
(Folkman, 2013; Meurk, Carter, Hall, & Lucke, 2014). This study, where WA participants
identified external intervening factors in their ‘addiction’, indicates that lay understand-
ings of workaholism are similarly multifactorial.
Taken together, our findings reveal a rich tapestry of factors beyond the boundaries of
the trait and addiction paradigms that illuminate the genesis of workaholism. The results
expand our understanding of the manner in which socio-cultural processes affect
workaholism (Kanai & Wakabayashi, 2004) and support family systems theory (Stevens-
Smith, 1994), socio-cultural theory (Lantolf, 2000) and theories of personal agency
(Bandura, 1999) as vehicles for understanding the origins of workaholism. Moreover, our
findings endorse the work of Mazzetti, Schaufeli, Guglielmi, and Depolo (2016) and
Johnstone & Johnston (2005) who identified the important role of the work environment
in encouraging excessive work patterns.
Our findings are directly related to the methodological approach adopted which was
formative in revealing the complex, layered understandings that characterize natural
thinking. The productive outcomes of this study suggest that studies of workaholism
should revisit its early roots in qualitative research (Machlowitz, 1980). Analysis of other
cultural material, such as popular psychology texts, mass media, or Internet chatrooms,
would shed further light on social constructions of workaholism. Another avenue to
pursue is an analysis of attitudes to excessive work in company reports, mission
statements and other corporate literature (e.g., Craig & Amernic, 2011), and an
ethnographic account of the informal processes through which these attitudes are
transmitted to employees.
250 Melrona Kirrane et al.
Limitations
While acceptable sample sizes in qualitative research vary according to the method-
ology deployed (Bertaux, 1981; Charmaz, 2006; Mason, 2010; Morse, 2000), a larger
sample size in future studies would be desirable. Stronger response rates could be
encouraged by direct organizational endorsement of the research (Anseel, Lievens,
Schollaert, & Choragwicka, 2010), site visits by researchers (Couper, Traugott, &
Lamias, 2001; Fricker & Schonlau, 2002), and completion incentives (Rose, Sidle, &
Griffith, 2007; Yu & Cooper, 1983). While qualitative research does not aim to be
statistically representative, diversifying the sample by people who have different
‘stakes’ in the issue would provide for a more holistic overview of the topic.
Researchers could consider using strategies to enhance response rates such as
emphasizing the value of the research, consent pre-screening, and social network
approaches as suggested by Cycyota and Harrison (2006). In this study, participants
were mostly drawn from the corporate sphere. It would be interesting to explore how
excessive investment in work is construed in occupational groups where roles are
characterized by more manual or emotional labour. It is also important to note that this
study attracted a disproportionate number of male participants. This meant that the
study afforded limited insight into the gendered nature of working lives (Majeed,
Forder, Kendig, & Byles, 2015; Sabelis & Schilling, 2013). This matter should be
addressed by future research, with an acknowledgement that ‘work’ is not restricted to
remunerated employment outside the home. Future longitudinal research would also
be useful in disentangling the trajectory through which people’s self-understanding
moves from hard worker to work addict. Finally, while our study explored current
work patterns, our question regarding the derivation of work behaviours necessarily
required participants to engage in recall. Evidence indicates that error and bias are not
unusual in memory retrieval (Shiffman et al., 1997). Future research should use
‘cognitive interviewing’ methods that are sensitive to the workings of autobiographical
memory in order to improve recall accuracy (Means, Habina, Swan, & Jack, 1992)
2
.
Nevertheless, in accordance with the principles of social constructionism (Burr, 2015),
the factual accuracy of these memories may be of secondary importance to their
narrative significance: the causal attributions revealed in our research are those that
individuals recruit in actively constructing their personal biography. It is these personal
constructions that guide individuals’ understandings of, emotional responses to and
attempts to change their work practices. As a result, for researchers and practitioners
interested in excessive working, understanding lay beliefs about the origins of work
practices, irrespective of their factual accuracy, is equally (or even more) critical than
understanding the empirically substantiated causal processes.
Conclusion
Lay accounts of a social phenomenon are of great empirical value, not only for their
detail but also for the validity conferred by accounts of first-hand experience of a
phenomenon. As the first study to directly explore lay constructions of workaholism,
this research offers an important contribution to the literature. People’s accounts of the
many external forces that influenced their working lives challenge the dominant
individualistic approaches to workaholism. Correspondingly, the retention of a sense of
2
We are grateful to one anonymous reviewer for pointing out this issue.
The origins of excessive work behaviour 251
agency challenges a totalistic view of work patterns as entirely dictated by economic and
political powers. The academic literature on workaholism should heed the wisdom of
‘workaholics’ themselves and seek to forge theoretical frameworks that acknowledge
the diversity of factors involved and enlighten how they interconnect in the genesis of
excessive work habits.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(DSM-5
�
). Washington, DC: Author.
Andreassen, C. S. (2013). Work addiction. In P. Miller (Ed.), Principles of addiction: Comprehensive
addictive behaviours and disorders (pp. 837–845). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.
1016/B978-0-12-398336-7.00084-X
Andreassen, C. S. (2014). Workaholism: An overview and current status of the research. Journal of
Behavioural Addictions, 3, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.2.2013.017
Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Hetland, H., Kravina, L. F., Jensen, F. S., & Pallesen, S. (2014). The
prevalence of workaholism: A survey study in a nationally representative sample of Norwegian
employees. PLoS ONE, 9, e102446. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102446
Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Hetland, J., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a work
addiction scale. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 265–272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1467-9450.2012.00947.x
Andreassen, C. S., Hetland, J., & Pallesen, S. (2010). The relationship between ‘workaholism’, basic
needs satisfaction at work and personality. European Journal of Personality, 24, 13–17.
Andreassen, C. S., &Pallesen, S.(2016). Workaholism: An addiction to work. The Neuropathology of
Drug Addictions and Substance Misuse, 3, 1–12.
Anseel, F., Lievens, F., Schollaert, E., & Choragwicka, B. (2010). Response rates in organizational
science, 1995–2008: A meta-analytic review and guidelines for survey researchers. Journal of
Business and Psychology, 25, 335–349. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-010-9157-6
Antaki, C. (1994). Explaining and arguing: The social organisation of accounts. London, UK: Sage.
Antaki, C., Billig, M., Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (2003). Discourse analysis means doing analysis: A
critique of six analytic shortcomings. Discourse Analysis Online, 1
Arksey, H., & Knight, P. T. (1999). Interviewing for social scientists: An introductory resource with
examples. London: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209335
Armstrong, D., Gosling, A., Weinman, J., & Marteau, T. (1997). The place of inter-rater reliability in
qualitative research: An empirical study. Sociology, 31, 597–606. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0038038597031003015
Arnett, J. J. (2014). Adolescence and emerging adulthood. New York, NY: Pearson Education
Limited.
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., Oerlemans, W., & Sonnentag, S. (2013). Workaholism and daily
recovery: A day reconstruction study of leisure activities. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
34(1), 87–107. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1796
Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Asian Journal of Social
Psychology, 2, 21–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.00024
Baruch, Y. (2011). The positive wellbeing aspects of workaholism in cross cultural perspective: The
chocoholism metaphor. Career Development International, 16, 572–591. https://doi.org/10.
1108/13620431111178335
Bauer, M. W., & Aarts, B. (2000). Corpus construction: A principle for qualitative data collection. In
M. W. Bauer & G. Gaskell (Eds.), Qualitative researching with text, image and sound (pp. 19–
37). London, UK: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209731
Berger, P. L.,& Luckmann, T. (1996). Thesocial construction of reality: A treatise in the sociology of
knowledge. Garden City, NY: Anchor.
252 Melrona Kirrane et al.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-398336-7.00084-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-398336-7.00084-X
https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.2.2013.017
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102446
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00947.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00947.x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-010-9157-6
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209335
https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038597031003015
https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038597031003015
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1796
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.00024
https://doi.org/10.1108/13620431111178335
https://doi.org/10.1108/13620431111178335
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209731
Bertaux, D. (1981). From the life-history approach to the transformation of sociological practice. In
D. Bertaux (Ed.), Biography and society: The life history approach in the social sciences (pp.
29–45). London, UK: Sage.
Billett, S. (1998). Transfer and social practice. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Vocational
Education Research, 6(1), 1.
Billig, M. (1997). The dialogic unconscious: Psychoanalysis, discursive psychology and the nature of
repression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 139–159. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-
8309.1997.tb01124.x
Bowen, G. A. (2009). Supporting a grounded theory with an audit trail: An illustration. International
Journal of Social Research Methodology, 12, 305–316. https://doi.org/10.1080/
13645570802156196
Bowler, J. L., Patel, A. S., Bowler, M. C., & Methe, S. E. (2012). A meta-analysis of workaholism.
International Journal of Business and Management, 7, 2–17.
Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y.-R. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward
conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychological Review, 114, 133–151.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University
Press.
Burke, R. J. (2001). Workaholism in organizations: The role of organizational values. Personnel
Review, 30(6), 637–645. https://doi.org/10.1108/EUM0000000005977
Burke, R. J. (2004). Introduction: Workaholism in organisations. Journal of Organisational Change
Management, 17, 420–423.
Burke, R. J., Matthiesen, S. B., & Pallesen, S. (2006). Personality correlates of workaholism.
Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1223–1233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.
10.017
Burr, V. (2015). Social constructionism. London and New York: Routledge.
Cain, C. (1991). Personal Stories: Identity acquisition and self- understanding in Alcoholics
Anonymous. Ethos, 19, 210–253. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1991.19.2.02a00040
Carnes, P. J., Murray, R. F., & Charpentier, L. (2005). Bargains with chaos: Sex addicts and addiction
interaction disorder. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 12, 79–120. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10720160500201371
Chamberlin, C. M., & Zhang, N. (2009). Workaholism, health, and self-acceptance. Journal of
Counseling & Development, 87(2), 159–169. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2009.tb
00563.x
Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative
analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Clark, M. A., Lelchook, A. M., & Taylor, M. L. (2010). Beyond the Big Five: How narcissism,
perfectionism, and dispositional affect relate to workaholism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 48, 786–791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.013
Couper, M. P., Traugott, M. W., & Lamias, M. J. (2001). Web survey design and administration. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 65, 230–253. https://doi.org/10.1086/322199
Covington, M. V. (1998). The will to learn: A guide for motivating young people. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Craig, R., & Amernic, J. (2011). Detecting linguistic traces of destructive narcissism at-a distance in a
CEO’s letter to shareholders. Journal of Business Ethics, 101, 563–575. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10551-011-0738-8
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). Toward a psychology of optimal experience. Flow and the
foundations of positive psychology (pp. 209–226). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer
Netherlands.
Cycyota, C. S., & Harrison, D. A. (2006). What (not) to expect when surveying executives: A meta-
analysis of top manager response rates and techniques over time. Organizational Research
Methods, 9(2), 133–160. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428105280770
The origins of excessive work behaviour 253
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1997.tb01124.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1997.tb01124.x
https://doi.org/10.1080/13645570802156196
https://doi.org/10.1080/13645570802156196
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133
https://doi.org/10.1108/EUM0000000005977
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.017
https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1991.19.2.02a00040
https://doi.org/10.1080/10720160500201371
https://doi.org/10.1080/10720160500201371
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2009.tb00563.x
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2009.tb00563.x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.013
https://doi.org/10.1086/322199
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-011-0738-8
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-011-0738-8
https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428105280770
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Self-determination. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://
doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0834
Doerfler, M. C., & Kammer, P. P. (1986). Workaholism, sex, and sex role stereotyping among female
professionals. Sex Roles, 14, 551–560. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287455
Edwards, D. (2005). Discursive psychology. In K. L. Fitch & R. E. Sanders (Eds.), Handbook of
language and social interaction (pp. 257–273). New York: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis
Group.
Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London, UK: Sage.
European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions (2015). Financial
services sector: Working conditions and job quality. Luxembourg: Office for Official
Publications of the European Communities.
Flowers, C. P., & Robinson, B. E. (2002). A structural and discriminant analysis of the Work Addiction
Risk Test. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 517–526. https://doi.org/10.
1177/00164402062003008
Folkman, S. (2013). Stress: Appraisal and coping. New York, NY: Springer.
Freimuth, M., Waddell, M., Stannard, J., Kelley, S., & Kipper, A. (2008). Expanding the scope of dual
diagnosis and co-addictions: Behavioral addictions. Journal of Groups in Addiction & Recovery,
3, 137–160. https://doi.org/10.1080/15560350802424944
Fricker, R. D., & Schonlau, M. (2002). Advantages and disadvantages of Internet research surveys:
Evidence from the literature. Field Methods, 14, 347–367. https://doi.org/10.1177/
152582202237725
Gahan, P., & Abeysekera, L. (2009). What shapes an individual’s work values? An integrated model of
the relationship between work values, national culture and self-construal. The International
Journal of Human Resource Management, 20, 126–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09585190802528524
Gaskell, G. (2000). Individual and group interviewing. In M. W. Bauer & G. Gaskell (Eds.),
Qualitative researching with text, image and sound (pp. 38–56). London, UK: Sage. https://
doi.org/10.4135/9781849209731
Gee, J. P. (1999). An introduction to discourse analysis: Theory and method. London: Routledge.
Giannini, M., & Scabia, A. (2014). Workaholism: An addiction or a quality to be appreciated. Journal
of Addiction Research & Therapy, 5(3), 1–9.
Gioia, D. A., Corley, K. G., & Hamilton, A. L. (2013). Seeking qualitative rigor in inductive research
notes on the Gioia methodology. Organisational Research Methods, 16, 15–31. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1094428112452151
Golden, L. (2014). Distinctions between overemployment, overwork, workaholism and heavy
investments in work time. In I. Harpaz & R. Snir (Eds), Heavy work investment (pp. 140–171).
New York and London: Routledge, https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2371082
Goodlad, J. I. (1984). A place called school: Prospects for the future. New York, NY: HarperCollins.
Granter, E., McCann, L., & Boyle, M. (2015). Extreme work/normal work: Intensification,
storytelling and hypermediation in the (re) construction of ‘the New Normal’. Organisation,
22, 443–456.
Graves, L. M., Ruderman, M. N., Ohlott, P. J., & Weber, T. J. (2012). Driven to work and enjoyment of
work: Effects on managers’ outcomes. Journal of Management, 38(5), 1655–1680. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0149206310363612
Greenberg, J. S. (2002). Comprehensive stress management (8th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Griffiths, M. (2011). Workaholism: A 21st century addiction. The Psychologist: Bulletin of the
British Psychological Society, 24, 740–744.
Guarino, N. (1997). Understanding, building and using ontologies. International Journal of
Human-Computer Studies, 46(2–3), 293–310. https://doi.org/10.1006/ijhc.1996.0091
Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1982). Epistemological and methodological bases of naturalistic
inquiry. Educational Communications and Technology Journal, 30, 233–252.
254 Melrona Kirrane et al.
https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0834
https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0834
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287455
https://doi.org/10.1177/00164402062003008
https://doi.org/10.1177/00164402062003008
https://doi.org/10.1080/15560350802424944
https://doi.org/10.1177/152582202237725
https://doi.org/10.1177/152582202237725
https://doi.org/10.1080/09585190802528524
https://doi.org/10.1080/09585190802528524
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209731
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849209731
https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428112452151
https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428112452151
https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2371082
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206310363612
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206310363612
https://doi.org/10.1006/ijhc.1996.0091
Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). How many interviews are enough? An experiment with
data saturation and variability. Field Methods, 18, 59–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1525822X05279903
Harpaz, I., & Snir, R. (2003). Workaholism: Its definition and nature. Human Relations, 56,
291–319. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726703056003613
Harvey, J. H., Turnquist, D. C., & Agostinelli, G. (1988). Identifying attributions in oral and written
explanations. In C. Antaki (Ed.), Analysing everyday explanation: A casebook of methods
(pp. 32-42). London, UK: Sage.
Haycock, K. P., Hart, P., & Irvin, J. J. (1991). Improving student achievement through partnerships.
Alexandria, VA: American Association for Higher Education, AAHE Office of School/College
Collaboration.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. https://doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
Hogan, S. J., & Coote, L. V. (2014). Organizational culture, innovation, and performance: A test of
Schein’s model. Journal of Business Research, 67, 1609–1621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusre
s.2013.09.007
Husserl, E. (1969). Formal and transcendental logic. The Hague, the Netherlands: Springer Science
and Business Media.
Ishiyama, I., & Kitayama, A. (1994). Overwork and career-centered self-validation among the
Japanese: Psychological issues and counselling implications. International Journal for the
Advancement of Counselling, 17, 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01407732
Janoski, T., & Wilson, J. (1995). Pathways to voluntarism: Family socialization and status
transmission models. Social Forces, 74(1), 271–292. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/74.1.271
Johnson, R., & Waterfield, J. (2004). Making words count: The value of qualitative research.
Physiotherapy Research International, 9, 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1471-2865
Johnstone, A., & Johnston, L. (2005). The relationship between organizational climate, occupational
type and workaholism. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 34, 181–188.
Kanai, A., & Wakabayashi, M. (2004). Effects of economic environmental changes on job demands
and workaholism in Japan. Journal of Organisational Change Management, 17, 537–548.
https://doi.org/10.1108/09534810410554533
Kanai, A., Wakabayashi, M., & Fling, S. (1996). Workaholism among employees in Japanese
corporations: An examination based on the Japanese version of the Workaholism Scales.
Japanese Psychological Research, 38, 192–203. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.1996.tb
00024.x
Keller, A. C., Spurk, D., Baumeler, F., & Hirschi, A. (2016). Competitive climate and workaholism:
Negative sides of future orientation and calling. Personality and Individual Differences, 96,
122–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.061
Kilburg, R. R., Nathan, P. E., & Thoreson, R. W. (1986). Professionals in distress: Issues, syndromes,
and solutions in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.
org/10.1037/10056-000
Konkola, R., Tuomi-Gr€ohn, T., Lambert, P., & Ludvigsen, S. (2007). Promoting learning and transfer
between school and workplace. Journal of Education and Work, 20, 211–228. https://doi.org/
10.1080/13639080701464483
Kourilsky, M. L., & Walstad, W. B. (1998). Entrepreneurship and female youth: Knowledge,
attitudes, gender differences, and educational practices. Journal of Business Venturing, 13, 77–
88. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0883-9026(97)00032-3
Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Introducing sociocultural theory. Sociocultural Theory and Second Language
Learning, 1, 1–26.
Lawson, K. M., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (2015). Links between family gender socialization
experiences in childhood and gendered occupational attainment in young adulthood. Journal
of Vocational Behaviour, 90, 26–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2015.07.003
Lee, S. K. J., & Yu, K. (2004). Corporate culture and organizational performance. Journal of
Managerial Psychology, 19(4), 340–359.
The origins of excessive work behaviour 255
https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X05279903
https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X05279903
https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726703056003613
https://doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.09.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.09.007
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01407732
https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/74.1.271
https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1471-2865
https://doi.org/10.1108/09534810410554533
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.1996.tb00024.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.1996.tb00024.x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.061
https://doi.org/10.1037/10056-000
https://doi.org/10.1037/10056-000
https://doi.org/10.1080/13639080701464483
https://doi.org/10.1080/13639080701464483
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0883-9026(97)00032-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2015.07.003
Lehr, D., Koch, S., & Hillert, A. (2010). Where is (im)balance? Necessity and construction of
evaluated cut-off points for effort–reward imbalance and over-commitment. Journal of
Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 83, 251–261. https://doi.org/10.1348/
096317909X406772
Liang, Y.-W., & Chu, C.-M. (2009). Personality traits and personal and organisational inducements:
Antecedents of workaholism. Social Behaviour and Personality, 37, 645–660. https://doi.org/
10.2224/sbp.2009.37.5.645
Livingston, B. A. (2014). Bargaining behind the scenes spousal negotiation, labor, and work– family
burnout. Journal of Management, 40, 949–977. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311428355
Machlowitz, M. (1980). Workaholics: Living with them, working with them. Reading, MA: Addison-
Wesley.
MacQueen, K., McLellan, E., Kay, K., & Milstein, B. (1998). Codebook development for team-based
qualitative analysis. Cultural Anthropology Methods, 10, 31–36.
Majeed, T., Forder, P., Kendig, M. H., & Byles, J. (2015). A gendered approach to workforce
participation patterns over the life course for an Australian baby boom cohort. Journal of
Vocational Behaviour, 87, 108–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2014.12.004
Mason, M. (2010). Sample size and saturation in PhD studies using qualitative interviews. Forum
Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 11, 1–19.
Mazzetti, G., Schaufeli, W. B., & Guglielmi, D. (2014). Are workaholics born or made? Relations of
workaholism with person characteristics and overwork climate. International Journal of Stress
Management, 21, 227–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035700
Mazzetti, G., Schaufeli, W. B., Guglielmi, D., & Depolo, M. (2016). Overwork climate scale:
Psychometric properties and relationships with working hard. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 31, 880–896. https://doi.org/10.1108/JMP-03-2014-0100
McMillan, L. H., Brady, E. C., O’Driscoll, M. P., & Marsh, N. V. (2002). A multifaceted validation
study of Spence and Robbins’ (1992) Workaholism Battery. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 75, 357–368. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317902320
369758
McMillan, L. H., O Driscoll, M. P., & Burke, R. J. (2003). Workaholism: A review of theory, research,
and future directions. International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 18,
167–190.
McMillan, L. H., O’Driscoll, M. P., Marsh, V., & Brady, E. C. (2001). Understanding workaholism: Data
synthesis, theoretical critique, and future design strategies. International Journal of Stress
Management, 8, 69–91. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009573129142
Means, B., Habina, K., Swan, G. E., & Jack, L. (1992). Cognitive research on response error in survey
questions on smoking. (Publication No. PHS 92-1080). HyattsviUe, MD: U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services.
Meek, C. B. (1999). Ganbatte: Understanding the Japanese employee. Business Horizons, Jan-Feb:
27- 46.
Meek, C. B. (2004). The dark side of Japanese management in the 1990s. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 19, 312–331. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940410527775
Meurk, C., Carter, A., Hall, W., & Lucke, J. (2014). Public understandings of addiction: Where do
neurobiological explanations fit? Neuroethics, 7, 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-013-
9180-1
Morse, J. M. (2000). Determining sample size. Qualitative Health Research, 10(1), 3–5. https://doi.
org/10.1177/104973200129118183
Mudrack, P. E., & Naughton, T. J. (2001). The assessment of workaholism as behavioural tendencies:
Scale development and preliminary empirical testing. International Journal of Stress
Management, 8, 93–111. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009525213213
Neimeyer, R. A. (1993). An appraisal of constructivist psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.61.2.221
256 Melrona Kirrane et al.
https://doi.org/10.1348/096317909X406772
https://doi.org/10.1348/096317909X406772
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2009.37.5.645
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2009.37.5.645
https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206311428355
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2014.12.004
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035700
https://doi.org/10.1108/JMP-03-2014-0100
https://doi.org/10.1348/096317902320369758
https://doi.org/10.1348/096317902320369758
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009573129142
https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940410527775
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-013-9180-1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-013-9180-1
https://doi.org/10.1177/104973200129118183
https://doi.org/10.1177/104973200129118183
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009525213213
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.61.2.221
Ng, T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L., & Feldman, D. C. (2007). Dimensions, antecedents, and consequences
of workaholism: A conceptual integration and extension. Journal of Organisational
Behaviour, 28, 111–136. https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1379
Oates, W. E. (1971). Confessions of a workaholic: The facts about work-addiction. New York, NY:
The World Publishing Company.
O’Connell, D. C., & Kowal, S. (1995). Basic principles of transcription. In J. A. Smith, R. Harre & L.
Van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 93–105). London, UK: Sage.
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446221792
O’Reilly, III, C. A., Caldwell, D. F., Chatman, J. A., & Doerr, B. (2014). The promise and problems of
organizational culture: CEO personality, culture, and firm performance. Group & Organization
Management, 39, 595–625. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059601114550713
Osipow, S. H. (1990). Convergence in theories of career choice and development: Review and
prospect. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 36, 122–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791
(90)90020-3
Parker, L. D. (2002). Twentieth-century textbook budgetary discourse: Formalization, normalization
and rebuttal in an Anglo-Saxon environment. European Accounting Review, 11(2), 305–327.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09638180220125535
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative interviewing. Qualitative Research and Evaluation Methods, 3,
344–347.
Phillips, N., & Hardy, C. (2002). Discourse analysis: Investigating processes of social construction.
Sage University Paper Series on Qualitative Research Methods, Vol. 50. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412983921
Piotrowski, C., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2006). The interface between workaholism and work-family
conflict: A review and conceptual framework. Organisation Development Journal, 24, 84–92.
Porter, G. (1996). Organisational impact of workaholism: Suggestions for researching the negative
outcomes of excessive work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 70–84. https://
doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.70
Potter, J. (1996). Representing reality: Discourse, rhetoric and social construction. London, UK:
Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446222119
Potter, J., & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social psychology: Beyond attitudes and
behaviour. London, UK: Sage.
Quinones, C., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Addiction to work: A critical review of the workaholism
construct and recommendations for assessment. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental
Health Services, 53(10), 48–59. https://doi.org/10.3928/02793695-20150923-04
Robinson, B. E. (1998). The workaholic family: A clinical perspective. American Journal of Family
Therapy, 26, 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1080/01926189808251087
Robinson, B. E. (2000). Workaholism: Bridging the gap between workplace, sociocultural, and
family research. Journal of Employment Counseling, 37(1), 31–47. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.
2161-1920.2000.tb01024.x
Robinson, B. E. (2001). Workaholism and family functioning: A profile of familial relationships,
psychological outcomes, and research considerations. Contemporary Family Therapy, 23,
123–135. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007880301342
Robinson, B. E. (2013). Chained to the desk: A guidebook for workaholics, their partners and
children, and the clinicians who treat them (3rd ed.). New York, NY: New York University
Press.
Robinson, B. E., Post, P., & J. Khakee, J. (1992). Test-retest reliability of the Work Addiction Risk Test.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 926. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.74.3.926
Rose, D. S., Sidle, S. D., & Griffith, K. H. (2007). A penny for your thoughts: Monetary incentives
improve response rates for company-sponsored employee surveys. Organizational Research
Methods, 10, 225–240. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428106294687
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.
Psychological Monographs, 80(1), 609.
The origins of excessive work behaviour 257
https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1379
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446221792
https://doi.org/10.1177/1059601114550713
https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(90)90020-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(90)90020-3
https://doi.org/10.1080/09638180220125535
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412983921
https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.70
https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.70
https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446222119
https://doi.org/10.3928/02793695-20150923-04
https://doi.org/10.1080/01926189808251087
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1920.2000.tb01024.x
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-1920.2000.tb01024.x
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007880301342
https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.74.3.926
https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428106294687
Rousseau, D. (2005). Psychological contracts in organisations: Understanding written and
unwritten agreements. London, UK: Sage.
Sabelis, I., & Schilling, S. E. (2013). Editorial: Frayed careers: exploring rhythms of working lives.
Gender, Work and Organisation, 20, 127–132. https://doi.org/10.1111/gwao.12020
Savickas, M. L. (2008). Helping people choose jobs: A history of the guidance profession. In J. A.
Athanason & R. Van Esbroeck (Eds.), International handbook of career guidance (pp. 97–113).
Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6230-8
Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (1996). Psychometric intelligence and aging. In F. Blanchard-Fields &
T. M. Hess (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging (pp. 293–322).
New York, NY: McGraw Hill.
Schaufeli, W. B., Shimazu, A., & Taris, T. W. (2009). Being driven to work excessively hard: The
evaluation of a two-factor measure of workaholism in the Netherlands and Japan. Cross-Cultural
Research, 43, 320–348. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397109337239
Schein, E. H. (1985). Organisational culture and leadership: A dynamic view. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Schneider, B., Ehrhart, M. G., & Macey, W. H. (2013). Organizational climate and culture. Annual
Review of Psychology, 64, 361–388. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143809
Schutz, A. (1972). The phenomenology of the social world. London, UK: Heinemann.
Scott, K. S., Moore, K. S., & Miceli, M. P. (1997). An exploration of the meaning and
consequences of workaholism. Human Relations, 50, 287–314. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
1016986307298
Shiffman, S., Hufford, M., Hickcox, M., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., & Kassel, J. D. (1997). Remember that? A
comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking lapses. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.65.2.292.a
Silverman, D. (2001). Interpreting qualitative data: Methods for analysing talk, text and
interaction (2nd ed.). London, UK: Sage.
Snir, R., & Harpaz, I. (2006). The workaholism phenomenon: a cross-national perspective. Career
Development International, 11, 374–393. https://doi.org/10.1108/13620430610683034
Snir, R., & Harpaz, I. (2012). Beyond workaholism: Towards a general model of heavy work
investment. Human Resource Management Review, 22, 232–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
hrmr.2011.11.011
Speer, S. A., & Potter, J. (2000). The management of heterosexist talk: Conversational resources and
prejudiced claims. Discourse & Society, 11, 543–572. https://doi.org/10.1177/09579265000
11004005
Spence, J. T., & Robbins, A. S. (1992). Workaholism: Definition, measurement, and preliminary
results. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 160–178. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532775
2jpa5801_15
Spurk, D., Hirschi, A., & Kauffeld, S. (2016). A new perspective on the etiology of workaholism: The
role of personal and contextual career-related antecedents. Journal of Career Assessment, 24
(4), 747–764. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072715616127
Stevens-Smith, P. (1994). Contextual issues in addiction. In J. A. Lewis (Ed.), Addictions: Concepts
and Strategies for Treatment (pp. 11–21). Gaithersburg, MD: An Aspen Publication.
Sugisawa, A., & Uehata, T. (1998). Onset of peptic ulcer and its relation to work-related factors and
life events: A prospective study. Journal of Occupational Health, 40, 22–31. https://doi.org/10.
1539/joh.40.22
Sussman, S. (2012). Workaholism: A review. Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy, 6, 1–18.
Sussman, S., Lisha, N., & Griffiths, M. (2011). Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority
or the minority? Evaluation and the Health Professions, 34, 3–56. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0163278710380124
Tomlinson, M. (2013). Education, work and identity: Themes and perspectives. London:
Bloomsbury.
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation.
258 Melrona Kirrane et al.
https://doi.org/10.1111/gwao.12020
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6230-8
https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397109337239
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143809
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016986307298
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016986307298
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.65.2.292.a
https://doi.org/10.1108/13620430610683034
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2011.11.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2011.11.011
https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926500011004005
https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926500011004005
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5801_15
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5801_15
https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072715616127
https://doi.org/10.1539/joh.40.22
https://doi.org/10.1539/joh.40.22
https://doi.org/10.1177/0163278710380124
https://doi.org/10.1177/0163278710380124
Vallerand, R., Paquet, Y., Philippe, F., & Charest, J. (2010). On the role of passion for work in
burnout: A process model. Journal of Personality, 78, 289–312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
1467-6494.2009.00616.x
van Beek, I., Taris, T. W., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2011). Workaholic and work engaged employees: Dead
ringers or worlds apart? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16, 468. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0024392
Van den Broeck, A., Schreurs, B., De Witte, H., Vansteenkiste, M., Germeys, F., & Schaufeli, W.
(2011). Understanding workaholics’ motivations: A self-determination perspective. Applied
Psychology, 60, 600–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00449.x
Van Wijhe, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Peeters, M. C. (2010). Understanding and treating workaholism:
Setting the stage for successful interventions. In R. J. Burke & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Risky business:
Psychological, physical and financial costs of high risk behavior in organizations (pp. 107–
134). New York: Gower Publishing.
Walters, G. D., & Gilbert, A. A. (2000). Defining addiction: Contrasting views of clients and experts.
Addiction Research, 8(3), 211–220. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066350009004421
Wayne, J. H., Casper, W. J., Matthews, R. A., & Allen, T. D. (2013). Family-supportive organisation
perceptions and organisational commitment: The mediating role of work– family conflict and
enrichment and partner attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 606–622. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0032491
Wetherell, M., Taylor, S., & Yates, S. J. (Eds.) (2001). Discourse as data: A guide for analysis.
London: Sage.
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.
1015
Wimpenny, P., & Gass, J. (2000). Interviewing in phenomenology and grounded theory: Is there a
difference? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31, 1485–1492. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-
2648.2000.01431.x
Wojdylo, K., Baumann, N., & Karlsson, W. (2016). Do I feel ill because I crave for work or do
I crave for work because I feel ill? A longitudinal analysis of work craving, self-regulation, and
health. Journal of Behavioural Addictions, 5, 90–99. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.
2016.005
Wood, L. A., & Kroger, R. O. (2000). Doing discourse analysis: Methods for studying action in talk
and text. London: Sage.
Worrall, L., Mather, K., & Cooper, C. L. (2016). The changing nature of professional and
managerial work: Issues and challenges from an empirical study of the UK (pp. 60–85).
Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.
Yu, J., & Cooper, H. (1983). A quantitative review of research design effects on response rates to
questionnaires. Journal of Marketing Research, 20, 36–44. https://doi.org/10.2307/3151410
Received 30 August 2017; revised version received 8 January 2018
The origins of excessive work behaviour 259
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00616.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00616.x
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024392
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024392
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2011.00449.x
https://doi.org/10.3109/16066350009004421
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032491
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032491
https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015
https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2000.01431.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2000.01431.x
https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.005
https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.005
https://doi.org/10.2307/3151410
Appendix : Interview schedule
Introduction
This study is investigating work patterns. We will be asking you questions about your
approach to work. There are no right or wrong answers so please feel free to answer at
length.
Questions
1. Could you please tell me what a typical day at work is like for you?
Probes
� What hours do you typically work? Start/finish?
� Week-end / holiday work?
2. What do you think drives you or motivates you to work the way you do?
Probes
� What aspects, if any, of your character do you think influence
your work pattern?
� What aspects, if any, of your personal circumstances do you think may have influenced
your work pattern?
� home environment
� school or college attended
� work environment
� society
Extra question for Workaholics Anonymous members
� Why did you join Workaholics Anonymous?
260 Melrona Kirrane et al.
Copyright of Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology is the property of Wiley-
Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder’s express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.
Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.
Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.
Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.
Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.
Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.
We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.
Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.
You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.
Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.
Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.
You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.
You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.
Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.
We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.
We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.
We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.
Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!
Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality
Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.
We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.
We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.
We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.
We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.