one and halffff

No copy please 

on time 

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
one and halffff
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

  

Use the link please and use the reading also 

Assignment Discussion Brief #5. What do you think of the concept of acculturation for health research? Do you think this model can be improved? What aspects, dimensions or specifics (ie Bornstein) do you think are critical to consider from sociocontextual/socioecological perspective? Do you think US has true “culture of destination”?Your discussion brief should be 1.5 page maximum in length, double-spaced, 12 point font

Use the link please and use the reading also

American Journal of Public Health | August 2006, Vol 96, No. 81342 | Commentary | Peer Reviewed | Abraído-Lanza

 COMMENTARY 

Toward a Theory-Driven Model of Acculturation in
Public Health Research

| Ana F. Abraído-Lanza, PhD, Adria N. Armbrister, MA, Karen R. Flórez, MPH, and Alejandra N. Aguirre, MPHInterest in studying the
impact of acculturation on
immigrant health has in-
creased in tandem with the
growth of the Latino popu-
lation in the United States.
Linear assimilation models
continue to dominate public
health research despite the
availability of more complex
acculturation theories that
propose multidimensional
frameworks, reciprocal in-
teractions between the indi-
vidual and the environment,
and other acculturative proc-
esses among various Latino
groups.

Because linear and uni-
dimensional assessments
(e.g., nativity, length of stay
in the United States, and
language use) provide con-
stricted measures of accul-
turation, the rare use of
multidimensional accultura-
tion measures and models
has inhibited a more com-
prehensive understanding
of the association between
specific components of ac-
culturation and particular
health outcomes. A public
health perspective that in-
corporates the roles of struc-
tural and cultural forces in
acculturation may help iden-
tify mechanisms underlying
links between acculturation
and health among Latinos.
(Am J Public Health. 2006;
96:1342–1346. doi:10.2105/
AJPH.2005.064980)

BECAUSE OF ITS ORIENTATION
in and emphasis on health dis-
parities, the field of public health
should pay particular attention
to the impact of acculturation
on the health of Latinos. Latinos
are currently the largest ethnic
minority group in the United
States, numbering 35.3 million
persons and comprising 12.5%
of the country’s population.1

Although the percentage varies
across the different Latino
groups, more than two thirds
(65.2%) of Latinos (excluding
Puerto Ricans) living in the
United States are foreign-born.2

This large proportion of immi-
grants illustrates, in part, the
importance of considering
acculturation in research on
the health of Latinos.

Although definitions vary, ac-
culturation is broadly described
as the process by which individu-
als adopt the attitudes, values,
customs, beliefs, and behaviors
of another culture.3,4 The process
of acculturation presents numer-
ous challenges and life changes
that could potentially benefit or
adversely affect the health of im-
migrants as well as subsequent
US-born generations. Therefore,
it is important to consider accul-
turation processes when studying
the health of all Latinos in the
United States.

In the social and behavioral sci-
ences, there is a rich theoretical
literature on acculturation; how-
ever, models from this literature
have not been applied to much
public health research. Theoreti-
cally grounded studies of accul-
turation could provide effective

analytic tools for current efforts to
address health disparities among
Latinos. Because of its orientation
toward and examination of large-
scale structural and cultural forces
that promote health, prevent dis-
ease, and affect illness experi-
ences, a public health perspective
on acculturation may offer a
deeper understanding of Latino
health. Thus, a public health ap-
proach could contribute much to
the development and refinement
of acculturation theory and simul-
taneously address the health
needs of Latino populations in the
United States.

ACCULTURATION THEORY
AND MEASUREMENT

Research on acculturation and
health has not kept pace with ac-
culturation theory. As illustrated
in detailed reviews of the accul-
turation literature,3–7 in the early
1900s numerous social scientists
offered various acculturation the-
ories. The most influential mod-
els were set forth by sociologists
from the human ecological
school of thought, most notably
Park.8 In essence, Park proposed
a linear and directional process
by which loss of the original cul-
ture occurs through greater ac-
culturation. Despite the evolution
of more elaborate paradigms in
the social and behavioral sci-
ences, these linear assimilation
models were adopted by much
of the public health research on
acculturation.

With few notable exceptions,9

reciprocal acculturation processes,
or the influence of immigrant

groups on American society, re-
main virtually untested,7 as do
other more expansive contempo-
rary theories. Such models posit
orthogonal relations between the
original and the new culture,10 re-
sulting in various orientations that
include biculturalism (e.g., strong
adherence to both Mexican and
American value systems). Other
multidimensional models propose
typologies on the basis of cultural
awareness and ethnic loyalty.11

Still others postulate that immi-
grants selectively adopt traits and
behaviors from the new culture,
especially those traits leading to
increased economic and social
mobility, while maintaining cer-
tain values from their original
culture.3,4

There are numerous scales
available to measure accultura-
tion, perhaps reflecting its di-
verse conceptualizations. Al-
though the measurement of
acculturation is a matter of con-
troversy and debate12 in public
health literature, indexes of ac-
culturation that predominate are
nativity or generational status,
length of residence in the
United States, and language use.
These simple descriptors are
useful in laying the groundwork
for acculturation and for de-
scribing the heterogeneity of the
Latino population, but they are
limited in their ability to capture
all the nuances of acculturation
and to tap directly immigrants’
adherence to American values.
Furthermore, such proxy mea-
sures largely reflect the linear
and directional assumption of
earlier acculturation theories. To

August 2006, Vol 96, No. 8 | American Journal of Public Health Abraído-Lanza | Peer Reviewed | Commentary | 1343

 COMMENTARY 

address some of these problems,
multidimensional scales have
been developed to tap domains
such as language, food, and
music preferences; the extent of
social ties and contacts with
friends of the same ethnic
group; parents’ place of birth;
ethnic identity; and social affilia-
tion with Latinos versus
Anglos.9,13,14 However, many of
these scales are scored by sum-
ming across items (with greater
scores reflecting strong adher-
ence to Anglo culture). Such
procedures minimize the utility
of measuring the multiple di-
mensions of acculturation.15,16

Greater advances in research
on acculturation and health
could be made if acculturation
were represented as a “latent
variable” with various indicators.
This allows the measurement of
Latino cultural “worldviews” or
belief systems, values (e.g., indi-
vidualism vs familialism), linguis-
tic preferences, and other behav-
iors and preferences, which
would represent the latent vari-
able of acculturation. Moreover,
the use of more elaborate statis-
tical techniques, such as struc-
tural equations,17 allows for the
associations between specific in-
dicators of acculturation and var-
ious health outcomes to be mod-
eled. Furthermore, because
health behaviors encompass a
disparate array of variables,
ranging from dietary practices to
the use of social support systems,
acculturation may be measured
best by considering factors rele-
vant to the particular health
issue at hand, rather than by a
monolithic “acculturation” con-
cept. For example, research on
obesity may be best served by
asking specific acculturation
questions on nutrition (e.g., ad-
herence to “traditional” diets
consisting of low-fat foods such

as beans, rice, and vegetables) or
other culturally based behaviors
(e.g., attitudes about exercise).
This would help identify the spe-
cific components of acculturation
that are associated with particu-
lar health outcomes.18

Many existing theories and
scales disregard historical, socie-
tal, and other structural factors,
as well as social dynamics that
promote and maintain specific
acculturation orientations or pat-
terns, such as biculturalism.
Pochismo, which is the fusion and
crystallization of American and
Mexican cultural elements that
evolved among Mexican Ameri-
cans of the southern border of
the United States,19 is a salient
example of this type of bicultur-
alism. In fact, pochismo could be
considered a distinct and free-
standing culture with its own
language (Spanglish), music, and
identity that evolved from the
dynamic and reciprocal interac-
tion of Mexican and American
cultures in the border region and
that would prove very difficult to
assess with current acculturation
measures.

As described in subsequent
sections, however, historical and
sociopolitical factors that influ-
ence immigration vary across the
different Latino groups. There-
fore, the specific types of social
and structural factors that should
be taken into account may de-
pend on the particular Latino
group being studied.

ACCULTURATION AS A
HEALTH RISK OR A
PROTECTIVE FACTOR

The integration of accultura-
tion theory into public health re-
search could advance the study
of various Latino health issues.
With respect to global health
indicators, such as all-cause

mortality and life expectancy,
there is growing evidence of bet-
ter health among Latinos than
among non-Latino Whites.20–24

However, high levels of accul-
turation among Latinos are asso-
ciated with increased rates of
cancer, infant mortality, and
other indicators of poor physical
and mental health.3,25 With
some exceptions,26 rates of risky
health behaviors (e.g., smoking,
alcohol use, high body mass
index) also increase with accul-
turation.17,23,27–33 These findings
suggest that, in the process of
acculturation, Latinos may be
exposed to different risk factors
or may adopt unhealthy behav-
iors that result in shifts in mor-
bidity and mortality for various
diseases.

The results are not all nega-
tive, however. Although accul-
turation is a “risk factor” for
myriad unhealthy behaviors,
there is also some evidence that
it is associated with several
healthy behaviors, such as
greater exercise and leisure-time
physical activity.17,27,28,34 The
observations that acculturation
can be both a risk and a protec-
tive factor for various health be-
haviors requires further study.17

However, research on these is-
sues has been hampered by the
measurement problems de-
scribed in the previous section.
For example, multidimensional
scales may be useful in identify-
ing specific components of ac-
culturation, such as norms con-
cerning smoking or alcohol
consumption, that present risk
or protective factors for particu-
lar health problems, such as to-
bacco use or binge drinking.9

The role of acculturation as a
risk and protective factor also
raises some intriguing theoreti-
cal issues and unanswered
questions.

NEED FOR THEORETICAL
MODELS

Despite growing evidence of
the association between accul-
turation and health behaviors
among Latinos in the United
States, few theories have been
proposed to explain these ef-
fects. In general, there is a great
lack of theoretical models on ac-
culturation and physical health
outcomes.35

Acculturation may be a proxy
for other variables, such as pro-
longed exposure to stressful
events or adverse circumstances,
including those associated with
immigration and eventual settle-
ment, or disadvantaged social
status. Although proxy variables
have not been fully investigated,
the adverse effect of accultura-
tion on health is not always at-
tenuated when adjusting for so-
cial disadvantage confounders
(specifically, socioeconomic sta-
tus).17,25 To date, there is a lack
of research on theoretical models
concerning the mechanisms by
which acculturation affects
health. Acculturation may affect
health behaviors as a conse-
quence of coping responses to
discrimination and poverty; loss
of social networks; exposure to
different models of health behav-
ior; and changes in identity, be-
havioral prescriptions, beliefs,
values, or norms.

An underlying assumption in
the literature is that beliefs or
norms concerning particular be-
haviors change with greater ac-
culturation. These mutable be-
liefs and norms are seldom
tested, however. One recent
study indicated that the majority
(almost 75%) of less accultur-
ated Latinas considered it worse
to be a smoker than to be obese,
and the majority (nearly 75%)
of more acculturated Latinas

American Journal of Public Health | August 2006, Vol 96, No. 81344 | Commentary | Peer Reviewed | Abraído-Lanza

 COMMENTARY 

held the opposite opinion.36 If
we assume that these norms are
reflected in women’s behavior
concerning smoking and mainte-
nance of weight, they are consis-
tent with some observations in
the literature (e.g., that accultura-
tion increases the odds of smok-
ing and exercise among Lati-
nas)27,28,37,38 but not others
(e.g., that acculturation increases
the likelihood of high body mass
index).27,28,30

Overall, changes in values,
belief systems, and worldviews
have remained unexplored in
public health research on accul-
turation and health outcomes.
Yet a growing literature docu-
ments the importance of consid-
ering the impact of acculturation
on these psychosocial variables
and their role in shaping the
health of Latinos.35,39 Whether
cultural values and other psycho-
social mechanisms—as well as
their associated effects on
health—decline with greater ac-
culturation remains a question
for further research.

ACCULTURATION TO
WHAT?

Another critical theoretical
question concerns the reference
culture to which Latinos are ac-
culturating. Although the refer-
ence group is not always speci-
fied,12 implicit in much research
on acculturation is the unwritten
understanding that White Ameri-
cans are the standard makers for
“American-ness.” In many studies
and measures, the assumption is
that increased acculturation
brings immigrants’ values and be-
haviors in line with a standard-
ized set of values, primarily those
associated with “White American
culture.”16(p39) Positing that White
culture is the reference point for
acculturation may misrepresent

acculturation and limit the under-
standing of complex health re-
sponses and outcomes among
Latinos. Therefore, a fuller
understanding of acculturation
processes among Latinos must in-
clude the interactions of Latinos
with other groups of color (whose
ability to disappear in the main-
stream is limited). This approach
must take into consideration the
prevalence of racial conflict and
the degree to which the dualistic
racial system is embedded in the
United States.40

Segmented assimilation the-
ory, which portrays immigrants
and their subsequent generations
as complex and active members
of their lived environments, pre-
sents an alternative to the as-
sumption that White culture is
invariably the reference point for
acculturation.5 Segmented assimi-
lation refers to diverse patterns
of adaptation whereby immigrant
groups differentially adopt the
attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors
of divergent cultural groups in
the United States. For example,
whereas some second-generation
Haitian adolescents do follow a
“standard” pattern of assimilation
to middle-class White America,
others adopt the values and
norms of Black inner-city youth.40

Thus, the segmented assimilation
framework documents various
potential patterns of accultura-
tion, highlighting the importance
of considering varied reference
groups and diverse patterns of
adaptation.

Pivotal to the concept of seg-
mented assimilation is the ac-
knowledgment of structural con-
straints faced by ethnic minority
groups, who often reside in large
metropolitan areas with high
rates of residential segregation
and racism.41 Often, the synergis-
tic effects of segregation and
racism isolate residents from

amenities and services and con-
centrate large numbers of mi-
norities in economically disad-
vantaged urban areas. The
inevitable interaction occurring
at economic, political, cultural,
and social levels between differ-
ent ethnic groups living in multi-
ethnic neighborhoods (e.g., Do-
minicans living with African
Americans in the South Bronx) is
largely neglected by the accultur-
ation literature. A paucity of
studies attempt to measure the
extent to which Latinos report
closeness or ideological familiar-
ity with African Americans.42

We are aware of no public health
studies that examine whether
Latinos adopt the culture of
other ethnic minority populations
in the United States, how struc-
tural factors (e.g., residential seg-
regation) may operate to promote
“ethnic minority acculturation,”
or the impact of this process on
health.

Issues such as acculturation in
the context of residential segrega-
tion, racism, and other deleteri-
ous aspects of life as a minority
in the United States demand at-
tention from a forward-thinking
public health research commu-
nity. Equally important to this
reconceptualized version of ac-
culturation is the exploration of
how ethnic enclaves might affect
health positively or negatively
through cultural, economic, and
social mechanisms.5,40 A public
health approach should consider
contextual and structural factors
in acculturation and challenge
the popular notion of White
American culture as the “accul-
turation standard.” This approach
could offer some innovative
methods to understand health
disparities in lived environments
while also effectively describing
the reality of minority groups in
the United States.

GENDER AND AGE

Research is also needed to bet-
ter understand why the effect of
acculturation on certain health
behaviors varies by gender and
age or developmental stage. For
example, as Latino men and
women acculturate, their alco-
hol use and smoking patterns
reflect the gender-related behav-
ioral norms in the United
States.31,33,38,43 In studies of
youths, greater acculturation in-
creases the likelihood of alcohol
use and smoking44–46; however,
acculturation operates with other
psychosocial factors pertinent to
the adolescent life stage (e.g.,
peer influence, low self-esteem,
self-efficacy to resist smoking and
alcohol use) to determine risky
health behaviors.47–49 Such find-
ings challenge the assumption of
a direct relation between accul-
turation and health behaviors.
These findings further illustrate
the need for more comprehen-
sive theoretical models that in-
corporate structural and contex-
tual factors, as well as mediating
variables, to explain the associa-
tion between acculturation and
health among Latinos in the
United States. Further studies
should also examine whether be-
haviors that are attributed to ac-
culturation (e.g., tobacco and al-
cohol use), instead reflect stages
of development or gender norms.

THE COMPLEXITIES OF
“CULTURE”

Simplifying culture into “eth-
nic,” “assimilated,” or other “risk”
categories (e.g., “high” vs “low”
acculturation) can inadvertently
fuel weak explanations of health
disparities by focusing attention
on culture rather than on struc-
tural constraints (e.g., lack of ac-
cess to resources).50 Yet much

August 2006, Vol 96, No. 8 | American Journal of Public Health Abraído-Lanza | Peer Reviewed | Commentary | 1345

 COMMENTARY 

current research uses proxies of
culture and acculturation without
examining the societal contexts
that promote or inhibit health.
The role of individual agency on
health can be overestimated if
structural constraints are not con-
sidered.51 For example, US immi-
gration policy was amenable to
Cuban immigrants fleeing the
Castro regime (especially in the
1960s–1970s), and federal set-
tlement-assistance programs (e.g.,
the US Migration and Refugee
Assistance Act of 196252) were
established specifically to assist
them. Partly because of the up-
ward social mobility afforded by
these programs, today Cubans
are among the most healthy of
all long-standing Latino groups
in the United States.25

Other groups seeking political
asylum (e.g., Salvadorans and
Mariel Cubans), however, were
treated to noticeably less hospi-
tality. What is the impact (if
any) of these historical and po-
litical factors on acculturation
processes and health outcomes,
and how might they contribute
to different patterns among the
various Latino groups in the
United States? Although the
contextual features of accultura-
tion (e.g., circumstances before
immigrating, the political and
social climate of the United
States upon arrival) could deter-
mine the extent to which indi-
viduals and heterogeneous La-
tino groups adapt to new
environments, these contexts
are rarely studied.15,17,40

The complexity of these issues
led some researchers to suggest
that the use of acculturation
measures be suspended.12 We
disagree with this recommenda-
tion. Instead, we propose that
to understand Latino realities in
the United States, it is critical to
describe the context in which

ongoing cultural negotiations
take place and the dynamics
that reproduce and reconfig-
ure “Latino culture” according to
the equally complex American
settings in which immigrants and
other people of color find them-
selves. A consideration of the
intersection of large-scale social
forces and culture is critical to
stimulating the exploration of
much-neglected sociological con-
cepts, namely, class and power
dynamics in the public health lit-
erature on acculturation. Such
avenues of research could prove
to be fruitful in explaining the
complexities surrounding Latino
acculturation and health in the
United States.

CONCLUSIONS

Although we raise more ques-
tions than we answer, we pro-
pose that a theory-based public
health framework could con-
tribute much to understanding
the factors and mechanisms un-
derlying the association between
acculturation and health among
Latinos. If cultural norms, beliefs,
and values as well as broader
structural factors are considered,
a public health research agenda
on acculturation and health may
help to shift the paradigm from
linear models to models that are
multidimensional and more
comprehensive. A public health
framework offers the promising
opportunity to build new para-
digms that incorporate and ex-
pand on social and behavioral
science acculturation theories
and that cross disciplinary
boundaries. There is no doubt
that Latinos in the United States
face many hardships (e.g., pov-
erty, inadequate access to health
care, discrimination). However,
perhaps it is time to identify
and differentiate the cultural

resources and structural factors
that better explain how accultur-
ation affects health.

About the Authors
All authors are with the Department of
Sociomedical Sciences, Mailman School
of Public Health, Columbia University,
New York, NY.

Requests for reprints should be sent to
Ana Abraído-Lanza, PhD, Department of
Sociomedical Sciences, Mailman School of
Public Health, Columbia University, 722
West 168 St, 5th floor, New York, NY
10032 (e-mail: aabraido@columbia.edu).

This article was accepted September 23,
2005.

Contributors
A. Abraído-Lanza originated the article
and took the lead role in its writing. All
authors participated in the literature re-
view and in the writing and revising of
the article.

Acknowledgments
Support for preparing this manuscript
was provided by the Initiative for Minor-
ity Student Development at Columbia’s
Mailman School of Public Health, an ed-
ucation project funded by the National
Institute of General Medical Sciences
(R25GM62454), by the National Can-
cer Institute (R03CA107876), and by
the Columbia Center for the Health of
Urban Minorities, funded by the Na-
tional Center on Minority Health and
Health Disparities (P60MD00206).

We give special thanks to Antonio T.
Abraído.

References
1. US Census Bureau. The Hispanic Pop-
ulation: Census 2000 Brief. Available at:
http://www.census.gov/prod/2001pubs/
c2kbr01-3 . Accessed April 21,
2006.

2. Population Profile of the United
States: 1997. Washington, DC: US
Bureau of the Census; 1998. Current
Population Reports Series P23–194.

3. Clark L, Hofsess L. Acculturation.
In: Loue S, ed. Handbook of Immigrant
Health. New York, NY: Plenum Press;
1998:37–59.

4. LaFromboise T, Coleman HLK,
Gerton J. Psychological impact of bicul-
turalism: evidence and theory. Psychol
Bull. 1993;114:395–412.

5. Zhou M. Segmented assimilation:
issues, controversies, and recent re-
search on the new second generation.
Int Migration Rev. 1997;31:975–1008.

6. Rudmin FW. Critical history of the

acculturation psychology of assimilation,
separation, integration, and marginaliza-
tion. Rev Gen Psychol. 2003;7:3–37.

7. Teske RHC, Nelson BH. Accultura-
tion and assimilation: a clarification. Am
Ethnologist. 1974;1:351–367.

8. Park RE. Human migration and
the marginal man. Am J Sociol. 1928;
33:881–893.

9. Hazuda HP, Stern MP, Haffner SM.
Acculturation and assimilation among
Mexican Americans: scales and population-
based data. Soc Sci Q. 1988;69:
687–705.

10. Berry JW. Conceptual approaches
to acculturation. In: Chun KM, Balls
Organista P, Marín G, eds. Acculturation:
Advances in Theory, Measurement and
Applied Research. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association;
2003:17–37.

11. Padilla AM. Bicultural develop-
ment: a theoretical and empirical exami-
nation. In: Malgady RG, Rodriguez O,
eds. Theoretical and Conceptual Issues in
Hispanic Mental Health. Malabar, Fla:
Krieger; 1994;19–51.

12. Hunt L, Schneider S, Comer B.
Should “acculturation” be a variable in
health research? A critical review of re-
search on US Hispanics. Soc Sci Med.
2004;59:973–986.

13. Cuellar I, Arnold B, Maldonado R.
Acculturation rating scale for Mexican
Americans, II: a revision of the original
ARSMA scale. Hispanic J Behav Sci.
1995;17:275–304.

14. Szapocznik J, Kurtines WM,
Fernandez T. Bicultural involvement
and adjustment in Hispanic-American
youths. Int J Intercultural Relations.
1980;4:353–365.

15. Cabassa LJ. Measuring accultura-
tion: where we are and where we need
to go. Hispanic J Behav Sci. 2003;25:
127–146.

16. Zane N, Mak W. Major approaches
to the measurement of acculturation
among ethnic minority populations: a con-
tent analysis and an alternative empirical
strategy. In: Chun KM, Balls Organista P,
Marín G, eds. Acculturation: Advances in
Theory, Measurement and Applied Re-
search. Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association; 2003:39–60.

17. Lara M, Gamboa C, Kahramanian I,
Morales L, Bautista D. Acculturation
and Latino health in the United States:
a review of the literature and its socio-
political context. Ann Rev Public Health.
2005;26:367–397.

18. National Heart, Lung, and Blood
Institute Web page. Epidemiologic Re-
search in Hispanic Populations Oppor-
tunities, Barriers and Solutions. Work-
ing Group July 31–August 1, 2003,

American Journal of Public Health | August 2006, Vol 96, No. 81346 | Commentary | Peer Reviewed | Abraído-Lanza

 COMMENTARY 

Bethesda, Md. Available at: http://www.
nhlbi.nih.gov/meetings/workshops/
hispanic.htm. Accessed April 11, 2006.

19. González Gutiérrez C. Fostering
identities: Mexico’s relationship with its di-
aspora. J Am History. 1999;86:545–567.

20. Abraído-Lanza AF, Dohrenwend
BP, Ng-Mak DS, Turner JB. The Latino
mortality paradox: a test of the “salmon
bias” and healthy migrant hypotheses. Am
J Public Health. 1999;89:1543–1548.

21. Hummer RA, Rogers RG, Amir SH,
Forbes D, Frisbie WP. Adult mortality
differentials among Hispanic subgroups
and non-Hispanic whites. Soc Sci Q.
2000;81:459–476.

22. Lin CC, Rogot E, Johnson NJ,
Sorlie PD, Arias E. A further study of
life expectancy by socioeconomic factors
in the National Longitudinal Mortality
Study. Ethn Dis. 2003;13:240–247.

23. Singh GK, Siahpush M. Ethnic-
immigrant differentials in health behav-
iors, morbidity, and cause-specific mor-
tality in the United States: an analysis
of two national databases. Hum Biol.
2002;74:83–109.

24. Sorlie PD, Backlund E, Johnson NJ,
Rogot E. Mortality by Hispanic status in
the United States. JAMA. 1993;270:
2464–2468.

25. Vega WA, Amaro H. Latino out-
look: good health, uncertain prognosis.
Annu Rev Public Health. 1994;15:39–67.

26. Hazuda HP, Haffner SM, Stern
MP, Clayton WE. Effects of accultura-
tion and socioeconomic status on obe-
sity and diabetes in Mexican Ameri-
cans. Am J Epidemiol. 1988;128:
1289–1301.

27. Abraído-Lanza AF, Chao MT,
Flórez KR. Do healthy behaviors de-
cline with greater acculturation? Impli-
cations for the Latino mortality paradox.
Soc Sci Med. 2005;61:1243–1255.

28. Cantero PJ, Richardson JL,
Baezconde-Garbanati L, Marks G. The
association between acculturation and
health practices among middle-aged and
elderly Latinas. Ethn Dis. 1999;9:
166–180.

29. Gordon-Larsen P, Harris KM,
Ward DS, Popkin BM. Acculturation and
overweight-related behaviors among
Hispanic immigrants to the United
States: the National Longitudinal study
of adolescent health. Soc Sci Med.
2003;57:2023–2034.

30. Himmelgreen DA, Pérez-Escamilla R,
Martinez D, et al. The longer you stay,
the bigger you get: length of time and
language use in the US are associated
with obesity in Puerto Rican Women.
Am J Phys Anthropol. 2004;25:90–96.

31. Marks G, Garcia M, Solis JM.

Health risk behaviors of Hispanics in
the United States: findings from
HHANES, 1982–84. Am J Public
Health. 1990;80(suppl):20–26.

32. Marín G, Posner SF. The role of
gender and acculturation on determin-
ing the consumption of alcoholic bever-
ages among Mexican-Americans and
Central Americans in the United States.
Int J Addict. 1995;30:779–794.

33. Pérez-Stable EJ, Ramirez A,
Villareal R, et al. Cigarette smoking be-
havior among US Latino men and
women from different countries of origin.
Am J Public Health. 2001;91:1424–1430.

34. Crespo CJ, Smit E, Carter-Pokras O,
Andersen RE. Acculturation and leisure-
time physical inactivity in Mexican
American adults: results from NHANES
III, 1988–1994. Am J Public Health.
2001;91:1254–1257.

35. Myers H, Rodriguez N. Accultura-
tion and physical health in racial and
ethnic minorities. In: Chun KM, Balls
Organista P, Marín G, eds. Acculturation:
Advances in Theory, Measurement and
Applied Research. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association;
2003:163–185.

36. Johnsen L, Spring B, Pingitore R,
Sommerfeld BK, MacKirnan D. Smoking
as subculture? Influence on Hispanic
and non-Hispanic white women’s atti-
tudes toward smoking and obesity.
Health Psychol. 2002;21:279–287.

37. Haynes SG, Harvey C, Montes H,
Nickens H, Cohen BH. Patterns of ciga-
rette smoking among Hispanics in the
United States: results from HHANES
1982–1984. Am J Public Health. 1990;
80(suppl):47–53.

38. Marín G, Pérez-Stable EJ, Marín
BV. Cigarette smoking among San Fran-
cisco Hispanics: the role of acculturation
and gender. Am J Public Health. 1989;
79:196–198.

39. Marín G, Gamba RJ. Acculturation
and changes in cultural values. In:
Chun KM, Balls Organista P, Marín G,
eds. Acculturation: Advances in Theory,
Measurement and Applied Research.
Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
cal Association; 2003:83–93.

40. Portes A, Zhou M. The new sec-
ond generation: segmented assimilation
and its variants. Ann Am Acad Political
Soc Sci. 1993;530:74–96.

41. Charles CZ. The dynamics of racial
residential segregation. Annu Rev Sociol.
2003;29:167–207.

42. Norris AE, Ford K, Bova CA. Psy-
chometrics of a brief acculturation scale
for Hispanics in a probability sample of
urban Hispanic adolescents and young
adults. Hispanic J Behav Sci. 1996;18:
29–38.

43. Markides KS, Ray LA, Stroup-
Benham CA, Trevizo F. Acculturation
and alcohol consumption in the Mexi-
can American population of the South-
western United States: findings from
HHANES 1982–84. Am J Public
Health. 1990;80(suppl):42–46.

44. Epstein JA, Botvin GJ, Diaz T.
Linguistic acculturation and gender
effects on smoking among Hispanic
youth. Prev Med 1998;27:583–589.

45. Kaplan CP, Napoles-Springer A,
Stewart SL, Perez-Stable EJ. Smoking
acquisition among adolescents and
young Latinas: the role of socioenviron-
mental and personal factors. Addict
Behav. 2001;26:531–550.

46. Guilamo-Ramos V, Jaccard J,
Johansson M, Tunisi R. Binge drinking
among Latino youth: role of acculturation-
related variables. Psychol Addict Behav.
2004;18:135–142.

47. Morgan-Lopez AA, Gonzalez
Castro F, Chassin L, MacKinnon DP. A
mediated moderation model of cigarette
use among Mexican American youth.
Addict Behav. 2003;28:583–589.

48. Bettes BA, Dusenbury L, Kerner J,
James-Ortiz S, Botvin GJ. Ethnicity and
psychosocial factors in alcohol and to-
bacco use in adolescence. Child Dev.
1990;61:557–565.

49. Unger JB, Cruz TB, Rohrbach LA,
et al. English language use as a risk
factor for smoking initiation among
Hispanic and Asian American adoles-
cents: evidence for mediation by tobacco-
related beliefs and social norms. Health
Psychol. 2000;19:403–410.

50. Link B, Phelan J. Social conditions as
fundamental causes of disease. J Health
Soc Behav 1995;35 (extra issue):80–94.

51. Farmer P. Infections and Inequali-
ties. Berkeley, Calif: University of Cali-
fornia Press; 1999.

52. US Migration and Refugee Assis-
tance Act of 1962. Available at: http://
www.lib.umich.edu/govdocs/jfkeo/eo/
11077.htm. Accessed April 21, 2006.

The Specifici

t

y Principle in Acculturation Science

Marc H. Bornstein
Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, National Institutes of Health, Public Health Service

  • Abstract
  • The Specificity Principle in Acculturation Science asserts that specific setting conditions of

    specific people at specific times moderate specific domains in acculturation by specific processes.

    Our understanding of acculturation depends critically on what is studied where, in whom, how,

    and when. This article defines, explains, and illustrates the Specificity Principle in Acculturation

    Science. Research hypotheses about acculturation can be more adequately tested, inconsistencies

    and discrepancies in the acculturation literature can be satisfactorily resolved, acculturation

    interventions can be tailored to be more successful, and acculturation policies can be brought to

    new levels of effectiveness if the specificity principle that governs acculturation science is more

    widely recognized.

  • Migration and Acculturation
  • The world is in motion. Throughout human history, people have been on the move, and

    migration and acculturation have been facts of the human condition ever since peoples of the

    African savannah radiated outward in their treks to new lands (Cann, Stoneking, & Wilson,

    1987; Jin et al., 1999; Stringer, 1988). Polynesian expansion, Phoenician trade, Jewish

    diaspora, Persian realm, Alexandrian conquest, Roman hegemony, Hun invasion, Umayyad

    caliphate, Viking settlement, Ifat sultanate, Hanseatic league, and Columbian exchange

    illuminate the history of successive intercultural contacts, just as, beginning 23 centuries BC

    with Sargon’s expansion of Akkad across the Fertile Crescent, Egyptian, Kushite, Aksumite,

    Ottoman, Inca, Mongol, Songhai, Moghul, Napoleonic, and British empires—all

    intercultural permeations—followed one another inexorably. Indeed, human beings have not

    ceased migrating even though virtually all habitable places on earth have long since been

    settled.

    It comes as little surprise, then, that migration and acculturation are global concerns in our

    own time. Everywhere one looks, the numbers stun. What in 1990 was 154 million and in

    2000 175 million, the United Nations (U.N.) Population Division reported that as of 2015

    nearly 245 million people live outside the country of their birth or citizenship. That

    translates into roughly 1 in every 30 people on the face of the globe (United Nations, 2016).

    Address correspondence to: Dr. Marc H. Bornstein, Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child
    Health and Human Development, Suite 8030, 6705 Rockledge Drive, Bethesda MD 20892-7971, USA, TEL: 301-496-6832, FAX:
    301-496-2766, Marc_H_Bornstein@nih.gov.

    HHS Public Access
    Author manuscript

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.

    Published in final edited form as:
    Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017 January ; 12(1): 3–45. doi:10.1177/1745691616655997.

    A

    u
    th

    o
    r M

    a
    n
    u
    scrip

    t
    A

    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    The top three countries in the world with foreign born as percentages of their populations are

    Luxembourg with 42%, Israel with 32%, and Switzerland with 28%. In 2010, about 6.5% of

    the total population of European countries had foreign nationalities, and 9.4% were born

    abroad (Vasileva, 2011). As has been observed, if subsequent generations are added to first-

    generation migrants the numbers become even bigger. Western European countries have

    about 20% immigrants (up to the third generation), and Australia has more than twice that

    number.

    The quantity of people forced to flee their homes – within their countries as well as

    internationally – far surpassed 60 million in 2015. The U.N. High Commissioner for

    Refugees estimated that at one point in 2015 an average of ~5,000 migrants a day travelled

    just from Turkey to Greece. In the “biblical march” of refugees from the Middle East to the

    European Union (E.U.), about 1.1 million people sought protection in Germany in 2015, and

    Austria reported that asylum applications increased 231% from 2014 to 2015; by the close

    of 2015, more than 170,000 migrants had entered Slovenia, equivalent to approximately 8%

    of the country’s extant population, and more than 160,000 Syrians, Iraqis, Afghans and

    others applied for asylum in Sweden in 2015.

    Most contemporary societies are far from culturally homogenous, but entertain (usually

    roiling) sociopolitical conditions associated with such vibrant emigration and/or

    immigration. Migration is consequently one of the defining issues of the 21st C and currently

    forms an essential feature of the social and economic life of virtually every contemporary

    nation state.

    The United States is “a nation of immigrants” (Kennedy, 1964). The largest numerical

    foreign-born population in the world resides in the United States, which was home to 47

    million foreign-residents in 2015, or 19% of the population (United Nations, 2016). In the

    18th C, when the colonies proclaimed their independence from Great Britain, the colonists

    were all immigrants clinging to a narrow strip of land on the North American continent’s
    eastern seaboard. Assembled in Philadelphia, the colonists’ representatives charged Thomas

    Jefferson to write out their Declaration of Independence. That celebrated document is

    surprisingly short, divided into three main parts. The first is a crisp statement of natural

    rights. In the second part, Jefferson and his compatriots enumerated 18 grievances the

    colonists held against the King of England. In Number 7, the colonists, being migrants and

    wanting to promote immigration and increase their numbers, remonstrated King George for

    having “endeavored to prevent the population of these States; … obstructing the Laws of

    Naturalization of Foreigners; [and] refusing to pass others to encourage their migrations

    hither ….”. In the third part, the Founding Fathers explicitly “reminded” their British

    cousins “of the circumstances of [their] emigration and settlement… .”

    So, the United States is, and has always been, a country of acculturating peoples, even if the

    countries of origin of its naturalizing citizens have constantly shifted. Since its founding, the

    United States has sustained two distinct waves of authorized immigration (discounting

    unauthorized immigration in the form of the slave trade that dominated the 18th to mid-19th

    Cs). The first from Europe reached a crescendo at the turn of the 20th C. Today’s second

    wave of immigrants emanates from Latin America and Asia. The total population of Latin

    Bornstein Page 2

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.

    A
    u
    th

    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Americans reached 55.4 million in 2014 or 17.4% of the total U.S. population (Krogstad &

    Lopez, 2015), making people of Latin origin the nation’s largest ethnic minority. Between

    July 1, 2011, and July 1, 2012, 1.1 million Latin Americans constituted close to half of the

    approximately 2.3 million people added to the U.S. population during the same period (U.S.

    Census Bureau, 2012a). The projected 2060 Latin American population of the United States

    is 128.8 million, or 31% of the U.S. population (Krogstad & Lopez, 2015; Passel, Cohn, &

    Gonzalez-Barrera, 2012; U.S. Census Bureau, 2012b).

    A record total of 20.25 million Asian Americans (U. S. Census Bureau, 2014) expanded

    more than 45% between 2000 and 2012 (Hoeffel, Rastogi, Kim, & Shahid, 2012) to

    constitute more than 5% of the population of the United States. Natural population growth

    accounted for a small proportion of this increase (Humes, Jones, Ramirez, 2011); rather,

    Asian American population growth was fueled by immigration and is projected to triple by

    2050, when approximately 50% of Asian Americans will be foreign born (Passel & Cohn,

    2008; U.S. Census Bureau, 2010). Children of immigrants are currently the fastest growing

    population of children in the United States, accounting for nearly 1 in 4 of all children living

    in America (Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2011).

    Migrants bring one culture into contact with another culture. Culture defines the ways in

    which a collection of people processes and makes sense of their experiences, and so

    generally speaking culture refers to shared meanings, understandings, or referents and

    permeates a wide array of biological, psychological, and social processes (Bornstein, 2010;

    Shore, 2002; Triandis, 1995). Acculturation traditionally refers to changes that take place in

    all those domains as a result of contacts between culturally dissimilar peoples (Gibson,

    2001; Hunt, Schneider & Comer, 2004; Redfield, Linton, & Herskovits, 1936).

    Acculturation transpires at both individual and societal levels. At the societal level,

    acculturation involves changes in social structures, service institutions, and cultural

    practices. At the individual level, acculturation involves changes in a person’s customs,

    habits, activities, language, and values. Immigrants face multiple challenges in acculturating

    within a dominant or existing society, retaining or surrendering beliefs and behaviors from

    their culture of origin while eschewing or adopting those from their culture of destination.

    Thus, acculturation is a complex phenomenon comprising multiple processes and is rightly

    thought of as an instance of the most thoroughgoing sort of individual disorganization and

    reorganization. For example, immigrants may convert overnight from membership in the

    majority group in their culture of origin to membership in a minority group in their culture

    of destination (Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga, & Szapocznik, 2010).

  • What this Article Aims to Do, its Context, and Limitations
  • The main aim of this article is to deconstruct the topical and higher-order construct of

    acculturation through the introduction, explication, and illustration of five terms that

    compose a Specificity Principle in Acculturation Science. The five terms consist of specific

    setting conditions, specific peoples, specific times, specific processes, and specific domains

    of acculturation. Their recognition and regard are meant to transform acculturation science

    in ways that may be consequential theoretically, heuristic empirically, and useful practically.

    The notion of acculturation dates back at least to Plato, and modern psychological thinking

    Bornstein Page 3

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    about acculturation began with G. Stanley Hall (1904) and Thomas and Znaniecki

    (1918/1958), even if the construct entered the scientific literature a bit earlier through

    linguistics (Powell, 1880, 1900) and sociology (McGee, 1898). Since Taft (1953, 1957) and

    Born (1970), acculturation has been widely theorized to consist, in a nutshell, of a small

    number of categories or types: assimilation, separation, integration, and marginalization. The

    Specificity Principle offers a more nuanced and realistic logical next step in the

    advancement of acculturation science. The Specificity Principle stands in contrast to that

    typological construction, asserting a perspective that is sensitive to the many variations

    found among contemporary migrants and their circumstances and so promises greater

    fidelity to acculturation study and greater purchase on explanatory power than any “one size

    fits all” belief. The overarching theory that grounds the Specificity Principle, and thus the

    central argument of specificity, appeals to a moderator view that focuses on how key factors

    influence the size and direction of acculturation: As the burgeoning literature amply reveals,

    moderator effects are pervasive in acculturation, and here I illustrate them with numerous

    examples.

    It is important to underscore at the outset that the following treatment is not exhaustive in

    delineating terms of specificity in acculturation, so this article does not pretend to be the

    final word on which variables to include or exclude in specificity. It is only a start. Nor are

    the five terms of the Specificity Principle as enumerated and elaborated here rigidly

    mutually exclusive, but as in many taxonomies the terms sometimes interact and marginally

    infringe on one another, and so their interactions are also discussed. Nonetheless, conceptual

    distinctions among the main terms of the Specificity Principle will be intuitive. Further, this

    article does not comprehensively review all studies in acculturation; already that flourishing

    literature does not submit to easy summary. Rather, the goal here is to demonstrate the value

    of integrating information from diverse perspectives in fundamentally re-thinking the next

    developments in acculturation theory, science, and research. To substantiate its practical

    worth, the article concludes with a brief exploration of prominent implications of this new

    specificity view in acculturation science for empiricism and praxis.

    In lifespan human development some characteristics and experiences have broad

    implications. Where one is born, how much education one accrues, one’s gender, as

    examples, doubtless have pervasive consequences over the life course. Even so, as life

    proceeds, advantages and disadvantages cumulate to heterogeneity—so much so that

    variability and so specificity are inevitable. That is, the lifespan development of specific

    characteristics in specific individuals is affected by specific experiences in specific ways at

    specific times. This is the Specificity Principle. To complement universals, understanding

    often depends on what is studied, in whom, how, and when. The Specificity Principle

    therefore differs from many common assumptions, for example that overall stimulation

    influences overall development or better diet ensures better health. Some such generalities

    may be valid. However, it is not the case necessarily that a monolithic global shared

    experience affects performance in all areas of life, is adequate to adaptive functioning, or

    compensates for selective deficiencies. Familial love, financial well-being, or a stimulating

    environment do not guarantee, or even speak to, lifespan development of specific

    characteristics, such as a healthy diet, empathic personality, verbal competence, sports

    prowess, ethical action, or myriad others. Rather, contemporary science indicates that

    Bornstein Page 4

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    specificities are often at play. The Specificity Principle advances a theory that is

    particularistic in nature, such that development depends on several separate identifiable

    factors, including the experience involved, who experiences and who generates the

    experience, when in life the experience occurs, how the experience occurs, and the domain

    of development affected by experience.

    Relatedly, with respect to philosophy of science, specificity is not contending absolute
    reductionism. As noted, the life course embraces some generalizations. As will become

    evident, acculturation naturally does so as well. It is possible to hold to specificity and to

    acknowledge some generalizations (marked along the way and in the conclusions).

  • Specificity in Acculturation
  • A widespread assumption in contemporary acculturation study is that the processes and

    pathways of acculturation are categorical and essentially universal. In précis, individuals

    who relinquish their culture-of-origin identity and adopt the values, norms, and traditions of

    their culture of destination assimilate into that new culture. Individuals who cling to their

    culture of origin and eschew interaction and identification with members of their culture of

    destination separate themselves. Individuals who maintain the integrity of their original

    culture while they also participate in the mainstream culture of destination integrate

    themselves. Individuals who fail to maintain their original culture and avoid relations with

    the majority culture are said to be marginalized (Berry, 1997). However, the emerging

    literature in acculturation science reveals that acculturation is actually a much more subtle

    and differentiated process moderated by multiple factors, disabusing us of typological

    notions or broad generalities.

    The Specificity Principle in Acculturation Science asserts that specific setting

    conditions of specific peoples at specific times moderate specific domains of

    acculturation via specific processes.

    In the context of the prevailing typological approach to acculturation, this principle is a bit

    counterintuitive and complicates our theoretical, empirical, and practical lives — but is

    nonetheless emerging as true. We need to parse this principle into its several “specifics” of

    setting, person, time, process, and domain to best understand the principle, how it works,

    and how to take advantage of it.

  • Figure 1
  • shows the plan. Here, I deconstruct acculturation

    and define, examine, and illustrate each term of the specificity principle as well as their

    interactions and afterward briefly discuss the principle’s manifold implications for science

    and policy, asserting a next step in the intellectual progression of understanding

    acculturation.

  • Specificity Principle: Setting Condition
  • Setting conditions of acculturation include reasons for migrating, place, experience, and

    status. That is, migration arises for myriad causes, such as natural or predictable responses

    with respect to resources, occupation, family reunification, demographic growth, climate

    change, financial insecurity, exploitation of human rights, political or religious persecution,

    and war. Emigration can occur from any country, and immigration to any country, and so all

    Bornstein Page 5

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    possible combinations and permutations of place are possible. Other relevant setting

    conditions at the societal and individual levels involve immersion experience, legal status,

    and so forth. Each setting condition moderates acculturation meaningfully.

    Setting Condition: Reason to Migrate

    Migration may be voluntary or involuntary, temporary or permanent. Some migrants
    leave their homelands by choice, in search of family reunion and marriage, economic

    opportunity and employment, the promise of a stable and prosperous future for themselves

    and their children, or a more compatible sociopolitical climate as in expatriation (Perreira,

    Chapman, & Stein, 2006). A frequent pathway to acculturation, accounting for about two-

    thirds of permanent immigration to the United States for example, is through desired family

    reunification as promoted by the Immigration Act of 1965 (and analogously Canada’s

    Immigration Act of 1952). Economic theory posits that migration is also motivated by the

    desire to maximize economic well-being (Borjas, 1987). As voluntary migrants to the United

    States, Jamaican families profess to emigrate for better economic and educational

    opportunities rather than to escape oppression or human rights violations (Ferguson,

    Bornstein, & Pottinger, 2012). Temporary sojourners (foreign students, religious

    proselytizers, occupying soldiers, guest workers) usually relocate on a time-limited basis

    with intentions to return to their country of origin (Berry, 2006; Schwartz et al., 2010).

    By contrast with these classes of voluntary migrants, involuntary migrants may be brought

    permanently into contact with a new culture for political reasons or through slavery, escape,

    abuse, persecution, defection, financial extortion, sexual exploitation, conquest, or

    colonization (Gieling, Thijs, & Verkuyten, 2011; Ogbu, 1991). During the reign of the

    generals in South American countries such as Argentina and Chile, droves of the middle

    class sought refuge in their ancestors’ home countries in Western Europe. Refugees

    displaced by war or natural disasters are currently estimated at 15.1 million persons

    worldwide (UN HCR, 2015). Asylum seekers from war zones and terror regimes pursue

    sanctuary in a new country because of fear of persecution or violence. Since 2013, Europe

    has witnessed a sharp increase in the number of such asylees. In Germany, more than 35,000

    people applied for political asylum in the month of June 2015 alone (Bundesamt für

    Migration und Flüchtlinge, 2015). The United States has witnessed a surge of families and

    unaccompanied minors since 2014 that originated in Central America on account of drug

    trafficking, gang violence, and extreme droughts across the Golden Triangle of El Salvador,

    Honduras, and Guatemala. North Korea is unwittingly fostering clandestine “chain

    migrations” among family members who escape hunger and repression to the South and then

    mount funds to bribe brokers and secret relatives out of the North in seriatim. These different

    motives surrounding migration – voluntary versus involuntary, temporary versus permanent

    – and the vulnerabilities and strengths that each introduces condition different pathways of

    acculturation.

    Impetuses and experiences of immigrants who choose their culture of destination versus

    ones who do not differ qualitatively and in ways that shape the drive or opportunity of each

    to adapt to their new culture (Akhtar, 1999). For example, historical reasons for migration

    have been invoked to explain why contemporary African American mothers parent in one

    Bornstein Page 6

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    way and Mexican American mothers parent in a different way: Individuals with family

    histories of voluntary immigration for economic reasons display less resistance to the

    dominant culture than individuals with family histories of forced migration through

    enslavement (Ispa et al., 2013; Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2010). Holroyd, Molassiotis, and

    Taylor-Pilliae (2001) compared three types of biculturals, keeping overarching cultural

    context constant: immigrants who permanently relocated from one cultural context to

    another (Mainland Chinese immigrants to Hong Kong), sojourners who worked in the

    foreign culture for limited periods of time (Filipina domestic helpers in Hong Kong), and

    majority individuals who came into contact with a second cultural group and language in

    their home culture (college students in Hong Kong and in Mainland China). Each group

    followed a distinct path of acculturative change.

    Setting Condition: Place

    Place, a second setting condition, has several construals, each of which moderates

    acculturation. One reading of place is the culture of origin. Unlike their Western European
    immigrant peers to Israel, immigrants from the former Soviet Union (FSU) to Israel were

    not able to withdraw their savings before emigration; these contrasting financial constraints

    had contrasting consequences for the subsequent economic independence and security of

    each group as immigrants (Mirsky & Barasch, 1993). Although 95% of authorized

    immigration to the United States from Mexico occurs for reasons of family unification,

    percentages appealing to that rationale vary dramatically among the top nations sending

    emigrants to the United States: Philippines (77%), Vietnam (68%), Taiwan (59%), and India

    (43%). Acculturation among Latin American immigrants to the United States similarly

    depends on their country of origin (Mendoza, Javier, & Burgos, 2007): Dominican American

    and Mexican American infants’ vocabulary acquisition, even though both are Spanish

    speaking and living New York City, differ (Song, Tamis-LeMonda, Yoshikawa, Kahana-

    Kalman, & Wu, 2012). Patterns of residence and geographic mobility among Puerto Ricans

    in the United States differ from those of other Latino groups, such as Mexicans and Cubans

    (Denton & Massey, 1989; Massey & Bitterman, 1985; South, Crowder, & Chavez, 2005);

    Puerto Ricans tend to be segregated from non-Latino Whites at higher rates, and Puerto

    Ricans fare more poorly on a variety of health outcomes (diabetes during pregnancy, asthma

    prevalence among children) compared with other Latino groups (Hajat, Lucas, & Kingston,

    2000; Kieffer, Martin, & Herman, 1999; Lara, Akinbami, Flores, & Morgenstern, 2006;

    Zsembik & Fennell, 2005). Likewise, Asian Americans describe heterogeneous groups of

    peoples in the United States who trace their ancestry to many Asian countries (Barringer,

    Gardner, & Levin, 1993; Lowe, 1991).

    Early adolescents emigrating from China, Central America, the Dominican Republic, Haiti,

    and Mexico and migrating to the United States all separated from one or both parents for

    extended periods and were all likely to report depressive symptoms (Suárez-Orozco &

    Suárez-Orozco, 2001). However, these groups differed in how separations were managed.

    Chinese families frequently migrated as a unit, whereas Haitians nearly always incurred a

    family disruption. Chinese children reported the fewest, and Haitian children reported the

    most, depressive symptoms. A meta-analysis of 83 studies revealed an overall positive

    association between biculturalism and adjustment (Nguyen & Benet-Martínez, 2013).

    Bornstein Page 7

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    However, associations varied across immigrant groups and across countries of origin: Latin,

    Asian, and European immigrants had positive biculturalism-adjustment associations,

    whereas African immigrants had negligible or negative associations. A study from Germany

    investigated whether ethnic differences in parenting reflect different investment strategies in

    the future welfare of offspring as indicated by parents’ expectations about the

    instrumentality (perceived costs and benefits) of schooling. Comparing German mother-

    child dyads, Vietnamese dyads, and Turkish dyads, Nauck and Lotter (2015) found

    differences in parenting styles, reflecting more active control in collectivist cultures

    (Vietnam) and a strong emphasis on children’s individual needs in individualist cultures

    (Germany) with an intermediate position of Turkey.

    Calling on data from the New Immigrant Survey, Bradley, Pennar, and Glick (2014)

    described the home environments of children (ages birth to 3 years) whose parents legally

    immigrated to the United States. Results revealed stark variation in 32 indicators of home

    conditions by both country (Mexico, El Salvador, India, Philippines) and region (East Asia,

    Europe, Caribbean, Africa). Similarly, when Xu, Farver, and Krieg (2017) compared the

    home environments of Chinese Americans, Filipino Americans, and Vietnamese Americans,

    they uncovered group differences in parents’ English reading skills and involvement in

    literacy activities in English and in their native language as well as in the numbers of

    children’s picture books in the home. Hernandez and Napierala (2012) reported that more

    than 60% of children whose parents immigrated from Western Europe, India, and Africa to

    the United States attended prekindergarten, whereas fewer than 50% of parents who

    migrated from other regions enrolled their children in pre-kindergarten. In Europe, the Dutch

    Generation R Study, a population-based birth cohort study from Rotterdam, revealed large

    differences in family structure across various ethnic minority groups. Only 5% of Dutch

    toddlers and their peers from Moroccan- or Turkish-origin families were reared by a single

    parent, but 40% of the children from Antillean, Cape Verdian, and Surinamese Creole

    children lived in single-parent families (Flink et al., 2012). Generally, immigrants from non-

    Western countries tend to have stronger feelings of filial obligation than immigrants from

    Western countries (e.g., Dykstra & Fokkema, 2012; Liefbroer & Mulder, 2006). Emigrants

    from some countries tend to quickly adopt their new culture, whereas emigrants from other

    countries tend to maintain their ancestral culture (van de Vijver, 2017).

    A second reading of place is the culture of destination. Here, the compositions of local and
    larger cultures are meaningful moderators of acculturation. Many immigrants reside among

    smaller or larger groups of co-nationals after migration. For example, contemporary

    Jamaican immigrants have established ethnic enclaves around the United States, including

    New York City and South Florida (Foner, 2006). Locally, coethnic communities may, on the

    one hand, assist immigrant acculturation through increased access to ethnic goods, social

    capital, informational resources, community services, and employment opportunities, that is

    the availability of instrumental or financial support, even protecting newcomers from

    experiences of discrimination. These “enclave communities” can provide important

    resources for immigrants and may have their own (ethnic) shops, places of worship, mass

    media, and festivities related to the ethnic culture and education in the ethnic language (van

    de Vijver, 2017). A network of coethnics can serve as a satisfying well spring of shared

    Bornstein Page 8

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    experiences that positively affects individual immigrant adjustment (Kosic, Kruglanski,

    Pierro, & Manneti, 2008). On the other hand, being embedded in a heritage-culture

    community of coethnics may obstruct immigrants’ exposure to or adopting facets of the

    culture of destination (Rodriguez, Myers, Mira, Flores, & Garcia-Henandez, 2002;

    Schwartz, Pantin, Sullivan, Prado, Szapocznik, 2006a), and ethnic enclaves may harbor

    concentrated poverty thereby circumscribing immigrants to resource-poor settings (Chiswick

    & Miller, 2005; Fernandez-Kelly & Schauffler, 1996; Galster, Metzger, & Waite, 1999;

    Osypuk, Galea, McArdle, & Acevedo-Garcia, 2009; Portes & Landolt, 1996). For example,

    island-born Puerto Ricans living in ethnically dense, low-SES neighborhoods report worse

    health than comparable island-born Puerto Ricans living in non-ethnic neighborhoods (Roy,

    Hughes, & Yoshikawa,

    2013).

    In communities where culture-of-origin values are heavily endorsed, culture-of-origin

    practices might endure despite passing years spent in the culture of destination, or they may

    even continue between foreign-born immigrants and those born in the culture of destination.

    In enclaves, immigrants may “remain isolated [from the culture of destination] into the third

    or even later generations” (Phinney & Flores, 2002, p. 322). As a result, although some

    culture-of-destination influences (e.g., media) encourage exposure to and adoption of

    culture-of-destination practices (Stilling, 1997), enclave living may counter the effects of the

    culture of destination on loss of culture-of-origin values and practices. In the end, the nature

    of the ethnic enclave facilitates or inhibits acculturation of successive generations of

    immigrants, and acculturation studies conducted in ethnic enclaves where the culture of

    origin prevails may yield different findings from studies conducted with otherwise

    comparable immigrants in immigrant settings where the culture of destination dominates

    (Kelly & Schauffler, 1996; Portes & Bach, 1985; Portes & Stepick, 1993; Wilson & Portes,

    1980). Acculturation is moderated by ethnic enclaves of different sorts. It also varies with

    the general diversity and orientation of the larger culture of destination.

    Some cultures of destination are pluralistic and accepting, others less so. Key influences on

    immigrant adjustment and adaptation are the tenor, structure, and resources of the

    community of reception (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). Receiving cultures constrain and direct

    migrants’ acculturation options. For example, members of receiving cultures may hold

    different attitudes toward migrants from different ethnic groups, migrants from different

    socioeconomic strata, and migrants who emigrate for different reasons. Even shared beliefs

    foster or discourage cooperation between peoples depending on how they are valued

    (Purzycki et al., 2016). A nation-wide public opinion poll conducted in the United States in

    the early 2000s indicated that native-born Americans viewed European and Canadian

    migrants more favorably and Latin American migrants less favorably (Cornelius, 2002).

    Further to this point, some majority groups or members might prefer immigrants who adopt

    culture-of-destination beliefs and behaviors, whereas others favor immigrants who remain

    separate (Maisonneuve & Teste, 2007; Nigbur et al., 2008; Van Acker & Vanbeselaere,

    2012; Van Oudenhoven, Prins, & Buunk, 1998). The countries with the highest rates of

    immigration are wealthy ones with relatively open nationality or migration laws, including

    the United States, Canada, United Kingdom, Australia, and the Persian Gulf States.

    However, countries vary widely in their official policies toward immigrants (Helbling, 2013;

    Huddleston, Niessen, Chaoimh, & White, 2011). Those with immigration laws that are more

    Bornstein Page 9

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    liberal and grant more rights to immigrants are known to foster feelings of inclusiveness,

    promote learning of the host country language and culture, and support well-being in

    immigrants (van de Vijver, 2017). (These considerations of the culture of destination have

    social policy implications discussed below.)

    Two-dimensional typological acculturation models tend to conceptualize migrant-sending

    and -receiving societies (such as Australia, Canada, or the United States) as homogeneous

    entities as opposed to their multicultural realities, and so inadequately capture acculturation

    experiences. England is today a destination country for migrants from Eastern Europe,

    Africa, and Asia but also a source country for emigration to other nations. By contrast, place

    in specificity acknowledges that many receiving countries are themselves multicultural as

    are many sending countries, and multicultural receiving and sending settings render

    acculturation multiplicatively multi-dimensional. Work in identity formation in Caribbean

    and Russian immigrants to the United States supports this conclusion. For example, Portes

    and Zhou’s (1993) research with Black and non-Black Caribbean immigrant youth in

    Florida demonstrated that Black immigrants were vulnerable to unique acculturation

    pressures because of their appearance as African Americans and shared African heritage;

    they were likely to live near inner-city African American communities due to limited

    resources typical of newcomers (Iceland & Scopilliti, 2008); and they experienced similar

    treatment as African Americans including discrimination (Foner, 2006). Many contemporary

    Jamaican immigrants to the United States follow a tridimensional model of identification

    with their heritage (Jamaican) culture, with the dominant destination (European American)

    culture, and with the relevant destination (African American) subculture (Ferguson et al.,

    2012). Birman, Persky, and Chan (2010) presented the parallel case of ethnically bicultural

    Jewish Russians emigrating to the United States. Jewish Russian immigrant youth were

    much less likely than non-Jewish Russian youth to self-identify as “Russian” and much

    more likely to self-identify as bicultural “Russian Jewish” or tricultural “Russian Jewish

    American”. Kim and Wong (2002) found that Chinese parents who immigrated to the United

    States professed more “Westernized” beliefs pertaining to parental control than did Chinese

    parents who immigrated to Taiwan (Krogstad & Lopez, 2015).

    Language is a bell-weather of acculturation to the culture of destination. Subtractive forms

    of bilingual education are normally designed to assimilate students to the language of the

    majority culture and do not support the minority language. By contrast, additive bilingual

    education programs are designed to foster bilingualism. Additive programs are also

    associated with benefits to academic performance, social development, and possibly

    cognitive benefits for both minority- and majority-language speakers (Esposito, Sirkin, &

    Bauer, 2017). Different countries emphasize additive versus subtractive approaches to

    bilingualism, and additive versus subtractive biculturalism may moderate acculturation.

    More generally, within the European Union languages and cultures of immigrant groups are

    often poorly protected relative to those of majorities or indigenous (national) minorities.

    A third reading of place is the relation between cultures of origin and destination;
    contacts may be objective in terms of language and law or subjective in terms of perceptions

    of similarity or difference (Ward & Kennedy, 1993). Bartlett (1923/1970) hypothesized that

    resemblance between heritage culture and receiving culture helps to determine the course of

    Bornstein Page 10

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    acculturation; Searle and Ward (1990) opined that the “fit” between personal characteristics

    and norms of the culture of destination predicts immigrants’ acculturation; and Rudmin

    (2003) predicted that the degree of acculturation needed to bridge cultural differences would

    be high to the extent that background and foreground cultures are experienced as different.

    Thus, Asian American immigrants, children and adults, who live in Hawai’i, where a

    traditional Asian value system prevails, could be expected to experience less conflict

    between cultures of home and school or work than Asian American immigrants living in

    Arkansas. Returning to the central role of language in acculturation, when ethnicity is held

    constant migrants hailing from English-speaking countries, or who are otherwise proficient

    in English, likely encounter less stress and resistance in the United States than non-English-

    speaking migrants. Among all Caribbean immigrants to the United States, other things being

    equal, English-speaking Jamaicans experience less discrimination and acculturative stress

    than French-speaking Haitians. Cultural distance (just as cultural conflict) moderates

    acculturation (Benet-Martínez & Haraitos, 2005).

    Immigrants are often more traditional peoples with historical ties to their religions and

    religious communities (e.g., Latinos to the Catholic Church, Turks to Islam) that do not

    always mesh well in the country of destination (King & Boyatzis, 2015). Much of our

    knowledge of processes of acculturation is based on studies of immigration from the “old

    world” to the “new world”. As described by Portes and Rumbaut (2001), over the course of

    two or three generations immigrants in the United States or Canada are likely to become

    either part of the majority society or part of a minority group but no longer consider

    themselves to be immigrants. Inter-European migration appears to follow a different pattern.

    Rather than becoming an integral part of the host society and striving to acquire citizenship,

    inter-European immigrants often prefer to become integral to both their country of

    destination and their country of origin (Leyendecker, 2011). Inexpensive flights across

    Europe, 4–6 week vacations per year, as well as modern communications facilitate staying

    connected with the country and the culture of origin, including its political and societal

    institutions.

    One study series that evaluated contrasts in cultural “fit” undertook two parallel accultural

    comparisons (Bornstein & Cote, 2001; Cote & Bornstein, 2001). In the first, mothers in

    Japan, Japanese immigrant mothers living in the United States, and European American

    mothers in the United States were compared on a set of variables. In the second, mothers in

    South America, South American immigrant mothers in the United States, and European

    American mothers in the United States were compared on the same variables. Theoretically,

    it is interesting to contrast Japanese American and South American immigrants to the United

    States because the two immigrant groups come from geographically different but similarly

    collectivist parts of the globe and are arriving on the same highly individualist U.S. shores.

    More generally, contrasting two groups from different cultures of origin allows assessing the

    generality versus specificity of acculturation by fit. South Americans and European

    Americans share cultural histories, religious traditions, and the like, more than do Japanese

    and European Americans. One important domain where the dynamics of acculturation play

    out is in everyday parent-child relationships. In this study series, different patterns in the two

    cultural contrasts emerged with respect, for example, to self-perceptions of competence in

    parenting: Japanese and European Americans did not self-evaluate the same, and Japanese

    Bornstein Page 11

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    immigrants fell between the two; South Americans and European Americans also did not

    self-evaluate the same, but South American immigrants responded like European Americans.

    A study from Italy focusing on families with infants compared Italian families to first-

    generation immigrants from Romania as well as Romanian families in Romania

    (Moscardino, Bertelli, & Altoè, 2011). Investigating mother-infant interaction and

    childrearing patterns, the authors found that immigrant mothers resemble more closely

    Italian mothers in the importance attributed to stimulating children’s cognitive competence,

    autonomy, and self-fulfillment, whereas Romanian mothers emphasized values and

    behaviors related to interdependence/sociocentrism.

    Even the perception that cultures of origin and destination differ may capture unique (e.g.,

    affective vs. cognitive) aspects of the accultural experience. Perceived cultural similarity

    increases liking, for example (Byrne, 1971), and, as years of exposure to U.S. culture

    increase, perceptions of cultural distance decrease (Benet-Martinez & Haritatos, 2005).

    Setting Conditions: Experience and Status

    Still other setting conditions moderate acculturation; they include, notably, variations in

    acculturation immersion experience and immigrant legal status. Experience in, exposure to,
    and change on account of a new culture all vary, and all reshape, immigrant beliefs and

    behaviors. These are largely within-group effects. Parents of Mexican descent report using

    more harsh discipline than European American parents residing in the same neighborhoods

    in a U.S. city (Hill, Bush, & Roosa, 2003). However, “less acculturated” Mexican

    Americans report greater use of harsh discipline than “more acculturated” Mexican

    Americans. Harsh discipline is also associated with fewer conduct problems and with higher

    levels of warmth among less acculturated Mexican American adolescents, a relation that is

    not present among more acculturated Mexican Americans and is negative among European

    Americans. Harsh discipline therefore appears to be distributed and to function differently

    among less versus more acculturated Mexican American families. Among immigrant

    Chinese parents in the United States, greater English media consumption (a marker of

    acculturation) is associated with more European-American normative authoritative parenting

    (e.g., high warmth, reasoning, responsiveness, and encouragement of children’s democratic

    participation), which in turn is associated with higher social and behavioral competence

    among children (Chen et al., 2014). Similarly, Korean immigrant mothers who are more

    acculturated to U.S. American culture use warmth more and encourage autonomy more

    toward their children than mothers who are less acculturated (Shin, Bayram-Ozdemir, Lee,

    & Cheah, 2010), just as Chinese immigrant mothers who are more acculturated to U.S.

    American culture use praise more to boost self-confidence in their children than mothers

    who are less acculturated (Cheah, Leung, & Zhou, 2013).

    Degree of acculturation affects adults (parents) and their children both. Less acculturated

    Asian Americans experience more challenges in their occupational careers even if they are

    well educated (Saad, Sue, Zane, & Cho, 2012; Zane & Song, 2007), and paternal income is

    protective of psychological well-being in highly acculturated, not in less acculturated,

    Mexican American families (Crouter, Davis, Updegraff, Delgado, & Fortner, 2006).

    Reciprocally, more acculturated parents are likely more accustomed to the mainstream

    Bornstein Page 12

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    educational system which may benefit their children; separate from paternal education and

    family income, more acculturated Chinese American fathers have adolescents who get better

    grades (Kim, Wang, Chen, Shen, & Hou, 2015). Educational attitudes are more closely tied

    to educational achievement among more acculturated Asian American mothers (Shen, Kim,

    & Wang, 2016). More acculturated Latin American mothers read more (Raikes et al., 2006),

    provide a more cognitively stimulating home environment (Schmitz, 2005), and engage in

    more structuring and verbal inquiry during parent-child interactions than less acculturated

    Latina American mothers (Howes & Obregon, 2009; Teichman & Contreras-Grau, 2006).

    Likewise, more acculturated Latino fathers spend more time in literacy-related activities

    with their infants than do less acculturated Latino fathers (Cabrera, Shannon, West, &

    Brooks-Gunn, 2006). A study of Indian immigrant fathers in the United States revealed that

    those who were more acculturated (e.g., use English more, eat non-Indian food, yearn less

    for their homeland) were more engaged with their toddlers than Indian immigrant fathers

    who were less acculturated, controlling for family characteristics (e.g., parents’ age, family

    size; Jain & Belsky, 1997). Mexican American and Chinese American fathers who are more

    acculturated (i.e., longer residency, United States citizenship, greater English proficiency)

    engage in more cognitively stimulating activities, than their less acculturated counterparts

    (i.e., shorter residency, preference for country-of-destination language use; Capps, Bronte-

    Tinkew, & Horowitz, 2010).

    Adolescents who are more oriented to their heritage culture are more likely to feel they

    matter to their parents and are more likely to experience feelings of efficacy related to

    language brokering (translating and interpreting for parents), whereas adolescents who are

    less oriented to the heritage culture are more likely to feel alienated from their parents and to

    experience feelings of burden related to language brokering (Wu & Kim, 2009). Not

    unexpectedly, degree of acculturative immersion has methodological implications: Marin,

    Gamba, and Marin (1992) learned that the tendency for Latin Americans to both agree and

    use more extreme response sets diminished as they acculturated more into mainstream

    European American society.

    Once, the status of an immigrant in the New World had thoroughgoing legal, economic, and
    cultural ramifications. The “casta system” distinguished fine gradations of lineage as, for

    example, peninsulares (Spanish born in Spain) versus criollos (pure Spanish descent but

    born in the Americas) versus mestizos (one Spanish and one Amerindian parent) versus

    castizos (one Spanish and one mestizo parent) versus cholos (one mestizo and one

    Amerindian parent) versus zambos (one Amerindian and one African parent), and on and on

    through chino, lobo, mulatto, pardo, etc. Sociocultural privileges as well as restrictions

    accompanied these differential statuses.

    Some nation states (Japan) deem citizenship by blood, jus sanguinis, others (the United
    States) by location of birth, jus soli. The statuses of foreign born – particularly their access
    to citizenship – differ globally. So-called “nationality laws” vary tremendously. Within the

    E.U., people could choose their country of residence freely, and an emerging trend for

    migrants is to acquire and to maintain dual citizenship. An E.U. law established at the end of

    2014 allows children to maintain dual citizenship for as long as they choose. In the Gulf

    States, independent of their length of residence foreign born guest workers have no right to

    Bornstein Page 13

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    citizenship; in Germany and Japan, it is trying (but not impossible) for a foreign born person

    to gain citizenship; in Canada and the United States, the foreign born regularly become

    citizens. Japan issues 27 types of visa each of whose requirements and authorized activities

    differ. The criteria for naturalization are provided in Article 5 of the Japanese Nationality

    Act and include continuous residence in Japan for 5 years or more, at least 20 years of age

    and otherwise legally competent, a history of good behavior generally, and no past history of

    seditious behavior, sufficient capital or skills, either personally or within family, to establish

    support for oneself, and renunciation of foreign citizenship. By contrast, ready naturalization

    has long been recognized as a crucial step toward full integration of immigrants into U.S.

    society. According to Warren and Kerwin (2015), 8.6 million U.S. residents were eligible to

    naturalize in 2013. Mexican nationals constitute the largest naturalization-eligible population

    at 2.7 million, followed by Indian (337,000), Chinese (320,000), Cuban (316,000), and

    Canadian (313,000) nationals. Fifty countries have 25,000 or more naturalization-eligible

    persons. Because Puerto Ricans are U.S. citizens, their patterns of migration and settlement

    in the United States differ from other immigrant Caribbean peoples to the United States

    (Baer, 1992; Massey, 1981; Santiago, 1992; South et al., 2005).

    Today, documented versus undocumented legal status constitutes a setting condition that has

    parallel critical thoroughgoing consequences for acculturates in terms of residence, school

    enrollment, availability of medical services, and standing before the law (Menjivar, 2006;

    Yoshikawa & Kalil, 2011). Referring to human beings as “illegal” may be offensive and

    dehumanizing as well as legally inaccurate: Under Title 8 immigration law, foreigners are

    only considered “immigrants” if granted an immigrant visa by a U.S. consul. Foreign

    nationals who are citizens of other countries are misnomered “immigrants.” Legal

    immigrants differ from the undocumented in level of education and occupational status as

    well as the psychological and personological turmoil the undocumented routinely face

    (Gonzales & Chavez, 2012; Hall & Greenman, 2015). In the United States, undocumented

    immigrants, even unaccompanied minors (a “humanitarian crisis” at more than 10,500 in

    just one month of 2015 in the words of the sitting U.S. President), are not entitled to a public

    defender in court (Nazario, 2013). One-third are children, and 5 to 10% of children are

    unaccompanied minors (Berthold, 2014). The undocumented immigrant population in the

    United States hovers around 11 million, and that is likely an underestimate given the

    difficulty in accurately tracking unauthorized immigrants (Pew Hispanic Center, 2013).

    In brief, setting condition, including reason for migrating, places of emigration and
    immigration and their mutual fit, variation in acculturation immersion and experience, and

    immigrant status before the law, shapes the ways people acculturate. Acculturation is

    moderated by setting condition.

  • Specificity Principle: People
  • Not every migrant or migrant group acculturates in the same manner. Whether taking place

    at the group or individual level, acculturation appears to proceed in different ways, to follow

    different timetables, and to achieve different final states (Phinney, 2006; Phinney & Flores,

    2002). As just discussed, culture of origin is a differentiating factor in acculturation; here I

    focus on individual factors. Vast disparities in psychological acculturation characterize

    Bornstein Page 14

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    individuals from different cultures of origin settling in different cultures of destination and

    even individuals from the same culture of origin settling in the same culture of destination

    (Nauck, 2008). Person is the second term in specificity where individual-difference

    characteristics of immigrants are key. They include, notably, gender, personality, and

    motivation, but also a raft of other individual-difference factors.

    Person: Gender

    Females and males may each account for about 50% of global migrants (Boehm, 2012;

    United Nations, 2016), but gender differences pervade life (Bornstein, 2013), and so

    acculturation is gendered in that girls and boys, women and men, acculturate differently

    (Chuang & Tamis-LeMonda, 2013). Transnational migration impacts females and males in

    contrasting ways, and it modifies relationships between them. Women tend to be more

    distressed by interpersonal adversities that are common after migration, whereas men tend to

    be more distressed by migration-associated work-related troubles, financial problems, and

    discrimination (Farmer & McGuffin, 2003). Immigrant girls who are separated from their

    parents are more likely to report depressive symptoms than immigrant boys (Suárez-Orozco,

    Todorova, & Louie, 2002), and immigrant boys score higher on emotional instability,

    aggression, and behavioral problems and lower on prosociality than immigrant girls

    (Derluyn, Broekaert & Schuyten, 2008; Dimitrova & Chasiotis, 2012; Samm et al., 2008).

    Turkish immigrant mothers and fathers in Germany disagree with respect to conservative

    gender role values, and Turkish daughters are more egalitarian than their mothers and their

    male peers, whereas Turkish sons more resemble their fathers in conservatism (Idema &

    Phalet, 2007). Second-generation Turkish immigrant adolescent girls in Belgium perceive

    less discrimination and adapt better than boys: They are more open to intercultural contact,

    have greater aspirations for achievement, and possess less conservative gender role attitudes

    (Güngör & Bornstein, 2009, 2013). Latina adolescents in the United States perform better

    academically than Latino adolescents (Henry, Merten, Plunkett, & Sands, 2008).

    Acculturation also follows gendered pathways that reflect different affordances and

    constraints on bicultural development (Suárez-Orozco & Qin, 2006), and girls and boys are

    often exposed to contradictory norms and demands of cultures of origin and destination.

    Immigrant children from more traditionally gendered societies may encounter modern

    mainstream cultures that are organized around ideals of gender equality, a circumstance that

    has different consequences for girls and boys (Donato, Gabaccia, Holdaway, Manalansan, &

    Pessar, 2006; Hondagneu-Sotelo, 2001; Suárez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco, 2001; Zhou,

    2000). Turkish immigrant parents in the Netherlands and Germany place enhanced

    achievement aspirations for, and greater conformity pressures on, sons than daughters as

    sons are considered future caregivers of their aged parents in traditional Turkish families

    (Phalet & Schönpflug, 2001). Among Vietnamese Australians (Rosenthal, Ranieri, &

    Klimidis, 1996) and Chinese university students in Canada (Tang & Dion, 1999), boys tend

    to be more similar than girls to their parents in terms of normative collectivism and

    traditionalism. Among Mexican American adolescents in the United States, perceptions of

    warmth from both mothers and fathers in early adolescence are related to later intimacy in

    friendships in girls, whereas only warmth from fathers is related in boys (Rodríguez, Perez-

    Brena, Updegraff, & Umaña-Taylor, 2014). Cultural messages may be delivered differently

    Bornstein Page 15

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    or interpreted differently based on gender. Together these findings point to gender as an

    important moderator of acculturation among immigrants.

    Gender likewise moderates culture-of-destination responses to immigrants. For example,

    Dutch adolescents tend to evaluate female immigrants more positively than male immigrants

    (Poppe & Verkuyten, 2012), and Dutch girls tend to be less concerned with ethnic group

    differences (Verkuyten & Thijs, 2001), and are more likely to consider their Turkish and

    Chinese peers in Holland as being Dutch, than Dutch boys (Verkuyten, Thijs, & Sierksma,

    2013).

    Turning from children to adults, prevailing gendered parenting in Dutch majority families

    distinguishes fathers and mothers in terms of authoritarian (paternal) parenting and

    permissive (maternal) stances, whereas relatively harsh parenting prevails among minority

    fathers and mothers alike in Antillean, Cape Verdian, Maroccon, Surinamese, and Turkish

    origin Dutch families (Flink et al., 2012). Mothers with stronger Mexican-heritage values

    engage in more cultural socialization, which is related to their adolescents’ higher ethnic

    identity and stronger endorsement of Mexican values over time, whereas fathers’ cultural

    socialization efforts show no similar links (Knight et al., 2011). Parental cultural values

    among immigrant Chinese Canadians show similar effects (Su & Costigan, 2009): Greater

    maternal (not paternal) endorsement of family obligation values is associated with higher

    ethnic identity achievement among adolescents as mediated through adolescents’

    perceptions of their mothers’ family obligation values. Adolescents need to perceive their

    mothers’ family values accurately to influence ethnic identity; cultural socialization efforts

    of mothers are more closely linked than are fathers’ to the formation of ethnic identity

    among adolescents. When mothers and adolescents both speak the same language (Chinese),

    mothers can be more involved in their children’s education, so that adolescents experience

    greater support (Costigan & Dokis, 2006).

    Person: Personality

    Other broad individual-difference person characteristics appear to affect the meaning and

    course of acculturation (Chiu, Morris, Hong, & Menon, 2000). Personality is consensually

    believed to be constituted of five factors (openness, neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness,

    and conscientiousness), each of which has been reported to have unique implications for

    acculturation (Kosic, 2006; Ryder, Alden, & Paulhus, 2000; Ward, Leong, & Low, 2004).

    Thus, constellations of personality dimensions will predispose people to acculturate

    differently (Schmitz, 1994). For example, greater openness may facilitate acculturation

    because individuals higher in this disposition can be expected to have less rigid views, make

    greater efforts to learn about a new culture, or modify their own behavior to accord with

    culture-of-destination norms (Mendenhall & Oddou, 1985). Reciprocally, less open

    immigrants may perceive cultures of origin and destination more rigidly as they do the

    boundaries between them (Yzerbyt, Corneille, & Estrada, 2001), and they can be expected to

    be more refractory to culture-of-destination norms. In this way, openness may lead to the

    embrace or the denial that one’s two cultural identities, values, and life styles can articulate

    or need to remain separate (Benet-Martinez & Haritatos, 2005). Other personality facets

    play roles as well. Conscientiousness predicts immigrant effectiveness (Ones &

    Bornstein Page 16

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Viswesvaran, 1999); extraversion and agreeableness relate negatively to immigrants’ desire

    to return to their culture of origin (Caligiuri, 2000); lower neuroticism and higher

    conscientiousness are associated with culture-of-destination maintenance, and lower

    neuroticism and higher extraversion and openness are associated with culture-of-destination

    adjustment (Ryder et al., 2000).

    Not only does personality affect acculturation, personality is also affected by acculturation.

    Normative personality profiles on the Big Five contrast in Japanese and European

    Americans (Bornstein et al., 2007). An examination of personality concordance in Japanese

    American immigrants with mainstream groups in Japan and the United States revealed that

    greater involvement in the mainstream European American culture changed personality in

    Japanese immigrants (Güngör, Bornstein, De Leersnyder, Cote, Ceulemans, & Mesquita,

    2013).

    Optimism, a positive expectation for the future, is a vital psychological resource for those

    experiencing negative or stressful life circumstances (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010).

    Being optimistic may be particularly important for immigrants. For example, many

    immigrants move to new cultures of destination in hopes of improving their living

    conditions as well as those of their children. The “immigrant optimism hypothesis” posits

    that immigrants have faith in the future despite the many barriers (e.g., language, poverty)

    they encounter to social and economic mobility. Immigrants higher in optimism have better

    outcomes than those without such faith in the future (Kao & Tienda, 1995; Taylor, Larsen-

    Rife, Conger, & Widaman, 2012).

    Some adults enter a new culture of destination with a strong desire to fully assimilate into a

    way of life, whereas others come with a desire to maintain values and ways of doing things

    from their culture of origin (Zhou, 1997). Thus, individual motivation plays multivocal parts

    in migration and acculturation. Motivation is involved in determining steps along the

    immigration path from first to last, from the decision to emigrate from the culture of origin

    to how hard to work to learn the language and mazeways of the culture of destination.

    Individuals who choose to emigrate have been hypothesized to possess a specific set of

    motivational needs (higher achievement and power motivation, lower affiliation motivation)

    that differentiates them from those who do not intend to emigrate (Boneva & Hanson Frieze,

    2001). Emigrants high in achievement motivation may migrate in search of better

    opportunities, to seek greater challenges, and to avoid the routine; those high in power

    motivation may be more willing to take risks to reach their goals and to impress others.

    Tartakovsky and Schwartz (2001) conceptualized multiple motivations to emigrate—

    presentation (physical, social, and psychological security), self-development (personal

    growth in abilities, knowledge, and skills), and materialism (financial wealth)—and they

    confirmed the relative importance of these motivations in predicting group identification,

    subjective well-being, and the eventual economic situations of immigrants.

    Person: Individual-Difference Characteristics

    A host of other individual-difference factors has also been implicated in acculturation. Some

    prominent ones are personological, social, and cognitive. Briefly, in terms of personological

    characteristics, anxiety can be detrimental to task performance when attention and deliberate

    Bornstein Page 17

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    effort are required, as is the case for immigrants’ learning a new language (Roccas &

    Brewer, 2002). So, lower social anxiety is associated with better adjustment to the culture of

    destination (Ryder et al., 2000). Likewise, high levels of aggravation undermine the

    immigrant experience (Yu & Singh, 2012), and aversion to ambiguity in immigrants is

    associated with greater stress, expressed emotional disorder, and psychosomatic symptoms

    (Kosic, 2002; Kurglanski & Webster, 1996). Self-esteem predicts immigrant adaptation

    (Valentine, 2001): Among Chileans, Turks, and Vietnamese in Norway and Sweden, higher

    self-esteem relates to possessing bicultural attitudes (Sam & Virta, 2003). Internal locus of

    control facilitates immigrants’ cultural adjustment, whereas external locus is associated with

    symptoms of psychological distress (Ward, Chang, & Lopez-Nerney, 1999; Ward &

    Kennedy, 1992). Immigrants higher in self-monitoring display greater adjustment

    (Montagliani & Giacalone, 1998). As Hamilton and Hamilton (2006) argued applies to

    adolescents transitioning to adulthood, immigrants fare better as they possess transparency

    (i.e., can “see through” challenges of their present circumstances to plan courses of action

    that will move them to their future goals) and permeability (i.e., will marshal the effort to put

    those plans into effect).

    Immigrants vary in their national and ethnic identities (Schwartz et al., 2010), as do

    members of the native population. Immigrants are likely to have more than one identity and

    to feel differently “at home” in the cultures of origin and destination. Biculturalism refers to

    one’s comfort interacting in both ethnic and mainstream contexts, ease of switching between

    the two contexts, and perceptions of advantages in being able to move between the two

    contexts (Basilio et al., 2014). So, cultural identity moderates acculturation. For example,

    positive associations obtain between degree of Jewish identity and psychological adjustment

    among immigrants from the FSU to Israel (Epstein & Levin, 1996). Turkish identity is a

    positive predictor, and mainstream identity a negative predictor, of language value and

    preference in Turkish immigrants to various countries (Yagmur & van de Vijver, 2011).

    Higher compared to lower national identification in Dutch children predicts more positive

    attitudes toward Turkish and Chinese peers who assimilated to Dutch culture than toward

    peers who preferred separation (Verkuyten et al., 2013): Higher identifying children are

    more concerned about identity threats (Nesdale, Maass, Durkin, & Griffiths, 2005; Pfeifer et

    al., 2007), and so higher identifiers are more positive about emigrants who prefer separation

    from Turkish culture and integration and assimilation with Dutch culture.

    Individual differences in cognitive and verbal abilities also influence acculturation, such as

    the ability to code switch (Carhill, Suárez-Orozco, & Páez, 2008), and commanding more

    intellectual skills, likely facilitates learning a new language and culture (van de Vijver,

    2017). For example, in the United States Mexican-origin fathers who speak only Spanish are

    less likely than other comparable Mexican-origin fathers who also speak English to become

    engaged in their children’s schools, even though both groups value education (Lopez, 2007).

    Asian American children are sometimes characterized as a “model minority” (Gewertz,

    2004); however, they – as all other groups — display wide variation in academic performance

    (Han & Huang, 2010; Qin, Way, & Mukherjee, 2008; Sy & Schulenberg, 2005).

    Finally, person informs acculturation methodology. The same acculturation construct may be

    moderated by the reporter assessing and quantifying it. According to Moroccan parents and

    Bornstein Page 18

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    children, problem behaviors in Moroccan children in the Netherlands are as frequent as

    those found in indigenous Dutch children; but, according to Dutch teachers, Moroccan

    children display higher rates of problem behaviors than their indigenous Dutch peers

    (Stevens et al., 2003). Albanian and Serbian immigrant children in Italy, their country of

    settlement, feel positively about their social relationships; however, these behavioral patterns

    go largely undetected by teachers (Dimitrova & Chasiotis, 2012). Teachers rate Asian boys

    as hyperactive, where by observational measures they are not (Sonuga-Barke, Monocha,

    Taylor, & Sandberg, 1993).

    In brief, person factors shape acculturation. Just as people distribute themselves along a
    continuum of vulnerability-to-resilience to specific experiences, and some are affected more,

    whereas others less, by the same experience on account of individual differences, the two

    genders and individuals with different personalities, motivations, or other dispositional

    characteristics acculturate differently – a massive experience. Most typological theories of

    acculturation elide over such individual-difference factors that centrally facilitate or impede

    acculturation, but acculturation is moderated by person.

  • Specificity Principle: Time
  • Time is likewise a moderator of acculturation. Just as some experiences (intervention

    program effects) vary in how long they take to establish themselves, and the effects of some

    experiences consolidate and strengthen or attenuate and weaken over time, so acculturation

    is an unfolding process, and when we catch it makes a difference. Time has several

    instantiations qua moderator of acculturation. Here, three prominent ones are reviewed: age
    of the immigrant, time in the culture of destination, and historical period.

    Time: Age

    One temporal consideration is the age (or developmental status) of the person acculturating.

    Wishing to keep Greek culture pure, Plato contended that people younger than 40 should not

    travel outside the country. Migrating children acquire beliefs and behaviors associated with

    the culture of destination more easily and readily than migrating adolescents or adults

    (Knight, Tein, Shell, & Roosa, 1992; Szapocznik & Kurtines, 1993), and adolescent children

    acculturate faster than their parents (Schwartz, Montgomery, & Briones, 2006b). Thus,

    immigrant youth are more often bicultural and bilingual, and immigrant adults less so

    (Carhill et al., 2008; Jia & Aaronson, 2003). From 2006 to 2013, the number of children of

    immigrants (children with at least one foreign-born parent) in the United States grew 12%,

    from 15.7 million to 17.6 million; in the United States, 24% of children have at least one

    immigrant parent (Urban Institute, 2016). Bilingual children exposed to high-quality input in

    two languages prior to the age of 3 perform better in phonological awareness, reading, and

    language competence than those first exposed to one of those languages after age 3

    (Kovelman, Baker, & Petitto, 2008). Individuals who migrate as adolescents or adults have

    had the longest and most direct contact with their cultures of origin and so retain more

    heritage mazeways (Shuval, 1982); these factors likely shape how they approach interactions

    with their culture-of-destination and culture-of-origin compatriots, but the same is less likely

    the case for child migrants (Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). Immigrants who move at a very early

    Bornstein Page 19

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    age to the country of settlement and attend school in the new context (sometimes called the

    1.5 generation) differ from the first generation that is foreign born and sometimes even the

    second generation that is born in the culture of destination (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). In the

    1.5 generation the adoption of the new culture can be complete or combined with a culture-

    of-origin orientation.

    German 3- to 6-year-olds with a migration background showed more behavioral problems

    than peers without such a background, and the migration/non-migration difference was

    larger in this age range than 7- to 17-year-olds (Hölling, Erhart, Ravens-Sieberer, & Schlack,

    2007). A study of Turkish adolescents in Belgium compared 14- to 17-year-olds with 18- to

    20-year-olds (Güngör & Bornstein, 2009); even among adolescents, age made a difference,

    and older adolescents attached greater importance to their culture of origin than did younger

    adolescents. Older adolescents assign more weight to maintaining their culture of origin in

    private life, but to adopting the culture of destination in public life, than do younger

    adolescents. For immigrant adolescents, acculturation is also entwined with normative

    developmental tasks associated with identity formation (Jensen, Arnett, & McKenzie, 2011;

    Motti-Stefanidi, Asendorpf, & Masten, 2012).

    Individuals who migrate as adults—and especially those who immigrate as older adults—

    appear to experience the most difficulties with the attitudes and practices of the culture of

    destination (Schwartz et al., 2006b). For adults, acculturation essentially constitutes a kind

    of resocialization with its attendant challenges. Recognizable foreign accents and frequent
    inability to speak or read the culture-of-destination language readily identify older migrants,

    inviting different receptions (including discrimination) from younger migrants and native-

    born individuals (Yoo, Gee, & Takeuchi, 2009). Thus, reciprocally, in the culture of

    destination, divergent perceived general obligations and responsibilities to adopt the culture

    of destination might underlie native children’s, adolescents’, and adults’ evaluations of child,

    adolescent, or adult immigrants (Gieling et al., 2011). More generically, with age a more

    complex understanding of groups and group relations develops (Killen & Rutland, 2011).

    Adolescents and adults consider group-specific norms, and the preservation of those norms,

    more important than do children (Killen, Rutland, Abrams, Mulvey, & Hitti, 2012). Sample

    demographic factors might even be expected to play a role in this connection. Latin

    Americans, with a median age of 29 years, are younger than most other immigrant groups in

    the United States, and so one might predict their easier and more rapid acculturation.

    Time: Penetration and Adjustment

    Time in a new culture (or generation) is a second temporal moderator of acculturation (Scott

    & Scott, 1989; Scott & Stumpf, 1984). Time and immersion experience (a setting condition)

    might covary, but are conceptually independent: A migrant might live a lifetime in a cultural

    enclave (a Chinatown, Japantown, or other) eating the foods, speaking the language, and

    reading the newspapers of the culture of origin. Nonetheless, family members with the same

    heritage may differ depending on their length of time in the culture of destination (Mendoza

    et al., 2007). Immigrant parents who reside in the United States for longer periods

    increasingly adopt American English-language and U.S. customs, and likewise change in

    their parenting (Cabrera et al., 2006; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008; Tamis-LeMonda, Sze,

    Bornstein Page 20

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Ng, Kahana-Kalman, & Yoshikawa, 2013). More time in the United States is a strong

    predictor of oral academic English-language proficiency as well as gains in social and

    economic resources among adolescent immigrants (Carhill et al., 2008). Time since arrival

    in the new country is also positively associated with identity and values, and so years spent

    in a culture of destination typically relate to adopting that culture’s beliefs and behaviors

    (Kwak & Berry, 2001).

    Thus, in families with at least one first-generation parent, the culture and language of origin

    are more likely kept alive than in families with two second-generation parents (Spiegler,

    Leyendecker, & Kohl, 2015). For offspring, speaking their parents’ heritage language is

    important to communicate with the first-generation parent (Leyendecker et al., 2014). Youth

    with migration backgrounds attribute increased importance to family members compared to

    their non-migrant peers (Wissenschaftlicher Beirat für Familienfragen, 2002). Parents and

    siblings are particularly important for adolescents with both parents born abroad, whereas

    those who have only one foreign-born parent indicate somewhat lesser importance of

    primary family members but still exceed their non-immigrant peers. Only in the third

    generation of immigrants, whose grandparents were born abroad, does the effect of

    enhanced family ties fade.

    Thus, family ties remain relatively strong among first- and second-generation immigrants

    but transition to those of the culture of destination in later generations. Navigating two sets

    of cultural norms and values simultaneously has implications for family relationships as well

    as individual development. In immigrant families, discrepancies often exist in parents’ and

    children’s language skills, social contacts, identifications, and values (Kwak, 2003). These

    parent-child differences may present significant impediments to positive adjustment among

    adolescents and their families (Unger, Ritt-Olson, Soto, & Baezconde-Garbanati, 2009;

    Wang, Kim, Anderson, Chen, & Yan, 2012). For example, acculturation gaps in language

    abilities can inhibit effective communication, rendering it more difficult to discuss sensitive

    emotional issues.

    Despite the socioeconomic challenges they face, immigrant youth show positive

    performance in academics and behavioral adjustment compared to their non-immigrant

    peers. This situation also changes with time in the culture of destination. For example, Asian

    and Latino immigrant youth in the United States report fewer behavior problems than their

    U.S. peers (Fuligni & Witkow, 2004; Coll & Marks, 2009), a difference also noted among

    Caribbean, African, and Eastern European immigrant youth in The Netherlands (van Geel &

    Vedder, 2010) and immigrant youth across the 13-country ICSEY study (Berry, Phinney,

    Sam, & Vedder, 2006; but see Albanian immigrants in Greece: Motti-Stefanidi et al., 2012).

    However, for many immigrant youth length of time in the culture of destination is associated

    with erosion of academic performance, familial bonds, and physical health (Hill & Torres,

    2010; Perreira, Fuligni & Potochnick, 2010; Portes & Rumbaut, 2001), a regular

    phenomenon now referred to as the “immigrant paradox” (Coll & Marks, 2009). Thus, for

    example, people of non-Dutch descent who grow up in the Netherlands acquire a cultural

    orientation that is similar to those of Dutch descent (Dykstra et al., 2013). The immigrant

    generation is mostly or exclusively fluent in their first language, their children are also fluent

    in that language to some degree (depending on which language their parents speak with

    Bornstein Page 21

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    them), but the third generation—immigrants’ grandchildren—speak the language of

    destination exclusively (Fishman, 1966; Verdon, McLeod, & Winsler, 2014). Nativity is an

    important predictor of physical health (Acevedo-Garcia, Bates, Osypuk, & McArdle, 2010),

    chronic disease (Huh, Prause, & Dooley, 2008), and mortality (Borrell & Crawford, 2009;

    Palloni & Arias, 2004), and better health outcomes often obtain in the first compared to later

    generations (Franzini & Fernandez-Esquer, 2004). It is also possible that the health paradox

    reflects person-specific selectivity – the migration of people with better health profiles than

    those remaining in culture of origin (Martinez, Aguayo-Tellez, & Rangel-Gonzalez, 2014).

    Time: History

    Historical time is a third temporal consideration in acculturation. Different cohorts of

    migrants (even from the same cultures of origin) migrate for different reasons, face different

    acculturation issues (even in a common culture of destination), and thus likely follow

    different accultural trajectories. Consider, first, historical changes in cultures of origin that

    inflect acculturation. Chinese immigrants to the United States in the 19th C worked as

    laborers on the first Transcontinental Railroad and emigrated due to overpopulation and

    poverty experienced in Canton Province; in the mid-20th C, refugees from Southeast Asia

    fled to the United States on account of wars in their homelands. Once, undocumented

    immigrants in the United States were predominately young men who crossed the U.S.

    southern border in search of work; contemporary demographic data reveal that the typical

    undocumented immigrant is 35 or older and has lived in the United States for a decade or

    more. In the early 21st C, the migrant flow from the roiling Middle East and Africa to

    Europe followed a path from Libya across the central Mediterranean to Italy; as that trek

    was long and increasingly dangerous, a shorter and safer route from Turkey to Greece across

    the eastern Mediterranean (Aegean) became preferable; but with the closure of Greece’s

    northern borders to migrants by neighboring countries, the Libyan-Italian route has partially

    reopened. Short-term socio-political changes in sending countries can have long-term

    immigration implications in receiving ones. January 1, 2016 marked the end of China’s 40-

    year one-child policy. Chinese immigrants to Australia’s Gold Coast born before and after

    that date will have experienced very different childhoods.

    So, historically changing circumstances in cultures of origin may be determinative in

    acculturation as are changes in cultures of destination. For example, contexts of migrant

    reception change over time. Italian and Jewish are today mainstream influences in New York

    City, but during the late 19th and early 20th Cs many Italian and Jewish immigrants in New

    York City were segregated (Sterba, 2003). Puerto Ricans migrating to New York in the

    1950s and 1960s met with discrimination, but New York in the 2000s and 2010s is more

    hospitable toward Latin American immigrants (Block, 2009). Conversely, Middle Eastern

    migrants in Europe and the United States may be experiencing more suspicion and

    discrimination since September 11, 2001 than they had experienced previously (Brüß, 2008;

    Critelli, 2008). In the Netherlands today, Moroccan and Turkish Muslim immigrants are

    evaluated similarly and negatively (Hagendoorn, 1995; Verkuyten & Kinket, 2000) as Dutch

    perceive high levels of threat from Muslim immigrants (Velasco Gonzalez, Verkuyten,

    Weesie, & Poppe, 2008), especially those who maintain their identity and culture (Velasco

    Gonzalez et al., 2008). Iraqis and Syrians who had the fortune to land on the beaches of

    Bornstein Page 22

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Lesbos, Greece before March 19, 2016 may petition for asylum in Europe; Iraqis and

    Syrians who had the misfortune to wash ashore after March 20, 2016 are being returned to

    Dikili, Turkey and only every other one may petition for asylum. The time difference of a

    day may alienate immigrant families forever.

    Many European nations once perceived themselves as countries of emigration rather than

    preferred countries of immigration. This perception changed from the 20th to the 21st Cs as

    now many of the same European countries are targets for immigration. OECD data from

    2010 indicate that Germany has higher immigration rates than the United States. In 2013, for

    example, 7 million people living in Germany were foreign nationals, and another 8.6 million

    were immigrants with a German passport. As a result, 20% of the German population is

    either foreign-born or has at least one foreign-born parent, and every third child age 5 or

    younger in Germany grows up in an immigrant family. However, Germany’s long-standing

    open-arms policy toward arriving migrants is now changing. Germany decided in 2015 to

    tighten its border controls and take in far fewer asylum seekers. Similarly, Sweden first

    offered permanent residency to all Syrians refugees, but in 2015 backed away from that

    commitment and now offers only temporary sanctuary as the norm. More generally, the

    principle of passport-free travel enshrined in Europe’s Schengen Agreement is coming under

    scrutiny. Sweden, Denmark, and Austria have abandoned passport free border crossings as

    the number of immigrants arriving at their borders surged to the highest levels since World

    War II. Forty thousand-plus refugees are stranded in Greece, and Turkey has agreed to

    become a regional refugee camp in return for Euros and E.U. considerations (visa-free

    European travel for Turks, etc.). Reciprocally, the number of undocumented immigrants

    from Mexico and Latin America to the United States, which more than tripled to 12.2

    million between 1990 and 2007, dropped by about 1 million in 2015 on account of the U.S.

    economic downturn (Krogstad, 2016).

    Changing technology is playing a role in acculturation as well. The flood of emigrants from

    Eastern Europe to the United States at the turn of the 20th C could not realistically expect to

    stay in contact with their cultures of origin, and the finality of their emigration doubtless

    engendered wholly embracing new prospects. Emigrants today phone, Skype, and text back

    home at will, fostering a starkly contrasting set of contingencies. So, Jamaican immigrants

    in the United States maintain close ties with relatives on that Caribbean island via diverse

    forms of communication and remittances (Ferguson et al., 2012), and Hmong immigrants in

    the United States Skype to maintain connections to Shaman practices back in SE Asia.

    Once, historically, immigration was essentially permanent and unidirectional; today,

    immigrant families often return to their native countries (reverse migration) temporarily (as

    tourists) or permanently (as repatriates). Such kaleidoscopically waxing forces of the new

    “Columbian exchange” (globalization) will likely continue to alter the complexion of

    acculturation. Even the shift in language use by generation (described earlier) may be

    changing in rate and taking place more rapidly (Hurtado & Vega, 2004). Young children in

    the United States who are from immigrant, multilingual families often become English-

    dominant during their preschool years, and decelerate their mastery of their parents’ native

    language due to increases in exposure to English inside and outside their home (Bridges &

    Hoff, 2014).

    Bornstein Page 23

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    In brief, time is a factor in acculturation. Acculturation is not static, but is dynamic on
    multiple planes. Yet, prevailing typological models of acculturation rest heavily on

    temporally frozen views of individuals and intergroup relations and so are of limited value.

    Acculturation is moderated by time.

  • Specificity Principle: Process
  • Beyond considerations of specific setting condition, person, and time, acculturation is

    moderated by the specific process through which a new culture is experienced and

    potentially absorbed. For Alexander the Great, the efficient method for spreading Helenism

    across Persia and the East in the 4th C BC was “acculturation by insemination.” So far,

    contemporary acculturation study has advanced only a handful of mechanisms by which

    acculturation is posited to proceed (Bornstein, Mortimer, Lutfey, & Bradley, 2011). One

    class of process pathway is socialization; a second is learning; a third is cognition; and a

    fourth is opportunity. In general, our understanding of the processes that moderate

    acculturation is remarkably poor.

    Process: Socialization

    Socialization is generally understood as a “process by which individuals acquire social skills

    or other characteristics necessary to function effectively in society or in a particular group”

    (American Psychological Association, 2013). In traditional conceptions of socialization,

    socializers’ primary roles are to communicate standards of behavior to socializees, help

    them understand and accept an appropriate identity, and guide them toward socially

    prescribed norms (Bandura, 1977; Davey, 1983; Devine, 1989). Often socialization is

    implicit, informal, and nondidactic. For these reasons, this process is an unspecified catch-all

    and frankly mysterious, hardly ever operationalized except by appellation – – internalization,

    introjection, identification, and the like. In the family, culture-of-origin socialization

    normatively reflects means by which parents transmit to their children the history, values,

    and customs of their culture of origin (Hughes et al., 2006; Peck, Brodish, Malanchuk,

    Banerjee, & Eccles, 2014). For adults, for whom acculturation necessitates that

    resocialization referred to earlier, the culture of destination must be the effective agent, and

    they must (somehow) pick up more culture-of-destination messages. Who socializes

    children into their new culture of destination is less clear and feasibly distributed. Parents

    maybe, but less probably; unlikely because of disjunctions that distinguish traditional and

    worn parent culture-of-origin beliefs and behaviors from new and exciting child culture-of-

    destination beliefs and behaviors. For immigrant children, parents’ cultural culture-of-origin

    values may be at odds with those of the culture of destination. Peers are likely effective

    agents (Bornstein, Jager, & Steinberg, 2012). The acculturating child’s compelling dilemma

    is whom to follow and what beliefs and behaviors to adopt when the cultures of parent and

    peer, of family and society, differ. For example, Japanese parents avoid confrontations and

    often retreat when children resist their requests, whereas U.S. American parents tend to

    exercise more direct control of children and may double down when children resist their

    requests. Japanese parents threaten to banish their disobedient children outside the home;

    U.S. American parents threaten to ground theirs inside the home. Chinese mothers opt for

    explicit verbal instruction when socializing control of negative emotions in their young

    Bornstein Page 24

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    children, whereas European American mothers favor modeling. Migrant children attempting

    to acculturate in Japanese American or Chinese American households in the United States

    would be exposed to warring accultural contingencies.

    Process: Learning

    A second more defined mechanism in acculturation is learning, addressing the cognitions

    migrants possess and the practices migrants pursue and their differential return. Here, as in

    classical operant theory, appropriate and desirable beliefs and behaviors associated with the

    culture of destination are incentivized and rewarded (through group acceptance, job

    promotion) and undesirable ones discouraged and even negatively reinforced (through peer

    rejection, discrimination). A popular form is social learning theory. From this perspective,

    individuals acquire new beliefs and behaviors through reinforcement, observation, and

    subsequent imitation of salient models, particularly those who are powerful. By dint of their

    higher-status position within the society and their roles as providers of support and advice,

    denizens of the culture of destination may be seen as both powerful and nurturant by

    acculturating individuals.

    Process: Instruction

    A third mechanism entails outright instruction in cultural practices. Formal tuition, curricula,

    and other similar institutional systems might constitute forms of didactic acculturative

    transfer, although Evans and colleagues (2012) argued that ethnic minority parents’ efforts to

    teach their children about the values and practices of culture should receive greater attention

    as central family influences on development. In this regard, Farver et al. (2013) observed

    that Latin American parents’ engagement in Spanish literacy activities is related to their

    children’s Spanish oral-language and print-knowledge skills, whereas parents’ involvement

    in English literacy activities is associated with their children’s English oral-language skills.

    Similarly, analyzing the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study—Kindergarten Cohort, Han

    (2008) learned that the academic trajectories of immigrant East Asian children related more

    to family factors (the home environment, parents’ educational practices, and expectations)

    than to school factors (resources, teaching practices, and learning environment), but the

    reverse was true for Caribbean and Latin American children. For adult migrants the

    workplace or second-language classrooms are likely locations of instruction. Indeed, an

    immigrant’s success at becoming a citizen of the United States entails studying for and

    passing a formal “Citizenship Interview and Test” (100 questions such as “What is the

    supreme law of the land?” and “What is one right or freedom from the first amendment?”

    (U.S. Citizenship and Immigration Services, n.d.).

    Process: Opportunity

    A fourth common route to acculturation occurs through the types of affordances provided or

    encouraged in the migrant’s life. Access to particular people or activities or settings

    (pediatricians, work, school) presents opportunities to acquire particular cognitions and

    practices; denial of such access operates oppositely (Reese & Gallimore, 2000). Better

    resourced families tend to invest in offspring well-being via providing more opportunities

    for productive encounters with potentially enriching materials and events (Conger &

    Donnellan, 2007). To the extent that certain culture-of-destination opportunities become

    Bornstein Page 25

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    regular in their lives, migrants’ knowledge, expectations, preferences, and actions are likely

    to acculturate. Early in life, children usually hear just (or mainly) the culture-of-origin

    language at home and only become bilingual through later access to the culture-of-

    destination language. The more children are exposed to any particular language, the more

    capable they become in that language. Thus, increased interaction with members of the

    culture of destination is associated with increased culture-of-destination language

    proficiency (Ward & Kennedy, 1999). Subtle and not-so-subtle facets of context contribute

    to immigrant youth development. Hamilton and Hamilton (2009) focus on developmental

    processes surrounding the transition from adolescence to adulthood, emphasizing programs

    and institutions that support the transition. For youth in disadvantage, including immigrants

    to Germany, the school-to-work transition affords a singular opportunity to advance. To take

    advantage and effect this change, however, youth must reside in a society that allows social

    mobility and live during a period when upward movement is possible. In Germany,

    apprenticeship is a popular model that supports this transition. Relative to their German

    peers, however, immigrant youth’s poorer school performance, limited language skills,

    conflicting cultural traditions, and discrimination hamper their access to and participation in

    apprenticeships. It takes small business owners in Germany to look past these differences

    and adopt immigrant youth into advantageous mentoring relationships. Children growing up

    in an Early Second Language Acquisition setting (ESLA; De Houwer, 1990) differ from

    children who may regularly hear both minority and majority languages in the home from

    birth, known as Bilingual First Language Acquisition (BFLA; Meisel, 1990). Exposure to

    and mere consumption of cultural products in the locale of destination (TV and foods)

    reinforce a cultural identity concordant with that culture (Cheryan & Monin, 2005;

    Guendelman, Cheryan, & Monin, 2011). Based on high levels of exposure to U.S. American

    culture in Jamaica (via U.S. media, consumer products, and tourism), some non-immigrant

    island Jamaicans display “remote” acculturation toward U.S. culture, beliefs and behaviors

    that in some respects resemble those of Jamaican immigrants actually living in the United

    States (Ferguson & Bornstein, 2012).

    Process: Transaction and Beyond

    In practice, acculturation is not unidirectional (as the foregoing socializer-socializee

    processes imply), but acculturation is interactive, transactional, and bidirectional. As Lerner

    (2002) crisply explained, individuals actively participate in their own development. So,

    observation and imitation are notably efficient mechanisms for acquiring cultural

    information. Migrants both search for and respond to cultural cues in different situations and

    draw implications from them that guide their future beliefs and behaviors. Thus, children

    think and seek to become imbued with culturally appropriate beliefs and behaviors, even

    ones they do not yet enact. Conformity to the culture of destination is typically seen as a

    disposition to copy an individual’s reference group rather than any minority displaying a

    different option. Conformity is especially common in contexts of ignorance or uncertainty,

    as characterizes the young and the acculturate. Normative conformity aligns the conformist’s

    behavior with local norms. In acculturation, conformity can pay off through capitalizing on

    established expertise of the majority in the new community (Whiten & McGuigan, 2017).

    Bornstein Page 26

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Only a small variety of acculturation processes has been identified, and as suggested earlier

    these mechanisms are still not well understood. Some new ones are also on the horizon. For

    example, intergroup attitudes appear to emerge in young children in forms indistinguishable

    from those of adults (Dunham, Chen, & Banaji, 2013). Implicit intergroup attitudes appear

    not to require a protracted period of internalization or social tuning, but are characterized by

    early enculturation and developmental invariance. In-group preference and status-based

    enculturation may be automatic and early-emerging mechanisms that represent rapid social

    orienting, in which children map membership and status onto existing social groups while

    simultaneously acquiring group representations. Deeper understandings of acculturation will

    depend on closer penetration of process mechanisms.

    In brief, to effect acculturation, four broad classes of process mechanism have been
    identified – socialization, learning, cognition, and opportunity. Participation in a culture of

    destination can structure acculturates’ experiences in ways that increase the likelihood that

    they acquire beliefs and behaviors that more faithfully jibe with those of the mainstream

    society and that children develop into culturally successful members of that society.

    However, cultures may vary in the normative use and effectiveness of different process

    mechanisms and employ different mechanisms for different outcomes: Socialization may be

    the primary mechanism for successfully acquiring social information about a new culture,

    but cognitive scaffolding may be more efficacious for acquiring declarative information.

    Add to these factors the agency of the acculturate. Each mechanism may be effective in its

    own way, so acculturation is likely moderated by process. The specific domain that is

    acculturating is a final moderating factor.

  • Specificity Principle: Domain
  • Acculturation is neither digital nor holistic, but a dynamic and multidimensional process. As

    individuals from one society come into contact with a new society, they will acculturate

    some beliefs and behaviors from the culture of destination but they will retain others from

    their culture of origin. Individuals likely seek to balance cultures of origin and destination

    (Stuart, Ward, & Adam, 2010). All manner of indices of beliefs and behaviors have been

    examined as outcomes in acculturation. Should we expect all domains of life (qua dependent
    variables) to acculturate identically? Assuredly not. Immigrants do not acculturate all facets

    of their being in a uniform fashion as assimilationists, integrationists, or marginalists, nor are

    they strict separatists (as typological views maintain). In accord with relational

    developmental systems metatheory (Overton, 2015), acculturation is best framed a

    multidimensional and dynamic process.

    Domain: Multidimensionality

    The domain term in specificity is not new. Acculturation science already broadly

    distinguishes among many classes of dependent variables that acculturate differently. So-

    called public domain activities tend to be aimed at participation in the social life of the

    culture of destination (interpersonal contacts), whereas so-called private domain activities

    tend to involve personal value-related matters of the culture of origin (family celebrations).

    The two realms acculturate differently. For example, Turkish and Moroccan immigrants in

    Bornstein Page 27

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    the Netherlands attribute more importance to their culture of origin in the private domains of

    home and family but more importance to their culture of destination in the public domains of

    school and work place (Phinney, 2006). From a different perspective, Ward (2001)

    distinguished among affective, behavioral, and cognitive areas in human life (referred to as

    the “ABCs of Acculturation”). Each, she observed, acculturates uniquely. Additionally,

    acculturation might be biological (including, e.g., medical health status), psychological

    (including, e.g., well-being), or sociocultural (including e.g., pragmatic communicative

    competence). Generally, domain-sensitive analyses reveal that different domains follow

    different trajectories of acculturation. Thus, an immigrant may seek to behave and be treated

    like culture-of-destination workmates, speak the languages of cultures of origin and

    destination, and maintain culture-of-origin relationship hierarchies or religion within the

    family. Acculturation is multidimensional and selective. Asian American and Muslim

    adolescents may or may not be proficient in or use their families’ native language, but still

    identify strongly with their heritage or religious values (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001), and Latin

    American adolescents who speak little or no Spanish might still endorse a Latin ethnic

    identity (Schwartz, Zamboanga, & Jarvis, 2007). For some Asian American adolescents

    (e.g., Chinese), socioemotional well-being appears to improve with acculturation (Chen,

    Hua, Zhou, Tao, Lee, Ly, & Main, 2014), but for other Asian American adolescents (e.g.,

    Filipino) acculturation is associated with poor adjustment, academic disappointment, and

    substance abuse (Eng, Kanitkar, Cleveland, Herbert, Fischer, & Wiersma, 2008; Hahm,

    Lahiff, & Guterman, 2004).

    Derluyn et al. (2008) compared migrant adolescents from 93 different countries to Belgian

    peers. They found more traumatic experiences, peer problems, and higher avoidance, but

    fewer anxiety, externalizing, and hyperactivity problems, among the immigrant adolescents

    than native Belgium youth. The same dependent variable can also have different effects in

    different contexts. For example, the status or prestige of a minority language versus a

    majority language has an impact on children’s success at school: Academic achievement of

    children whose minority language is as respected as the majority language is much closer to

    that of the majority than when a discrepancy in status of the two languages is considerable

    (Goldenberg, Rueda, & August, 2006). In college students from immigrant families, self-

    endorsed attitudes regarding obligations to family are positively related to academics, but

    behaviors initiated in response to perceived family demands are negatively related to

    academics (Tseng, 2004). Studies of Japanese and Latin American immigrants to the United

    States have suggested that transformations of parenting styles are uneven, with a tendency

    for behavioral adjustments in parenting practices to occur before changes in parenting

    beliefs (Bornstein & Cote, 2001, 2004; Cote & Bornstein, 2001).

    Domain: Dynamic Adaptability

    So, acculturation is multidimensional and selective; it is also dynamic. Increasing

    involvement with a new culture is believed to instigate changes in immigrants that result in

    better fit with the norms of the culture of destination and reduced fit with the norms of the

    culture of origin. However, this dynamic depends on the domain and is not always

    advantageous. In some domains immigrants may maintain their culture of origin (e.g.,

    religious practices in Foner & Alba, 2008; Ross-Sheriff, Tirmazi, & Walsh, 2007); in other

    Bornstein Page 28

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    domains, immigrants may be in the process of changing from culture of origin to destination

    (e.g., vocal interactions of parents in Gratier, 2003; knowledge of child development in

    Bornstein & Cote, 2007; restrictive childrearing attitudes; Chiu, 1987; Lin & Fu, 1990); in

    still others, immigrants may have completed a change from their culture of origin to

    resemble their culture of destination (e.g., personality in Güngör, Bornstein, & Phalet, 2012;

    mother-infant interaction in Bornstein, Cote, Haynes, Suwalsky, & Bakeman, 2012; Caudill

    & Frost, 1972, 1974). Caudill and Frost (1972, 1974), for example, observed Sansei (third-

    generation) Japanese American mothers and their Yonsei (fourth-generation) infants. In a

    three-culture comparison, Sansei mothers behaved more similarly to European American

    mothers than to mothers in Japan. To the extent that origin and destination culture norms are

    dissimilar, exposure to and involvement in the culture of destination may establish or

    strengthen ways of being that are more relevant and adaptive to the acculturation context.

    When investigators focus on a single outcome domain in acculturation, or in the typological

    school on one general pattern, they obscure the actual and more differentiated domain-

    specific nature of acculturation. Furthermore, domain variability in acculturation suggests

    certain other conclusions. One is that some aspects of life might be more plastic and

    sensitive to situation-specific demands, whereas others are less so. A second is that change

    in one acculturation indicator does not guarantee that others will change or change in the

    same degree or direction or on the same timetable. Acculturation research in many domains

    (self-evaluations, emotions) has revealed that immigrants become increasingly concordant

    with the normative patterns of the culture of destination over time and through contact (De

    Leersnyder, Mesquita, & Kim, 2011; Heine & Lehman, 2004). Such acculturation may be

    positive or adaptive. As discussed in reference to the immigrant paradox, acculturation is not

    uniformly positive; Riosmena, Everett, Rogers, Dennis (2015) examined how the health of

    foreign- and U.S.-born Latin Americans deteriorates with increasing exposure to mainstream

    U.S. society. Acquiring U.S. culture traits that have untoward health effects – negative

    acculturation – has become the primary explanation for generational Latin American health

    deterioration in the United States. Startlingly, the same domain may be positive and

    negative. Language brokering refers to assistance that children provide their immigrant

    parents in translating and interpreting written or spoken, formal or informal, cultural

    material from the culture of destination (Chao, 2006). Language brokering might benefit

    children’s cognitive and emotional development (Hua & Costigan, 2012) because it is

    associated with greater biculturalism and academic performance (Buriel, Perez, DeMent,

    Chavez, & Moran, 1998), higher ethnic identity and cultural value endorsement (Weisskirch

    et al., 2011), enhanced perspective taking and greater empathic concern (Guan, Greenfield,

    & Orellana, 2014), and greater respect for mothers and fathers (Chao, 2006). However,

    language brokering also confers risks on children by placing too much responsibility on

    them or exposing them to sensitive personal information about a parent or themselves.

    In brief, acculturation does not affect all domains of the psyche identically. Domain
    specificity predicts that different indices of acculturation can and will vary. Some indicators

    of acculturation may not change, and others change or change at different rates, to different

    degrees, in some but not other migrants, and so forth. Any uniform model of acculturation is

    Bornstein Page 29

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    inappropriate and misplaced. Rather, specificity appears more apt: Acculturation is

    moderated by domain.

  • Terms of the Specificity Principle Interact
  • The different terms of the Specificity Principle moderate acculturation. In bioecological

    theory (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006), development is hypothesized to be the joint

    product of several defining properties: Process X Person X Context X Time. In this

    formulation, characteristics and qualities of the developing person are posited to interact,

    and so on this model terms of specificity might be expected to condition one another. They

    do. Only a few illustrations are necessary to support this assertion. In theory, the ability to

    speak a heritage language is independent of the ability to speak the language of a

    mainstream society; however, setting condition interacts with person. Thus, Chinese

    immigrants in Canada and Australia often maintain their native language across generations,

    but Dutch immigrants to the very same places often lose their native language. Likewise, as

    suggested, different processes are appropriate to different domains of acculturation. And so

    forth.

    To bring this point home, consider how setting condition, person, process, and domain each

    interacts with time.

    Setting Condition x Time

    Years spent in the culture of destination may be unrelated to adopting majority culture

    practices among individuals who immigrate as adults to ethnic enclaves where culture-of-

    origin cognitions and practices prevail. In such enclaves, those immigrants can remain

    separated from the culture of destination across generations (Phinney & Flores, 2002). As a

    result, even if enveloping media promote acquisition of culture-of-destination cognitions and

    practices (Stilling, 1997), enclave conditions offset the effects of the culture of destination

    on loss of culture-of-origin cognitions and practices. New immigrants and older immigrants

    born long before in the sending society may continue to resemble one another. However,

    spatial assimilation theory posits that, as they adapt in a society, immigrants tend to move

    out of enclaves and into less ethnically circumscribed areas (Alba & Logan, 1991; Massey &

    Mullan, 1984). Whereas first-generation immigrants may choose to live with co-ethnics as a

    strategy of adaptation, for later generations continued residence in an ethnic enclave may be

    a consequence of blocked opportunities and housing market segregation. Culture is treated

    as static and non-changing, but culture is dynamic and is constantly changing. There is

    variability regarding what cultures value or find relevant at different points in time along the

    lifespan. As noted earlier, at one time Asians immigrated to the United States for economic

    reasons, whereas at a later time political motivations prevailed.

    Person x Time

    Asian American mothers are primarily responsible for socializing younger children (Inman,

    Howard, Beaumont, & Walker, 2007), and Asian American fathers are primarily responsible

    for socializing older children (Costigan & Dokis, 2006; Kim, Wang, Orozco-Lapray, Shen,

    & Murtuza, 2013). In the contemporary migration from the Middle East and Africa to

    Bornstein Page 30

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Europe, most initial asylees were young single men, but the balance later shifted to women

    and children, as well as unaccompanied minors, who have since accounted for the plurality

    of asylees; the gender gap in acculturation of values is larger in older than younger Turkish

    adolescents in Belgium (Güngör & Bornstein, 2009, 2013); years in the culture of

    destination are more strongly associated with adoption of culture-of-destination practices

    among adolescent girls than boys (Schwartz et al., 2006b); the risk for depression among

    Vietnamese refugees to the United States increases 2 years after their arrival, but only among

    the elderly (Hinton, Tiet, Giaouyen, & Chensney, 1997). First-generation women are

    sometimes depicted as carriers of their heritage culture because of their more limited

    connections to the wider receiving society beyond the home (Ward, Bochner, & Furnham,

    2001); as later-generation women acquire greater understanding of culture-of-destination

    policies surrounding education, welfare, and health care, and become skilled in the social

    mazeways of the culture of destination, they appear to embrace attitudes of those cultures of

    destination (e.g., gender role egalitarianism) more readily than do men (Hojat et al., 2000;

    Phinney & Flores, 2002; Valentine & Mosley, 2000).

    Process x Time

    Adults do not call on the same rewards to encourage behavior in infants, toddlers, children,

    and adolescents, even if the desired acculturation goals across development (e.g.,

    adjustment) are the same. Mainland China had a closed-door policy to immigrants until the

    late 1970s, and Chinese there, unlike Chinese in Hong Kong, had few contacts with Western

    cultures until products from the West were introduced through commerce, media, and travel.

    So, it is hardly surprising that Mainland China is less multicultural vis-à-vis the world at

    large than Hong Kong (Chen, Benet-Martínez, & Bond, 2008). Research with immigrant

    families often assumes that immigrant parents are threatened by their children’s increasing

    involvement in the mainstream society in the wake of immigration. Adolescents’

    involvement, however, does not inevitably create conflict or distress in immigrant families,

    even when parents do not share a high orientation to the mainstream culture (Costigan &

    Dokis, 2006). This is likely because immigrant parents typically desire their children to be

    successful in the new culture, and their children’s abilities to speak the new culture’s

    language, to form positive relationships with new culture peers and others, and to understand

    nuances in how members of the new culture think and behave are all indications of

    successful attainment of this goal. Parents are introduced to a host of new ideas regarding

    childrearing after immigration, and they may modify their parenting cognitions and practices

    to more closely match those of the larger society in order to socialize their children to be

    successful in a new multicultural context (Yee, Huang, & Lew, 1998). Value transmission

    studies within families demonstrate that parents understand what is normatively important in

    a society and socialize their children towards those values, even when those values do not

    mirror parents’ own (Benish-Weisman, Levy, & Knafo, 2013). Thus, immigrant parents’

    socialization goals for their children may not perfectly match their own personal values (e.g.,

    Farver, Xu, Bhadha, Narang, & Lieber, 2007), and parents may come to actively and

    intentionally foster their children’s competencies in the mainstream culture regardless of

    their own cultural orientation.

    Bornstein Page 31

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Domain x Time

    Different adaptations appear to follow different developmental courses. Acculturation

    studies point to decreases in ethnic behaviors, but not beliefs, from the first to the second

    generation (Bornstein & Cote, 2006; Nagata, 1994). Among immigrants to Israel from the

    FSU, more positive changes in acculturation took place 5 years after immigration as

    compared to 1 year (Shuval, 1982). Acculturation effects may need time to consolidate. The

    “immigrant paradox” suggests that physical, psychological, social, and cultural outcomes

    vary depending on the time since immigration (Borrell & Crawford, 2009; Coll & Marks,

    2009; Palloni & Arias, 2004). Thus, cumulative attainment must be considered to fully

    explain health deterioration among immigrants in the United States (Riosmena et al., 2015).

    The acculturation literature even evidences 3-way interactions: In ethnic enclaves, years

    spent in the culture of destination as a marker for adopting culture-of-destination practices

    applies to females who immigrated at young ages, but its weight with males and with

    females who immigrated as adults is questionable (Schwartz et al., 2006b). The effects of

    acculturation on peer liking depend in part on perceived host national belonging, vary across

    immigrant groups, relate to intergroup contexts rather than to migration per se, and differ for

    high versus low national identifiers (Verkuyten et al., 2013).

    In brief, as is the case in moderator analyses, the terms of the Specificity Principle of

    Acculturation Science interact.

  • Is Specificity New to Acculturation?
  • This article attempts to formalize a Specificity Principle in Acculturation Science. However,

    specificity is not entirely new to acculturation or, actually, to developmental and

    psychological science generally. Indeed, the field of acculturation studies has been drifting

    rather consistently toward specificity. Formalization here of the Specificity Principle is only

    a next logical (but nonetheless important) step.

    For example, the works of Ward and Phinney (mentioned above) already hinted at some

    specificities in acculturation, as did Arends-Tóth and van de Vijver (2006). Immigration is

    experienced in widely varied ways between and within groups, as a function of economic,

    cultural, sociological, political, and many other factors (Berry, 1997), and the notion of

    “segmented assimilation” suggests that acculturation could differ depending on the

    immigrant’s country of origin and the context of reception in the country of destination

    (Zhou, 1997). More generally, as in developmental science, this specificity perspective on

    acculturation implicitly references a PPCT-type approach (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006).

    Still others in development (Wachs, 2000; Wohlwill, 1971), and earlier Paul (1969) in

    clinical psychology, have championed specificity (even if not calling it that).

    To paraphrase Ecclesiastes (1:9), there is little new under the sun. Specificity — not the

    formal principle developed here, but, in crystallis form or one or another guise as an

    important psychological perspective — has been considered in the past by many authors.

    Here, I have merely built on that work and attempted to present the principle formally, to

    parse its central constituents, and to divine their implications with respect to acculturation.

    Bornstein Page 32

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    The typological view has also been thoroughly debunked previously (Rudmin, 2003, 2008).

    This treatment of acculturation to fill the lacuna in theory is inspired by the prevailing

    relational-developmental systems metatheory (Lerner, Lerner, Bowers, & Geldhof, 2015;

    Overton, 2015). This theory provides a rich and generative framework for organizing

    acculturation, drawing attention to central active constituents and features of acculturation

    and formulating ways to conceptualize relations among them, because it comprises

    numerous constituents that together account for the plasticity and dynamism that constitute

    acculturation. Together these considerations advance beyond the dated typological approach

    to acculturation in favor of a fresh specificity perspective.

  • The Specificity Principle, Acculturation Science, and Social Policy
  • With the foregoing in mind, we can see that the Specificity Principle has implications for

    theory and research and for social policy concerned with acculturation. A brief foray into

    each is warranted to round out this thesis.

    Pertinence of the Specificity Principle to Acculturation Theory and Research

    The Specificity Principle has immediate implications for defining acculturation.
    Acculturation has traditionally included “…those phenomena which result when groups of

    individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with

    subsequent changes in the original culture patterns of either or both groups…” (Redfield et

    al., 1936, p. 149–150). However, considering a broadened definition of process, for example,

    reveals that remote acculturation of immigrants through education and globalization occurs

    even in the absence of “first-hand contact” (Ferguson & Bornstein, 2012). Clearly, the five

    terms of the Specificity Principle identified here refine, concretize, and reinvigorate the

    classical, limited, and essentially vague definition of acculturation.

    Beyond definition, specificity has implications for theory, design, measurement, and

    methodology as it has for understanding the acculturation literature itself. The Specificity

    Principle advances theory and helps to make sense of disparate findings in acculturation

    research, to enhance the design of investigations of acculturation, and to identify gaps in

    acculturation science. No extant theory of acculturation explains how it is that individuals
    from the same educational, socioeconomic, generational, or familial backgrounds might still

    differ in their keenness and competence to acculturate. Moderators in the Specificity

    Principle offer a beginning explanation. Attending to specificity may also help to

    disambiguate factors that affect acculturation. For example, all families (immigrant and non-

    immigrant) experience intergenerational differences to some extent, so disentangling parent-

    child conflict that is related to cultural differences from parent-child conflict that is

    developmentally normative is challenging but necessary (Phinney & Vedder, 2006).

    Similarly, ethnic minority and immigrant families both face challenges and risks, such as

    discrimination, prejudice, and low income, and these risks have deleterious effects on

    individuals and families, but they differ by minority versus immigrant statuses.

    With the Specificity Principle in mind, acculturation researchers will want to re-evaluate

    some central design features of their research. For example, specificity requires reports with
    more detailed information about participants and the reference group to whom their findings

    Bornstein Page 33

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    apply (Bornstein, Jager, & Putnick, 2013). Researchers often adopt a pan-ethnic approach to

    studying certain immigrant groups, say “Latinos” or “Asians,” regardless of country of

    origin, but such a stance likely obscures significant and meaningful variation in some

    domains among peoples from different countries or cultures (Tamis-LeMonda, Baumwell, &

    Diaz, 2011). Most cultural subgroups diverge in language, immigration history, and cultural

    tradition. Mexican and Ukrainian immigrants likely did not acculturate similarly to U.S.

    cultural mazeways yesterday, just as Syrian and Afghan immigrants likely will not

    acculturate similarly to German cultural mazeways tomorrow. Analyses that combine even

    seemingly similar acculturating groups may mislead because levels of variables, and

    associations among them, often differ across groups, and results based on heterogeneous

    groups may obfuscate meaningful subgroup patterns (Song et al., 2012). Moreover, such

    lumping also confounds ethnicity, immigration status, and SES. In short, specificity shows

    how aggregating acculturating groups increases risks of unwarranted and inaccurate

    overgeneralization. Even the use of blanket appellations like “immigrants”, without

    specifying participant composition, age at immigration, time in the culture of destination,

    and so forth, can misinform.

    Until now, acculturation has also been measured haphazardly and poorly, applying years in
    the culture of destination, generation, nativity, and the like… sometimes interchangeably.

    Attending to each of the five terms of the Specificity Principle should help to bring a greater

    degree of operationalization, precision, and quantitative rigor to the measurement of

    acculturation. For example, nativity (i.e., being born in the country of origin versus being

    born in the country of destination; Harker, 2001; Kao, 1999) needs to be distinguished from

    the number of years spent in the culture of destination (Coatsworth et al., 2002; Gfroerer &

    Tan, 2003) as the two are not necessarily interchangeable and may have different

    implications and consequences. In ethnic enclaves, nativity may be a poor marker for

    adoption of culture-of-destination practices because results obtained using nativity may

    differ from results that would be obtained by directly measuring culture-of-destination

    practices.

    Specificity speaks to other investigative concerns as well. Many studies compare minority

    families and children to those of the majority group (Kouider, Koglin, & Petermann, 2014;

    Molcho et al., 2010; Nauck & Lotter, 2015). Research in acculturation also often relies on

    cross-sectional designs and samples of convenience (that frequently overrepresent low-

    income families) to assess relations between family or parenting influences and aspects of

    child or youth development. There are many fewer longitudinal studies of middle-class

    immigrants that might tease apart the common confound of SES and acculturation and their

    relative effects over time. The lack of longitudinal data is also a barrier to understanding

    processes and directions of associations among constructs of interest in acculturation

    research, and it is difficult to explain negative acculturation effects because most studies are

    correlational, vary in how acculturation is measured, and do not often disentangle

    developmental from acculturation change (Phinney, Ong, & Madden, 2000). Likewise, self-

    report and interview are common in the field, but the inclusion of experimental and

    observational research methods would expand the scope of the questions that are asked. It is

    past time to turn to systematic comparisons of different acculturating groups in similar

    Bornstein Page 34

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    settings, the same acculturating group in different settings, etc. so as to logically tease apart

    specific issues. Moreover, research conclusions based on large-N community samples,
    longitudinal designs, and independent reports from parents, youth, and others (e.g., teachers)

    would enhance the validity and generalizability of acculturation research. For example, the

    Specificity Principle points to the need for longitudinal designs involving pre- and

    postmigration assessments to address the temporal term of specificity more adequately.

    Acculturation is valenced and may take place towards the culture of destination or away

    from the culture of origin or both. Thus, specificity calls for pre- and postmigration two-

    sided comparisons in three-culture designs that include the acculturating culture as well as

    cultures of origin and destination. Daglar, Melhuish, and Barnes (2011) studied families with

    stable residence in Turkey and residentially mobile migrants within Turkey as comparison

    groups to Turkish immigrants in the United Kingdom to capture more precisely effects of

    mobility on acculturation. Such designs are needed to expose the specific complexities of

    acculturation.

    As elsewhere, methodology matters in acculturation science, and many methodological
    criteria must be met to ensure the validity of specific accultural comparisons (Matsumoto &

    van de Vijver, 2011). For example, who conducts the acculturation study, their culture of

    origin, their assumptions in asking certain questions, posing certain hypotheses, whom they

    study, and so forth can all shape acculturation findings. In the Netherlands, compared to

    native Dutch children, Turkish and Moroccan immigrant children show higher levels of

    internalizing and externalizing problems as reported by teachers. In contrast, immigrant

    children themselves report fewer emotional and behavioral problems compared to native

    Dutch children (Stevens et al., 2003; see also Loo & Rapport, 1998). It is possible that

    differences in behavioral adjustment are not perceived in school settings by teachers, or it is

    possible that teachers share negative social stereotypes of immigrants. Contra a typological
    approach (which tend to reify individual styles into static entities), the Specificity Principle

    recommends that acculturation researchers reconsider both person-oriented and variable-

    oriented approaches in their work (Seelye & Brewer, 1970; Taft, 1957), just as segmented

    assimilation theory is followed in sociology (Portes & Zhou, 1993). A dominant approach to

    assessment in psychological science uses single variables, combinations of variables, or

    relations among variables as the main conceptual and analytical units. Here a single datum

    for an individual derives psychological meaning from its position relative to the positions of

    data from other individuals. “In a variable approach, the lawfulness of structures and

    processes in individual functioning and development is studied in terms of statistical

    relations among variables. …” (Magnusson, 1998, pp. 45–46). However, the configuration

    of individual variables in a system also has meaning, and information about the individual as

    a Gestalt is of interest as well. The person approach is based on a wholistic-interactionistic

    research paradigm to development and functioning, meaning that it sees the individual or

    dyad as an organized whole, functioning and developing as a totality (Magnusson & Allen,

    1983). The totality derives its characteristic features and properties from interactions among

    its elements (the whole is more than the sum of the parts) rather than from the effect of

    isolated parts of the totality or as an integration of variables. In the person approach, the total

    dynamic complex process is not understood by summing the results of single aspects; rather,

    the whole individual or dyad is the main conceptual and analytical unit (von Eye, Bergman,

    Bornstein Page 35

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    & Hsieh, 2015). As Lerner et al. (2015, pp. 633–634) wrote, “Accurately capturing the

    oftentimes idiographic nature of development … requires that researchers consider the

    contexts in which their participants are embedded, as well as which coactions with those

    contexts are adaptive. This problem can be tackled through idiographic research designs and

    analyses …, or they may alternatively be represented [by] statistical interaction … (e.g.,

    Theokas et al., 2005).”

    The Specificity Principle addresses and resolves inconsistencies and discrepancies in the

    current acculturation literature and identifies gaps that will stimulate future research.
    Multiple moderation explains why the sizes of acculturation effects often vary depending on

    the particular situations, populations, periods, mechanisms, or variables of acculturation that

    have been considered; for example, why research uncovers links between particular setting

    conditions and domains for some, but not other, acculturating groups. Absent specificity, a

    great deal of variation in acculturation goes unexplained (Pessar & Mahler, 2003). That said,

    investigators should expect that different terms of specificity will explain only portions of

    variance in acculturation (Schmitz, 1994).

    Cultures that adapt to immigrants (via colonization, invasion, or globalization) without

    migrating themselves also “acculturate.” So, for example, still too little is known about

    indigenous children’s understanding of immigrants (Gieling et al., 2011; Malti, Killen, &

    Gasser, 2012; Pfeifer et al., 2007) or their evaluation of immigrants’ acculturation strategies

    (Nigbur et al., 2008). However, Verkuyten et al. (2013) examined how native Dutch children

    (8–13 years) evaluated migrant peers who followed different paths to acculturation. Some

    immigrants discard features of their culture-of-origin identity and adopt those of the culture

    of destination (Hartmann & Gerteis, 2005). These immigrants may be perceived to value the

    culture of destination and to become more similar to “us” and thereby “one of us.” In-group

    identity theory posits that inclusion in a shared category improves attitudes toward out-group

    members through the general tendency to esteem people who belong to the same category as

    oneself (Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000). Not surprisingly, members of the culture of destination

    prefer and evaluate more positively immigrants who adopt mainstream modes to immigrants

    who maintain their culture of origin (Van Oudenhoven et al., 1998; Verkuyten, 2005).

    Other gaps in the current acculturation literature reflect surprising “unknown unknowns”.

    Despite the wide range of domains that populate the contemporary research literature,

    developmental outcomes that are assessed are still limited in scope. Two examples. First,

    Yarumal in Colombia is well-known for an odd reason: It has the world’s largest population

    of people with Alzheimer’s disease; an estimated 5,000 of its denizens carry a gene mutation

    that causes an early-onset form of the disease. Taking acculturation into consideration (as

    provided by an identity-by-descent analysis), Kosik and his colleagues (2015) sequenced

    genomes of the town’s inhabitants and identified E280A as the mutation causing Yarumal’s

    form of early-onset Alzheimer’s. Kosik then traced the mutation to a common ancestor — a

    conquistador, soldier, and explorer of the Spanish Empire who arrived in Colombia 375

    years ago. Second, Cabrera and Leyendecker (2016) have argued for a shift in focus from

    deficit models of immigrant youth development towards positive psychology models that

    consider the effects of cultures of origin and destination in conjunction with other salient

    ecological contexts, such as social class, gender hierarchies, and school and neighborhood

    Bornstein Page 36

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    environments. Positive youth development is an undeveloped area in acculturation research

    ripe for study. Increasingly, research is identifying strengths and protective processes that

    promote positive development, and future research should embrace the study of positive

    development among immigrant youth (Neblett Rivas-Drake, & Umaña-Taylor, 2012; Zhou

    et al., 2012). Is positive development defined differently in diverse acculturating

    communities? Beliefs and behaviors that are valued in mainstream cultures of destination

    may be irrelevant or devalued in certain immigrant groups, and other culture-specific

    developmental strengths may be overlooked.

    Thus, attention to the five terms of specificity raises novel questions. For example,

    acculturation theories that collect individuals into types (assimilationist, etc.) essentially

    ignore or relegate to noise potentially meaningful individual-difference factors, such as

    personality, but as demonstrated above, different facets of personality moderate acculturation

    as the personality fits between individuals and their cultures of origin and destination play

    key roles in the process (Güngör et al., 2012; Kosic, 2006). Likewise, motivations condition

    both emigrant decision-making and immigrant adjustment. Boneva and Hanson Frieze

    (2001) deduced that individuals in a culture of origin who choose to emigrate possess

    different motivational needs from those in the same culture of origin with no intention to

    emigrate. Most contemporary research in acculturation focuses on peoples migrating from

    the developing to the developed world of usually Western, educated, industrial, rich, and

    democratic societies (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). What of acculturation in the

    opposite direction? When culturally disparate peoples come into contact with each other, the

    differences between them may become salient and can change cultural patterns in sending,

    acculturating, and receiving societies alike. To date, the focus of acculturation research is

    occupied with the acculturating group, but the effects on the cultures of origin and

    destination alike are of equivalent empirical interest.

    As noted, acculturation science has inadequately investigated basic mechanisms of action.

    What are the relative efficacies of media and communication, consumer products and

    tourism, technology and trade as vehicles of acculturation? It is not well-known how

    acculturation proceeds for those embedded in homogeneous ethnic enclaves where the

    culture of origin predominates versus those living in other heterogeneous contexts in the

    same culture of destination. Existing research on country-of-origin and country-of-

    destination members’ evaluations of immigrants has not adequately considered setting

    conditions of emigrants (Gieling et al., 2011). Immigrants who adopt the mazeways of their

    new culture of destination may be evaluated more positively in that culture than those who

    do not, but such emigrants are likely to be evaluated more negatively by their culture of

    origin than emigrants who maintain their heritage culture. Studies which differentiate

    between these different status groups allow one to tease apart the influence of the minority

    status per se, the support provided by the government, and degrees of discrimination

    experienced by children and families from different groups.

    Because ethnic minority children in pluralistic societies are developing in multicultural

    contexts, there is a need to understand what parents value of their own culture of origin and

    of their culture of destination to pass on to their children. The immigration context might

    provide a particularly fortuitous opportunity for immigrant parents to reflect on the values

    Bornstein Page 37

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    they transmit (or not) to their children as well as those they themselves do not possess but

    wish their children might.

    Finally, the five terms of specificity discussed here are not exhaustive, but rather illustrative
    of the principle. The tenability and value of these five terms will be a matter of continuing

    empirical scrutiny, and other terms (or variants) in specificity might arise in the future.

    Consider objective versus subjective acculturation qua independent variable (Spencer, 2006).
    Acculturation of immigrants is often measured by objective parameters, such as years in the

    culture of destination, employment status, language proficiency, and social network.

    However, more subjective components of acculturation may be operative. The numbers of

    years of settlement in the United States do not predict immigrant fathers’ involvement in

    their children’s lives, whereas acculturated attitudes constitute a significant predictor (Jain &

    Belsky, 1997). Perceptions — that one’s two cultures overlap or do not, are associated or

    dissociated, or are close or distant from one another — condition the acculturation

    experience, including exposure to each culture, language proficiency and use, and

    identification. In this connection, cognitive appraisal processes constitute important

    heretofore uninvestigated intervening variables in immigrant acculturation. It follows that

    “acculturation” is to some degree in the eye of beholder: Japanese university students who

    return to Japan after spending some time in the United States report “feeling un-Japanese”

    and experiencing lasting adjustment problems. When with peers, returnees claim to feel

    altered with respect to physical appearance, behaviors, and interpersonal styles (Kidder,

    1992; Yoshida et al., 2003, discussed peer perceptions that matched these self-reported

    feelings of anomie and poor fit). Japanese who had lived in Western cultures were more

    assertive and direct in expressing their opinions, which their Japanese homeland peers

    perceived as inconsiderate and crass (Minoura, 1988). These findings imply that Japanese

    Americans experience deep ambivalence: They may feel “Japanese” among European

    Americans but “American” in Japan. Reciprocally, from a European American perspective,

    Japanese Americans may seem rightly to retain their heritage cultural patterns, but from the

    deeply monocultural Japanese perspective, they may seem Americanized. In short, objective

    measures, such as time, as well as subjective ones, such as self-definitional markers that

    embrace phenomenology and attempt to capture psychological feelings of belonging to the

    new culture, are both meaningful, depending….

    In brief, the specificity principle has several implications for the conduct of acculturation

    science. As the way of the world continues, we can clearly foresee that the Specificity
    Principle will exert increasingly significant sway over the conduct of acculturation science.
    It will also profitably insinuate into social policy.

    Pertinence of the Specificity Principle to Acculturation Social Policy

    The Specificity Principle has implications for the design of interventions and programs in

    the service of acculturation, for respectful understanding of migrant cultures, and for policy

    in sending and receiving societies’ ideologies.

    Policy makers need to become mindful of the Specificity Principle and how it can inform

    their goals and good works. As detailed, acculturation effects are conditional and not

    absolute (i.e., true for all people under all conditions and so forth), yet majoritarian positions

    Bornstein Page 38

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    traditionally shape program development and policy recommendations. In the real world,

    interventions are applied to specific individuals and specific groups, rather than to abstract
    populations, and so to be effective they must be appropriate to specific people and specific

    goals as well as the specific social-cultural-ecological settings those specific people inhabit.

    Facing the same challenges, different migrants acculturate in different ways and different

    people respond to migrants in different ways. Different peoples carry with them their shared

    histories as a cultural group and their idiosyncratic immigration experiences (hardships,

    discrimination, sense of ethnic pride), and so programs, which are themselves cultural

    systems, must engage immigrants in activities, roles, and relationships within a framework

    that is compatible with those beliefs, values, meanings, and expectations. That said, such

    frameworks are all too frequently rooted within a uniform culture of the destination society.

    Therefore, cultural miss-fit can create misunderstandings, breed conflicts, and impede

    cultural engagement. For example, perceptions of discrimination are frequently cited by

    Latino youth and parents in the United States to explain the poor participation of youth in

    school (Simpkins, Delgado, Price, Quach, & Starbuck, 2013).

    Effective programs allow immigrants to build on and apply their knowledge, skills, and
    strengths (Cole, 2006; Villarruel, Montero-Sieburth, Dunbar, & Outley, 2005). Program

    designs therefore need to take into consideration specific setting conditions, people, times,

    processes, and domains of acculturation. Only a specificity orientation can hope to inform

    policies of specific means that will productively meet the specific needs of specific migrants

    in specific contexts at specific times. Structural characteristics of programs, such as the

    mode of delivery, duration, and frequency of services, and whether the program is part of a

    multimodal effort that includes other services, need to be accounted for to ensure program

    efficacy and improve program possibilities. In this connection, recall that Hamilton and

    Hamilton (2006) described a model German apprenticeship program for adolescents

    transitioning to the adult labor force; alas, national structural characteristics proved telling as

    85% of native German 25- to 35-year-olds came to possess a formal occupational certificate

    by this route versus 43% of Turk immigrants to Germany.

    Cultural backgrounds of immigrants from outside the dominant cultural group in a society

    are sometimes viewed from a deficit perspective. Yet each culture provides its members with

    funds of knowledge, tools, norms, and ways of thinking that can contribute to immigrants’

    engagement in and learning from program activities (Morland, 2007; Villarruel et al., 2005).

    For example, the high cultural value placed on mutual trust and cooperation (confianza) in
    many Latino cultures is a valuable asset among some Latino youth in immigrant programs

    (Salusky et al., 2014). This asset might aid youth’s collaborative work on program projects

    and contribute to processes that help them grow in responsibility, confidence, and maturity.

    More research is needed to understand how other specific cultural assets (e.g., cultural

    precepts, ties to family, models of relationships) can optimize program effectiveness for

    specific groups.

    Perhaps the clearest policy recommendation that has emerged from the acculturation

    literature is the value of supporting transmission of the culture of origin within immigrant

    families. One major way in which families foster positive acculturation is by exposing

    children to and valuing diverse frames of reference so that children become familiar with

    Bornstein Page 39

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    and feel comfortable interacting in multiple cultures (Mistry & Wu, 2010). Fostering

    culture-of-origin assets, programs that promote and support positive parenting and build

    family cohesion likely contribute to the positive development of immigrant youth (Corona et

    al., 2012; Leidy, Guerra, & Toro, 2012). A 2014 Council of Europe publication states that

    programs to promote adult migrants’ “linguistic integration” should “encourage them to pass

    [their mother tongue(s)] on to their children (at least using them within the family)” (Beacco

    et al., 2014). A UNICEF report covering several E.U. countries likewise stressed the

    importance of proficient bilingualism for young immigrant children’s well-being

    (Hernandez, 2012).

    Some programs, by chance or by design, provide socialization for specific cultural groups,

    and research suggests that those programs succeed in strengthening cultural consciousness

    and identity–– including understanding prejudice and acquiring coping skills – that

    contribute to well-being (Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, 2011; Riggs, Bohnert, Guzman, &

    Davidson, 2010). Sharing ethnic identity with peers is believed to provide a condition for

    youth to engage in identity building in a safe and supportive environment. Such situations

    may be especially valuable for youth from ethnic groups that have experienced trauma,

    injustice, or marginalization (Ginwright, 2010) as they provide opportunities for overcoming

    a sense of isolation, learning about and analyzing experiences of discrimination, and

    grappling with the “vexing and contradictory forces that shape their lives” (HoSang, 2006, p.

    16). Those same conditions may also provide opportunities for youth to become proactive in

    asserting their ethnic identity and to develop civic skills to effectively address grievances

    (Ginwright, 2010; Kwon, 2008). Further research is needed on the promising practices of

    such programs and how their moderations will best fit the needs, and capitalize on the assets,

    of specific immigrant groups.

    The value of specificity becomes even clearer in the shortcomings of programs that attempt

    to serve diverse cultural backgrounds. The possible combinations of immigrants from

    different groups in such programs overwhelm, and these different combinations likely

    contribute to widely varying program dynamics and successes. On specificity, programs that

    attempt to serve diverse ethnic groups with diverse goals in mind are likely to be much less

    effective in meeting culture-specific needs of immigrants from any one group because of

    challenges to provide specific supports of same-ethnicity staff, a critical mass of ethnic

    peers, and so forth (Okamoto, Gast, & Feldman, 2012). Only by focusing on Latina

    immigrant mothers’ daily pro-educational beliefs and behaviors were the negative effects of

    low maternal education overcome (Schaller, Rocha, & Barshinger, 2007). The difficulty of

    providing targeted experiences to multiple immigrant groups helps to explain the infrequent

    successes of “one size fits all” program models (Fredricks & Simpkins, 2012). An important

    related issue therefore regards instances in which a general program model is employed with

    youth from one immigrant group that differs from the group with whom it was originally

    developed. Cole (2006) described the adaptation of the Fifth Dimension to culturally fit

    children in a Latino neighborhood. Latino staff were recruited, and Spanish language and

    Mexican American heritage infused into activities, games, and program relationships. The

    program also drew on Latino norms of multi-generational assistance as a cultural asset,

    cultivating parent participation and encouraging older youth to take responsibility for

    younger youth. Specificity in action. Research is needed on the effectiveness of, and best

    Bornstein Page 40

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    practices for, adaptation of general program models to specific cultural and immigrant

    groups, in specific settings, etc.

    Immigrant families present distinctive issues and opportunities for social programs.

    Immigrant parents and children are typically involved in an ongoing process of cultural

    adjustment, one in which (as suggested) children may assimilate more quickly than parents

    toward the culture of destination; this generational disjunction can provoke family conflict

    (Berry, 1997; Chao & Otsuki-Clutter, 2011). Recently arrived immigrant youth are often

    isolated in their schools and neighborhoods, and youth programs can play an important

    “bridging role” in facilitating adjustment (Gaytan, Carhill & Suárez-Orozco, 2007). They

    can connect youth to peers in similar situations and to program staff and service

    professionals who can act as cultural guides. They can also provide opportunities to identify

    youth’s cultural backgrounds as assets (Morland, 2007) and help youth to develop bicultural/

    multicultural competencies (Schmidt, Morland, & Rose, 2006; Larson, Jensen, Kang,

    Griffith, & Rompala, 2012).

    Migrant families are economic and legal units just as they are social and emotional entities.

    However, the actual process of immigration presents many challenges (especially for the

    undocumented, whose access to health, education, legal, and social services is typically

    impeded or barred; Cavazos-Rehg, Zayas, & Spitznagel, 2007; Yoshikawa & Kalil, 2011).

    One parent and two child examples: Immigrant parents may feel optimistic that in a new

    homeland they might better fulfill the role of being a parent, but they can be thwarted.

    Migrant families are sometimes exposed to unique legal circumstances that arise when their

    normative culture-of-origin practices come into conflict with culture-of-destination customs

    or laws. Parenting practices that may be routine and perfectly acceptable in the migrant’s

    origin culture may be deemed “neglectful” or “abusive” in certain destination cultures. For

    example, Korean Americans living in Los Angeles regard certain American laws concerning

    child abuse to conflict with Korean heritage cultural values and childrearing norms (Song,

    1986). Such differences of opinion over “normative” parenting have been known to create

    cultural clashes between minority foreign-born parents and child protective services systems

    in the majority culture (Coleman, 2007). Belgium and the Netherlands enforce strict laws

    regarding child marriage, whereas religious leaders in Syria make exceptions for marriage

    starting at age 13. Now 13- to 15-year-old Syrian “child brides” in Belgian and Dutch

    asylum centers are posing similarly thorny questions to public officials.

    International adoption has been called a “silent migration.” Consider the experience of the

    adopted Chinese or Nigerian children growing up in the United States or the United

    Kingdom, where there is considerable cultural heterogeneity, versus in Scandinavian

    countries, where there is more cultural homogeneity. Children in the first instance are likely

    to have many more opportunities to learn about their cultural heritage and to meet others

    who share their cultural origins. Internationally and domestically placed adoptees fare

    differently (Juffer & van IJzendoorn, 2005, 2007), but the differential adjustment of these

    children is a function of where they come from, when they are placed, and where they wind

    up (Barni, Leon, Ronsati, & Palacios, 2008). Many children today are fleeing drug cartels,

    gang violence, sexual exploitation, and domestic abuse in the Central American triangle of

    El Salvador, Guatemala, and Honduras. Since 2011, more than 125,000 such unaccompanied

    Bornstein Page 41

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    minors have been stopped at the U.S. border (many placed in shelters funded by the federal

    Office of Refugee Resettlement) and nearly 20,500 younger than 18 were apprehended by

    the U.S. Border Patrol in Fiscal Year 2016 alone (U.S. Customs and Border Protection,

    2016). In a recent 2-year period (October 1, 2013 to December 31, 2015), more than 95,000

    unaccompanied minors were released into U.S. communities. The Vera Institute of Justice

    (Byrne & Miller, 2012) reported that ~40% of unaccompanied immigrant children

    potentially qualify for statuses that would exempt them from deportation from the United

    States, including asylum (because they fear persecution in their home country) or special

    immigrant juvenile status (because they have been abused or abandoned by a parent).

    However, the differential treatment of immigrants by their developmental status is woefully

    unappreciated. Even children, sometimes toddlers, who are charged with violating

    immigration laws have no right to appointed counsel and can be compelled to appear in

    court alone and unrepresented, when the U.S. Government is represented by Department of

    Homeland Security lawyers. The U.S. Justice Department reports that 42% of the more than

    20,000 unaccompanied minors involved in deportation proceedings completed between July

    2014 and late December had no attorneys. One senior U.S. Justice Department official

    contended in 2016 that 3- and 4-year-olds can learn immigration law well enough to

    represent themselves in court. The U.S. Court of Appeals for the 9th Circuit is hearing an

    appeal stemming from a Seattle federal court case regarding whether such immigrant

    children are entitled to taxpayer funded attorneys.

    On the basis of their nationalist immigration policies, whole countries can be arrayed from
    more pluralistic and open climates to more restrictive and closed ones; generally, the latter

    show lower levels of sociocultural adjustment and higher levels of ethnic orientation

    (Huddleston & Niessen, 2011). Immigration from outside of the E.U. is basically limited to

    family unification, to (acknowledged) refugees, and to highly skilled professionals. Some

    acculturating groups may be evaluated positively by their culture of destination, others

    negatively. The common esteem of ethnic groups according to liking or social distance from

    the mainstream is sometimes referred to as the “ethnic hierarchy.” In the Netherlands, for

    example, Chinese, Surinamer, and Antillean immigrants are evaluated more positively than

    Turkish and Moroccan immigrants (Ben Sira, 1997; Gijsberts, Huijnk, & Vogels, 2011;

    Verkuyten & Thijs, 2002). In some E.U. countries, Syrian asylum seekers are acceptable, but

    Afghans and Iraqis are not. Less well accepted migrants often experience hostility, rejection,

    and discrimination, encounters that in turn predict poor long-term adjustment. Societies that

    foster cultural pluralism are less likely to enforce cultural change or exclusion of immigrants

    and tend to provide positive settlement contexts. Holding culture-of-origin constant

    (Turkey), Yagmur and van de Vijver (2011) found that immigrants to Australia (an

    immigration country with a pluralistic ideology) showed higher levels of identification with

    their new mainstream culture and lower levels than their brethren immigrants to France and

    Germany (where cultural ideologies are more homogenous and the differentiation of

    indigenous versus immigrant peoples is more pronounced). Therefore, the receiving

    country’s acculturation climate helped to explain differences in the course of Turkish

    immigrants’ acculturation. Because of its proximity to the Balkan States, Northeast Italy has

    a longer history of migratory movements from Eastern Europe than do other parts of Italy;

    this region has stable settlements of Albanians and Serbians, greater numbers of family

    Bornstein Page 42

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    reunifications, mixed marriages, and local policies on education and community settings that

    facilitate the acculturation of newly arriving families (Marra, 2002). These factors partly

    explain positive social adjustment outcomes for Albanian and Serbian immigrant children in

    the area. By contrast, Albanian immigrant children in less-accommodating Greece

    experience more behavioral problems (Motti-Stefanidi et al., 2008). The social policies and

    ideological nature of the specific community to which immigrants acculturate thereby

    shapes whether and how they acquire culture-of-destination practices and accommodate

    those of their culture of origin (Phinney, Horenczyk, Liebkind, & Vedder, 2001; Ryder et al.,

    2000; Schwartz et al., 2006b).

    The 2015 world refugee crisis polarized Europe and altered cultural landscapes of the

    Americas. In Europe, every E.U. member agreed to accept an immigrant quota based on its

    gross domestic product and population. Under new rules, asylum seekers cannot choose their

    culture of destination. Previously allowing refugees to decide their country of destination led

    to an imbalanced distribution of the burden, and the quota system was abandoned. Sweden

    absorbed the largest per capita share of refugees of any E.U. country, but having exhausted

    accommodations in hotels, apartment buildings, tents, abandoned army barracks, and

    converted prisons, Sweden eventually housed migrants in the headquarters of the Swedish

    Migration Agency. When Angela Merkel opened Germany’s borders to Middle Eastern

    migrants, her German critics observed that she could say, “Yes we can,” but she also had to

    say, “What, when and how.” Ultimately, 1.1 million refugees reached Germany in 2015,

    whose population is ~82 million. Even Merkel’s sympathizers wondered about the country’s

    ability to accommodate that drove of new arrivals. Facing social disorder and political

    disruption, Germany eventually tightened its border controls to admit fewer asylum seekers,

    and Europe decided to pay Turkey to keep asylees penned there. Other once sympathetic

    European states turned away migrants who were unable to prove that they were fleeing war

    in Syria, Afghanistan, or Iraq, and instituted stricter screenings, balancing humane treatment

    with security of national borders. Thus, tens of thousands of emigrants are stranded in

    Greece, the Balkan countries, and now Turkey. The soldiers of Odin and Finnish Resistance

    vigilantes fashion a case study in escalating immigrant fear and suspicion; the Danish

    Parliament sanctioned seizures of cash and valuables from migrants, cut refugee benefits

    (even advertising the cuts in Lebanese newspapers), and prosecuted Danish Samaritans who

    aided asylum seekers; and Austria, Denmark, France, Germany, and Sweden imposed

    checks that placed the future of Europe’s open borders at risk as these nations seek ways to

    curb the flow of migrants.

    In South America, a liberal discourse of universally welcoming immigrants replaced

    formally restrictive immigration policies. But contrary to rights publically claimed in laws

    and policies, in practice South American governments reject increasing “irregular migration”

    from Africa, Asia, and the Caribbean to varying degrees. A “reverse immigration policy

    paradox” obtains in which South America de jure welcomes, but de facto obstructs,
    migrants. Ecuador and Brazil, for example, have immigration systems in which restrictive

    actions clash with liberal policy. In Argentina, policy is more consistent with a welcoming,

    liberal immigration discourse (Arcarazo & Freier, 2015). Meanwhile in the United States,

    between 2006 and 2013 children of immigrants accounted for all growth in the U.S.

    Bornstein Page 43

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    population of children under age 18. (The number of children of native-born parents fell 1.3

    million, while the number of children with at least one immigrant parent grew 1.9 million.)

    Paradoxically, on account of increasing life expectancies and decreasing birth rates in many

    developed countries, national economies as well as nonimmigrant senior citizens’ retirement

    pensions partly depend on the economic contributions of immigrants. In the 21st C,

    immigrants are expected to become a prominent force in the economies of these societies

    and contribute meaningfully to the care and support of their aging nonimmigrant populations

    (Hernandez, 2012). Immigrants in many countries will pay more in taxes over their lifetimes

    than they receive in social benefits (Dustmann, Frattini, & Preston, 2013). European

    reactions to this economic fact of life can be fierce: The Prime Minister of Hungary urged

    Europeans to have more children instead of welcoming refugees from South Asia, North

    Africa, and the Middle East.

    Pressures associated with globalization across multiple domains of language skills, work

    force, intercultural relations, refugee status, and so forth moderate national policy in cultures

    of destination. Within countries, immigrants may exert short-term downward pressures on

    wages among the lower more than middle- or upper-economic classes. Between countries,

    integration strategies obtain for valued immigrants and separation or assimilation strategies

    for less valued immigrants (Canada: Montreuil & Bourhis, 2001), where other countries

    show no differences in national strategies for different immigrant groups (France:

    Maisonneuve & Teste, 2007; the Netherlands: Van Oudenhoven et al., 1998).

    In brief, the Specificity Principle reveals that acculturation is moderated by the

    sociopolitical ideologies and policies of countries of destination (Baubock, Heller, &
    Zolberg, 1996; Bourhis, Moise, Perreault, & Senecal, 1997; Koenig, 1999).

  • Conclusions
  • Main effects tell us about the size and direction of differences; moderator effects tell us to

    what extent certain factors influence the size and direction of main effects. Acculturation is

    subject to multiple moderation. Therefore, acculturation theoreticians, researchers, and

    policy makers need to concern themselves with understanding which circumstances affect

    which aspects of which migrants when and how, and they should be interested to learn the

    ways in which migrants with which individual characteristics are affected, as well as the

    ways migrants affect their culture of origin, their own acculturation, and their culture of

    destination. The Specificity Principle provides a framework intended to explain trajectories

    of acculturation in individuals or groups who may or may not share significant

    characteristics.

    Typological assumptions in acculturation study have held that pathways of acculturation are

    categorical and universal: assimilation, integration, separation, or marginalization. The

    Specificity Principle offers a distinctively new, realistic, nuanced and more comprehensive

    replacement for that outmoded and increasingly irrelevant singular perspective. The

    Specificity Principle presents a pluralistic option that is sensitive to the many variations

    among present-day migrants and their situations. Thus, specificity assures greater precision

    Bornstein Page 44

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    with respect to acculturation and a more valid account of it. As war, oppression, extremism,

    climate change, and poverty show no signs of abating in the near future, and

    communications technology, ease of transportation, liberal ideology, and free trade continue

    to rise, the Specificity Principle, as acculturation science itself, will wax in significance.

    Some readers will take issue with the further fragmentation of acculturation (already

    instigated in typological systems). The Specificity Principle, as advanced here, is not

    intended to be blindly dogmatic or reductionist. Very likely, students of acculturation can

    arrive at many generalizations (some mentioned in the foregoing text): immigrants who

    know the language of the culture of destination, those with higher IQs or more open

    personalities, or those who are more motivated will all likely fare better through the

    acculturation process, where those with histories of misbehavior or conflicting religious

    orientations will fare worse. Since Heisenberg (1927), however, scientists have

    acknowledged that different individuals approach and understand the world in ways that

    reflect their unique interactions and experiences. To fathom acculturation, therefore, it is

    necessary and desirable to differentiate among specific setting conditions, people, times,

    processes, and domains. This is difficult to do and has been done in only piecemeal fashion

    to date. The unhappy truth is that not so much as we would like is currently known

    scientifically about the threads that are woven into the intricate fabric of acculturation or
    about the delicate weaving process itself. However, mounting evidence suggests that more

    sophisticated and sound approaches in a refreshed future acculturation science – motivated

    by the Specificity Principle — will help to expose, explain, and appreciate the refined
    tapestry that is acculturation. Eventually, acculturation science will have to embrace the

    Specificity Principle’s moderating terms if it is to move forward, as acculturation itself

    keeps the world in motion.

    Acknowledgments

    Supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NICHD.

  • References
  • Acevedo-Garcia D, Bates LM, Osypuk TL, McArdle N. The effect of immigrant generation and
    duration on self-rated health among US adults 2003–2007. Social Science and Medicine. 2010;
    71:1161–1172. DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.034 [PubMed: 20624666]

    Akhtar S. The immigrant, the exile, and the experience of nostalgia. Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic
    Studies. 1999; 1:123–130. DOI: 10.1023/A:1023029020496

    Alba RD, Logan JR. Variations on two themes—Racial and ethnic patterns in the attainment of
    suburban residence. Demography. 1991; 28:431–453. DOI: 10.2307/2061466 [PubMed: 1936377]

    American Psychological Association. Thesaurus of psychological index terms. 2013. Retrieved from
    http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=termfinder.displayTerms&id=082EA93B-9DE8-
    AA40-8271-8250B456440A

    Arcarazo DA, Freier LF. Turning the Immigration Policy Paradox Upside Down? Populist Liberalism
    and Discursive Gaps in South America. International Migration Review. 2015; 49:659–696. DOI:
    10.1111/imre.12146

    Arends-Tóth, J.; van de Vijver, FJR. Issues in the conceptualization and assessment of acculturation.
    In: Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LR., editors. Acculturation and parent-child relationships: Measurement
    and development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. p. 33-62.

    Bornstein Page 45

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=termfinder.displayTerms&id=082EA93B-9DE8-AA40-8271-8250B456440A

    http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=termfinder.displayTerms&id=082EA93B-9DE8-AA40-8271-8250B456440A

    Baer, W. The Puerto Rican economy and the United States economic fluctuations. Rio Piedras: Social
    Science Research Center, University of Puerto Rico; 1992.

    Bandura, A. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1977.

    Barni D, Leon E, Rosnati R, Palacios J. Behavioral and socioemotional adjustment in international
    adoptees: A comparison between Italian and Spanish adoptive parents’ reports. Adoption Quarterly.
    2008; 11:235–254. DOI: 10.1080/10926750802569780

    Bartlett, FC. Psychology and primitive culture. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press; 1970. Original work
    published 1923

    Barringer, H.; Gardner, RW.; Levin, MJ. Asians and Pacific Islanders in the United States. New York:
    Russel Sage Foundation; 1993.

    Basilio CD, Knight GP, O’Donnell M, Roosa MW, Gonzales NA, Umaña-Taylor AJ, Torres M. The
    Mexican American biculturalism scale: Bicultural comfort, facility, and advantages for adolescents
    and adults. Psychol Assess. 2014; 26(2):539–554. DOI: 10.1037/a0035951 [PubMed: 24548151]

    Baubock, R.; Heller, R.; Zolberg, A., editors. The challenge of diversity. Integration and pluralism in
    societies of immigration. Aldershot, UK: Avesbury; 1996.

    Beacco, JC.; Extramiana, C.; Krumm, HJ.; Simpson, BL.; Little, D.; Rossner, R.; … van Avermaet, P.
    The linguistic integration of adult migrants: From one country to another, from one language to
    another. Strasbourg: Council of Europe; 2014.

    Ben Sira, Z. Immigration, stress and readjustment. Preager; Westport, CT: 1997.

    Benet-Martinez V, Haritatos J. Bicultural identity integration (BII): Components and psychosocial
    antecedents. Journal of Personality. 2005; 73(4):1015–1050. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1467-6494.2005.00337.x [PubMed: 15958143]

    Benish-Weisman M, Levy S, Knafo A. Parents differentiate between their personal values and their
    socialization values: The role of adolescents’ values. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2013;
    23(4):614–620. DOI: 10.1111/jora.12058

    Berry, JW. Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. In: Frese, M., editor. Applied Psychology: An
    International Review. Vol. 46. 1997. p. 5-68.

    Berry, JW. Contexts of acculturation. In: Sam, DL.; Berry, JW., editors. Cambridge handbook of
    acculturation psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2006. p. 27-42.

    Berry, JW.; Phinney, JS.; Sam, DL.; Vedder, P., editors. Immigrant youth in cultural transition.
    Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006.

    Berthold, T. In erster linie kinder. 2014. Retrieved from http://www.unicef.de/blob/56282/
    fa13c2eefcd41dfca5d89d44c72e72e3/fluechtlingskinder-in-deutschland-unicef-studie-2014-
    data

    Birman D, Persky I, Chan YY. Multiple identities of Jewish immigrant adolescents from the former
    Soviet Union: An exploration of salience and impact of ethnic identity. International Journal of
    Behavioral Development. 2010; 34(3):193–205. DOI: 10.1177/0165025409350948

    Block, D. Census says African, Hispanic immigrants flocking to Bronx boro. New York Daily News.
    2009. Retrieved from http://www.nydailynews.com/ny_local/bronx/
    2009/02/03/2009-02-03_census_says_african_hispanic_immigrants_.html

    Boehm, DA. Intimate migrations: Gender, family, and illegality among transnational Mexicans. New
    York: NYU Press; 2012.

    Boneva BS, Hanson Frieze I. Toward a concept of a migrant personality. Journal of Social Issues.
    2001; 57:477–491. DOI: 10.1111/0022-4537.00224

    Borjas GJ. Self-selection and the earnings of immigrants. American Economic Review. 1987; 77:531–
    553. DOI: 10.3386/w2248

    Born DO. Psychological adaptation and development under acculturative stress. Social Science and
    Medicine. 1970; 3:529–547. [PubMed: 5483545]

    Bornstein, MH., editor. The Handbook of Cultural Developmental Science. Part 1. Domains of
    Development across Cultures. Part 2. Development in Different Places on Earth. New York:
    Psychology Press; 2010.

    Bornstein MH. Parenting and child mental health: A cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry.
    2013; 12(3):258–265. DOI: 10.1002/wps.20071 [PubMed: 24096792]

    Bornstein Page 46

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.unicef.de/blob/56282/fa13c2eefcd41dfca5d89d44c72e72e3/fluechtlingskinder-in-deutschland-unicef-studie-2014-data

    http://www.unicef.de/blob/56282/fa13c2eefcd41dfca5d89d44c72e72e3/fluechtlingskinder-in-deutschland-unicef-studie-2014-data

    http://www.unicef.de/blob/56282/fa13c2eefcd41dfca5d89d44c72e72e3/fluechtlingskinder-in-deutschland-unicef-studie-2014-data

    http://www.nydailynews.com/ny_local/bronx/2009/02/03/2009-02-03_census_says_african_hispanic_immigrants_.html

    http://www.nydailynews.com/ny_local/bronx/2009/02/03/2009-02-03_census_says_african_hispanic_immigrants_.html

    Bornstein MH, Cote LR. Mother-infant interaction and acculturation I: Behavioral comparisons in
    Japanese American and South American families. International Journal of Behavioral
    Development. 2001; 25:549–563. DOI: 10.1080/01650250042000546

    Bornstein MH, Cote LR. Mothers-parenting cognitions in cultures of origin, acculturating cultures, and
    cultures of destination. Child Development. 2004; 75:221–235. [PubMed: 15015686]

    Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LR. Parenting cognitions and practices in the acculturative process. In:
    Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LR., editors. Acculturation and parent-child relationships: Measurement
    and development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. p. 173-196.

    Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LC. Knowledge of child development and family interactions among
    immigrants to America: Perspectives from developmental science. In: Lansford, J.; Deater-
    Deckard, K.; Bornstein, MH., editors. Immigrant families in contemporary society. New York:
    Guilford; 2007. p. 121-136.

    Bornstein MH, Cote LR, Haynes OM, Suwalsky JTD, Bakeman R. Modalities of infant-mother
    interaction in Japanese, Japanese American immigrant, and European American dyads. Child
    Development. 2012; 83:2073–2088. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01822.x [PubMed:
    22860874]

    Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Haynes OM, Belsky J, Azuma H, Kwak K, …de Galperin CZ. Maternal
    personality and parenting cognitions in cross-cultural perspective. International Journal of
    Behavioral Development. 2007; 31:193–209. DOI: 10.1177/0165025407074632

    Bornstein MH, Jager J, Putnick DL. Sampling in developmental science: Situations, shortcomings,
    solutions, and standards. Developmental Review. 2013; 33:357–370. DOI: 10.1016/j.dr.
    2013.08.003 [PubMed: 25580049]

    Bornstein, MH.; Jager, J.; Steinberg, LD. Adolescents, parents, friends/peers: A relationships model
    (with commentary and illustrations). In: Weiner, I., editor; Lerner, RM.; Easterbrooks, MA.;
    Mistry, J., editors. Handbook of Psychology. 2. Vol. 6. New York: Wiley; 2012. p.
    393-433.Developmental Psychology

    Bornstein, MH.; Mortimer, JT.; Lutfey, K.; Bradley, R. Theories and processes of life-span
    socialization. In: Fingerman, K.; Berg, C.; Antonucci, T.; Smith, J., editors. Handbook of lifespan
    development. New York: Springer; 2011. p. 27-55.

    Borrell LN, Crawford ND. All-cause mortality among Hispanics in the United States: Exploring
    heterogeneity by nativity status, country of origin, and race in the National Health Interview
    Survey-linked mortality files. Annals of Epidemiology. 2009; 19:336–343. DOI: 10.1016/
    j.annepidem.2008.12.003 [PubMed: 19230710]

    Bourhis RY, Moise LC, Perreault S, Senecal S. Towards an interactive acculturation model: A social
    psychological approach. International Journal of Psychology. 1997; 32:369–386. DOI:
    10.1080/002075997400629

    Bradley RH, Pennar A, Glick J. Home environments of infants from immigrant families in the United
    States: Findings from the new immigrant survey. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2014; 35(6):565–
    579. DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21477 [PubMed: 25798506]

    Bridges K, Hoff E. Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of
    toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2014; 35:225–241. DOI: 10.1017/
    S0142716412000379 [PubMed: 25346557]

    Bronfenbrenner, U.; Morris, PA. The bioecological model of human development. In: Damon, W.;
    Lerner, RM., editors. Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human
    development. 6. Vol. 793. New York: John Wiley; 2006. p. 828

    Brüß J. Experiences of discrimination reported by Turkish, Moroccan, and Bangladeshi Muslims in
    three European cities. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies. 2008; 34:875–894. DOI:
    10.1080/13691830802211166

    Bundesamt für Migration und Flüchtlinge. Aktuelle Zahlen zu Asyl. 2015. Retrieved from http://
    www.bamf.de/SharedDocs/Anlagen/DE/Downloads/Infothek/Statistik/Asyl/statistik-anlage-teil-4-
    aktuelle-zahlen-zu-asyl ?__blob=publicationFile

    Buriel R, Perez W, DeMent TL, Chavez DV, Moran VR. The relationship of language brokering to
    academic performance, biculturalism, and self-efficacy among Latino adolescents. Hispanic
    Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 1998; 20(3):283–297. DOI: 10.1177/07399863980203001

    Bornstein Page 47

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.bamf.de/SharedDocs/Anlagen/DE/Downloads/Infothek/Statistik/Asyl/statistik-anlage-teil-4-aktuelle-zahlen-zu-asyl ?__blob=publicationFile

    http://www.bamf.de/SharedDocs/Anlagen/DE/Downloads/Infothek/Statistik/Asyl/statistik-anlage-teil-4-aktuelle-zahlen-zu-asyl ?__blob=publicationFile

    http://www.bamf.de/SharedDocs/Anlagen/DE/Downloads/Infothek/Statistik/Asyl/statistik-anlage-teil-4-aktuelle-zahlen-zu-asyl ?__blob=publicationFile

    Byrne, DE. The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press; 1971.

    Byrne, O.; Miller, E. The Flow of Unaccompanied Children Through the Immigration System: A
    Resource for Practitioners, Policy Makers, and Researchers. 2012. Retrieved from http://
    www.vera.org/sites/default/files/resources/downloads/the-flow-of-unaccompanied-children-
    through-the-immigration-system

    Cabrera, N.; Leyendecker, B., editors. Positive development of minority children. The Netherlands:
    Springer; 2016.

    Cabrera NJ, Shannon JD, West J, Brooks-Gunn J. Parental interactions with Latino infants: Variation
    by country of origin and English proficiency. Child Development. 2006; 77:1190–1207. DOI:
    10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00928.x [PubMed: 16999792]

    Caligiuri PM. Selecting expatriates for personality characteristics: a moderating effect of personality
    on the relationship between host national contact and cross-cultural adjustment. Management
    International Review. 2000; 40:61–80.

    Cann RL, Stoneking M, Wilson AC. Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature. 1987; 325:31–
    36. DOI: 10.1038/325031a0 [PubMed: 3025745]

    Capps RC, Bronte-Tinkew J, Horowitz A. Acculturation and father engagement with infants among
    Chinese- and Mexican-origin immigrant fathers. Fathering. 2010; 8(1):61–92. DOI: 10.3149/fth.
    0801.61

    Carhill A, Suárez-Orozco C, Páez M. Explaining English language proficiency among adolescent
    immigrant students. American Educational Research Journal. 2008; 45:1155–1179. DOI:
    10.3102/0002831208321443

    Carver CS, Scheier MF, Segerstrom SC. Optimism. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010; 30:879–889.
    DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.006 [PubMed: 20170998]

    Caudill, W.; Frost, L. A comparison of maternal care and infant behavior in Japanese-American,
    American, and Japanese Families. In: Bronfenbrenner, U., editor. Influences on human
    development. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden; 1972. p. 329-342.

    Caudill, W.; Frost, LA. A comparison of maternal care and infant behavior in Japanese-American,
    American, and Japanese families. In: Lebra, WP., editor. Youth, socialization, and mental health.
    Vol. 3. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii; 1974. p. 3-15.

    Cavazos-Rehg PA, Zayas LH, Spitznagel EL. Legal status, emotional well-being and subjective health
    status of Latino immigrants. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2007; 99:1126–1131.
    [PubMed: 17987916]

    Chao, RK. The prevalence and consequences of adolescents’ language brokering for their immigrant
    parents. In: Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LR., editors. Acculturation and parent-child relationships:
    Measurement and development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. p. 271-296.

    Chao RK, Otsuki-Clutter M. Racial and Ethnic Differences: Sociocultural and Contextual
    Explanations. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011; 21:47–60. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1532-7795.2010.00714.x

    Cheah CSL, Leung CYY, Zhou N. Understanding “tiger parenting” through the perceptions of Chinese
    immigrant mothers: Can Chinese and U. S. parenting Coexist? Asian American Journal of
    Psychology. 2013; 4:30–40. DOI: 10.1037/a0031217 [PubMed: 23914284]

    Chen SX, Benet-Martínez V, Bond MH. Bicultural identity, bilingualism, and psychological
    adjustment in multicultural societies: Immigration-based and globalization-based acculturation.
    Journal of Personality. 2008; 76:803–837. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00505.x [PubMed:
    18482355]

    Chen SH, Hua M, Zhou Q, Tao A, Lee EH, Ly J, Main A. Parent-child cultural orientations and child
    adjustment in Chinese American immigrant families. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50(1):
    189–201. DOI: 10.1037/a0032473 [PubMed: 23566081]

    Cheryan S, Monin B. Where are you really from?: Asian Americans and identity denial. Journal of
    Personality and Social Psychology. 2005; 89(5):717–730. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.717
    [PubMed: 16351364]

    Chiswick BR, Miller PW. Do enclaves matter in immigrant adjustment? City & Community. 2005;
    4:5–35. DOI: 10.1111/j.1535-6841.2005.00101.x

    Bornstein Page 48

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.vera.org/sites/default/files/resources/downloads/the-flow-of-unaccompanied-children-through-the-immigration-system

    http://www.vera.org/sites/default/files/resources/downloads/the-flow-of-unaccompanied-children-through-the-immigration-system

    http://www.vera.org/sites/default/files/resources/downloads/the-flow-of-unaccompanied-children-through-the-immigration-system

    Chiu LH. Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Chinese-American, and Anglo-American mothers.
    International Journal of Psychology. 1987; 22(4):409–419. DOI: 10.1080/00207598708246782

    Chiu C, Morris MW, Hong Y, Menon T. Motivated cultural cognition: the impact of cultural theories
    on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and
    Social Psychology. 2000; 78:247–259. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.247 [PubMed: 10707332]

    Chuang, CS.; Tamis-LeMonda, CS., editors. Gender roles in immigrant families. New York: Springer;
    2013.

    Coatsworth JD, Pantin H, McBride C, Briones E, Kurtines W, Szapocznik J. Ecodevelopmental
    correlates of behavior problems in young Hispanic females. Applied Developmental Science.
    2002; 6(3):126–143. DOI: 10.1207/s1532480xads0603_3

    Cole M. Internationalism in psychology: We need it now more than ever. American Psychologist.
    2006; 61(8):904–917. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.61.8.904 [PubMed: 17115849]

    Coleman, DL. The role of the law in relationships within immigrant families. In: Lansford, JE.;
    Deater-Deckard, K.; Bornstein, MH., editors. Immigrant families in contemporary society. New
    York, NY: Guildford Press; 2007. p. 287-303.

    Coll, CTG.; Marks, AK. Immigrant stories: Ethnicity and academics in middle childhood. New York,
    NY: Oxford University Press; 2009.

    Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human
    development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007; 58:175–199. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.
    58.110405.085551 [PubMed: 16903807]

    Cornelius, W. Ambivalent reception: Mass public responses to the “new” Latino immigration to the
    United States. In: Suárez-Orozco, MM.; Páez, MM., editors. Latinos: Remaking America.
    Berkeley: University of California Press; 2002. p. 165-189.

    Corona R, Stevens LF, Halfond RW, Shaffer CM, Reid-Quinones K, Tanya G. A Qualitative Analysis
    of What Latino Parents and Adolescents Think and Feel About Language Brokering. Journal of
    Child and Family Studies. 2012; 21:788–798. DOI: 10.1007/s10826-011-9536-2

    Costigan CL, Dokis DP. Relations between parent-child acculturation differences and adjustment
    within immigrant Chinese families. Child Development. 2006; 77(5):1252–1267. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1467-8624.2006.00932.x [PubMed: 16999796]

    Cote LR, Bornstein MH. Mother-infant interaction and acculturation II: Behavioral coherence and
    correspondence in Japanese American and South American families. International Journal of
    Behavioral Development. 2001; 25:564–576. DOI: 10.1080/01650250042000555

    Critelli F. The impact of September 11th on immigrants in the United States. Journal of Immigrant and
    Refugee Studies. 2008; 6(2):141–167. DOI: 10.1080/15362940802198793

    Crouter AC, Davis KD, Updegraff K, Delgado M, Fortner M. Mexican American fathers’ occupational
    conditions: Links to family members’ psychological adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family.
    2006; 68:843–858. DOI: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00299.x [PubMed: 18414596]

    Daglar M, Melhuish E, Barnes J. Parenting and preschool child behavior among Turkish immigrant,
    migrant and non-migrant families. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2011; 8(3):
    261–279. DOI: 10.1080/17405621003710827

    Davey, A. Learning to be prejudiced. London, England: Edward Arnold; 1983.

    De Houwer, A. The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge
    University Press; 1990.

    De Leersnyder J, Mesquita B, Kim H. Where do my emotions belong? A study of immigrants’
    emotional acculturation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2011; 37:451–463. DOI:
    10.1177/0146167211399103 [PubMed: 21357754]

    Denton NA, Massey DS. Racial identity among Caribbean Hispanics: The effect of double minority
    status on residential segregation. American Sociological Review. 1989; 54:790–808. DOI:
    10.2307/2117754

    Derluyn I, Broekaert E, Schuyten G. Emotional and behavioural problems in migrant adolescents in
    Belgium. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008; 17:54–62. DOI: 10.1007/
    s00787-007-0636-x [PubMed: 17846817]

    Devine P. Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of
    Personality and Social Psychology. 1989; 56:5–18. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5

    Bornstein Page 49

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Dimitrova, R.; Chasiotis, A. Are immigrant children in Italy better adjusted than mainstream Italian
    children?. In: Garcia-Coll, C., editor. The impact of immigration on children’s development.
    Basel: Karger; 2012. p. 35-48.

    Dionne, EJ, Jr. Germany’s political crisis. The Washington Post. 2015 Dec 2. Retrieved from https://
    www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/merkels-yes-we-can-blues/2015/12/02/3d585d26-9920-11e5-
    b499-76cbec161973_story.html

    Donato KM, Gabaccia D, Holdaway J, Manalansan M, Pessar PR. A glass half full? Gender in
    migration studies. International Migration Review. 2006; 40:3–26. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1747-7379.2006.00001.x

    Dunham Y, Chen EE, Banaji MR. Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental
    invariance and early enculturation. Psychological Science. 2013; 24:860–868. DOI:
    10.1177/0956797612463081 [PubMed: 23558550]

    Dustmann C, Frattini T, Preston IP. The effect of immigration along the distribution of wages. The
    Review of Economic Studies. 2013; 80(1):145–173.

    Dykstra PA, Fokkema T. Norms of filial obligation in the Netherlands. Population (English edition).
    2012; 67(1):97–122. DOI: 10.3917/popu.1201.0103

    Dykstra, PA.; van den Broek, t; Muresan, C.; Haragus, M.; Haragus, P-T.; Abramowska-Kmon, A.;
    Kotowska, IE. State-of-the-art report: Intergenerational linkages in families. Families and Societies
    Working Paper Series, Changing Families and Sustainable Societies: Policy Contexts and Diversity
    over the Life Course and across Generations. 2013. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/
    1765/50356

    Eng S, Kanitkar K, Cleveland HH, Herbert R, Fischer J, Wiersma JD. School achievement differences
    among Chinese and Filipino American students: Acculturation and the family. Educational
    Psychology. 2008; 28:535–550. DOI: 10.1080/01443410701861308

    Epstein A, Levin R. The impact of Jewish identity on the psychological adjustment of Soviet Jewish
    immigrants to Israel. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences. 1996; 33:21–31. [PubMed:
    8766430]

    Esposito, AG.; Sirkin, RE.; Bauer, PJ. Bilingual education. In: Bornstein, MH.; Arterberry, ME.;
    Fingerman, KL.; Lansford, JE., editors. The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development.
    Los Angeles, CA: SAGE; 2017. p. xx-xx.

    Evans AB, Banerjee M, Meyer R, Aldana A, Foust M, Rowley S. Racial socialization as a mechanism
    for positive development among African American youth. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;
    6(3):251–257. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00226.x

    Farmer AE, McGuffin P. Humiliation, loss and other types of life events and difficulties: A comparison
    of depressed subjects, healthy controls and their siblings. Psychological Medicine. 2003; 33:1169–
    1175. DOI: 10.1017/s0033291703008419 [PubMed: 14580071]

    Farver JM, Xu Y, Bhadha BR, Narang S, Lieber E. Ethnic identity, acculturation, parenting beliefs, and
    adolescent adjustment: A comparison of Asian Indian and European American families. Merrill-
    Palmer Quarterly. 2007; 53(2):184–215. DOI: 10.1353/mpq.2007.0010

    Farver JAM, Xu Y, Lonigan CJ, Eppe S. The home literacy environment and Latino head start
    children’s emergent literacy skills. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49:775–791. [PubMed:
    22662767]

    Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. Children of at least one foreign-born
    parent: Percentage of children ages 0–17 by nativity of child and parents, parent’s education,
    poverty status, and other characteristics, selected years 1994–2014. 2011. Retrieved from http://
    www.childstats.gov/americaschildren/tables.asp

    Ferguson GM, Bornstein MH. Remote acculturation: The “Americanizaton” of Jamaican Islanders.
    International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2012; 36:167–177. DOI:
    10.1177/0165025412437066

    Ferguson G, Bornstein MH, Pottinger AM. Tridimensional acculturation and adaptation among
    Jamaican adolescent-mother dyads in the United States. Child Development. 2012; 83:1486–
    1493. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01787.x [PubMed: 22966917]

    Fishman, JA., editor. Language loyalty in the United States. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton
    Publishers; 1966.

    Bornstein Page 50

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    https://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/merkels-yes-we-can-blues/2015/12/02/3d585d26-9920-11e5-b499-76cbec161973_story.html

    https://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/merkels-yes-we-can-blues/2015/12/02/3d585d26-9920-11e5-b499-76cbec161973_story.html

    https://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/merkels-yes-we-can-blues/2015/12/02/3d585d26-9920-11e5-b499-76cbec161973_story.html

    http://hdl.handle.net/1765/50356

    http://hdl.handle.net/1765/50356

    http://www.childstats.gov/americaschildren/tables.asp

    http://www.childstats.gov/americaschildren/tables.asp

    Flink IJE, Jansen PW, Beirens TMJ, Tiemeier H, van IJzendoorn MH, Jaddoe VWV, … Raat H.
    Differences in problem behavior among ethnic minority and majority preschoolers in the
    Netherlands and the role of family functioning and parenting factors as mediators: The
    Generation R Study. BMC Public Health. 2012; 12:1092.doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1092
    [PubMed: 23253397]

    Foner N. Then and now or then to now: Immigration to New York in contemporary and historical
    perspective. Journal of American Ethnic History. 2006; 25:33–47.

    Foner N, Alba R. Immigrant religion in the U. S. and Western Europe: Bridge or barrier to inclusion?
    International Management Review. 2008; 42:360–392. DOI: 10.1111/j.1747-7379.2008.00128.x

    Franzini L, Fernandez-Esquer ME. Socioeconomic, cultural, and personal influences on health
    outcomes in low-income Mexican-origin individuals in Texas. Social Science and Medicine.
    2004; 59:1629–1646. DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.02.014 [PubMed: 15279921]

    Fernandez-Kelly, P.; Schauffler, R. Divided fates: Immigrant children and the new assimilation. In:
    Portes, A., editor. The new second generation. New York: Russel Sage Foundation; 1996. p.
    30-53.

    Fredricks JA, Simpkins SD. Promoting Positive Youth Development Through Organized After-School
    Activities: Taking a Closer Look at Participation of Ethnic Minority Youth. Child Development
    Perspectives. 2012; 6:280–287. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00206.x

    Fuligni AJ, Witkow M. The postsecondary educational progress of youth from immigrant families.
    Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2004; 14(2):159–183. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1532-7795.2004.01402002.x

    Gaertner, SL.; Dovidio, JF. Reducing intergroup bias: The common in-group identity model.
    Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press; 2000.

    Galster G, Metzger K, Waite R. Neighborhood opportunity structures of immigrant populations, 1980
    and 1990. Housing Policy Debate. 1999; 10:395–442. DOI: 10.1080/10511482.1999.9521337

    Gaytan FX, Carhill A, Suárez-Orozco C. Understanding and responding to the needs of newcomer
    immigrant youth and families. The Prevention Researcher. 2007; 14:10–13. DOI: 10.1037/
    e717692007-003

    Gewertz C. Asian student’s need overlooked in NYC, advocacy group says. Education Week. 2004;
    23(42):15.

    Gfroerer JC, Tan LL. Substance use among foreign-born youths in the United States: Does the length
    of residence matter? American Journal of Public Health. 2003; 93:1892–1895. DOI: 10.2105/
    ajph.93.11.1892 [PubMed: 14600061]

    Gibson MA. Immigrant adaptation and patterns of acculturation. Human Development. 2001; 44:19–
    23. DOI: 10.1159/000057037

    Gieling M, Thijs J, Verkuyten M. Voluntary and involuntary immigrants and adolescents’ endorsement
    of multiculturalism. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 2011; 35:259–267. DOI:
    10.1016/j.ijintrel.2010.09.003

    Gijsberts, M.; Huijnk, W.; Vogels, R., editors. Chinese Nederlanders: Van horeca naar hogeschool.
    Hague, the Netherlands: Social and Cultural Planning Office; 2011.

    Ginwright SA. Peace out to revolution! Activism among African American youth: An argument for
    radical healing. Young. 2010; 18:77–96. DOI: 10.1177/110330880901800106

    Goldenberg, C.; Rueda, RS.; August, D. Sociocultural influences on the literacy attainment of
    language-minority children and youth. In: August, D.; Shanahan, T., editors. Developing literacy
    in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority
    Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2006. p. 269-318.

    Gonzales RG, Chavez LR. Awakening to a nightmare: Abjectivity and illegality in the lives of
    undocumented 1.5-generation Latino immigrants to the United States. Current Anthropology.
    2012; 53(3):255–281. DOI: 10.1086/665414

    Gratier M. Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: Cultural
    similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cognitive Development. 2003;
    18:533–554. DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.009

    Bornstein Page 51

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Guan SA, Greenfield PM, Orellana MF. Translating into understanding: Language brokering and
    prosocial development in emerging adults from immigrant families. Journal of Adolescent
    Research. 2014; 29(3):331–355. DOI: 10.1177/0743558413520223

    Guendelman MD, Cheryan S, Monin B. Fitting In but Getting Fat Identity Threat and Dietary Choices
    Among US Immigrant Groups. Psychological science. 2011; 22(7):959–967. DOI:
    10.1177/0956797611411585 [PubMed: 21653909]

    Güngör D, Bornstein MH. Gender, development, values, adaptation, and discrimination in
    acculturating adolescents: The case of Turk heritage youth born and living in Belgium. Sex
    Roles. 2009; 60:537–548. DOI: 10.1007/s11199-008-9531-2

    Güngör, D.; Bornstein, MH. Gender and developmental pathways of acculturation and adaptation in
    immigrant adolescents. In: Chuang, SS.; Tamis-LeMonda, CS., editors. Gender roles in
    immigrant families. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 177-190.

    Güngör D, Bornstein MH, Phalet K. Religiosity, values, and acculturation: A study of Turkish,
    Turkish-Belgian, and Belgian adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development.
    2012; 36:367–373. DOI: 10.1177/0165025412448357 [PubMed: 23155300]

    Güngör D, Bornstein MH, De Leersnyder J, Cote LR, Ceulemans E, Mesquita B. Acculturation of
    personality: A three-culture study of Japanese, Japanese Americans, and European Americans.
    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2013; 44:701–718. DOI: 10.1177/0022022112470749
    [PubMed: 23935211]

    Hagendoorn L. Intergroup biases in multiple group systems: The perception of ethnic hierarchies.
    European Review of Social Psychology. 1995; 6:199–228. DOI: 10.1080/14792779443000058

    Hahm HC, Lahiff M, Guterman NB. Asian American adolescents’ acculturation, binge drinking, and
    alcohol- and tobacco-using peers. Journal of Community Psychology. 2004; 32:295–308. DOI:
    10.1002/jcop.20002

    Hajat, A.; Lucas, JB.; Kingston, R. Health outcomes among Hispanic subgroups: Data from the
    National Health Interview Survey, 1992–1995. Advance data from vital and health statistics 310.
    Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 2000.

    Hall, GS. Adolescence. Vol. 2. New York: Appleton; 1904.

    Hall M, Greenman E. The occupational cost of being illegal in the United States: Legal status, job
    hazards, and compensating differentials. International Migration Review. 2015; 49(2):406–442.
    DOI: 10.1111/imre.12090

    Hamilton, SF.; Hamilton, MA. The transition to adulthood: Challenges of poverty and structural lag.
    In: Lerner, RM.; Steinberg, L., editors. Handbook of adolescent psychology, Vol 2: Contextual
    influences on adolescent development. 3. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2009. p. 492-526.

    Han WJ. The academic trajectories of children of immigrants and their school environments.
    Developmental Psychology. 2008; 44:1572–1590. [PubMed: 18999323]

    Han WJ, Huang CC. The forgotten treasure: Bilingualism and Asian children’s emotional and
    behavioral health. American Journal of Public Health. 2010; 100:831–838. [PubMed: 20299654]

    Harker K. Immigrant generation, assimilation, and adolescent psychological well-being. Social Forces.
    2001; 79:969–1004. DOI: 10.1353/sof.2001.0010

    Hartmann D, Gerteis J. Dealing with diversity: Mapping multiculturalism in sociological terms.
    Sociological Theory. 2005; 23:218–240. DOI: 10.1111/j.0735-2751.2005.00251.x

    Heine, SJ.; Lehman, DR. Move the body, change the self: Acculturative effects on the self-concept. In:
    Schaller, M.; Crandall, C., editors. Psychological Foundations of Culture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum;
    2004. p. 305-331.

    Heisenberg W. Über den anschaulichen Inhalt der quantentheoretischen Kinematik und Mechanik.
    Zeitschrift für Physik. 1927; 43:172–198. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-61659-4_30

    Helbling M. Validating integration and citizenship policy indices. Comparative European Politics.
    2013; 11:555–576. DOI: 10.1057/cep.2013.11

    Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
    2010; 33:1–75. DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x0999152x [PubMed: 20377929]

    Henry CS, Merten MJ, Plunkett SW, Sands T. Neighborhood, parenting, and adolescent factors and
    academic achievement in Latino adolescents from immigrant families. Family Relations. 2008;
    57(5):579–590. DOI: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2008.00524.x

    Bornstein Page 52

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Hernandez, DJ. Jacobs Foundation. Resources, strenghts, and challenges for children in immigrant
    families in eight affluent countries. In: Masten, AS.; Liebkind, K.; Hernandez, DJ., editors.
    Realizing the potential of immigrant youth. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2012. p.
    17-40.

    Hernández, D.; Napierala, JS. Children in immigrant families: Essential to America’s future. Child and
    Youth Well-Being Index Policy Brief: Foundation for Child Development. 2012. Retrieved from
    http://fcd-us.org/resources/children-immigrant-families-essential-americas-future?
    destination=resources%2Fsearch%3Ftopic%3D0%26authors%3DHernandez%26kkeyword%3D

    Hill NE, Bush KR, Roosa MW. Relations between parenting and family socialization strategies and
    children’s mental health: Low income, Mexican American and Euro-American mothers’ and
    children’s perspectives. Child Development. 2003; 74:189–204. DOI:
    10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00530 [PubMed: 12625445]

    Hill NE, Torres K. Negotiating the American dream: The paradox of aspirations and achievement
    among Latino students and engagement between their families and schools. Journal of Social
    Issues. 2010; 66:95–112. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01635.x

    Hinton WL, Tiet Q, Giaouyen T, Chensney M. Predictors of depression among refugees from Vietnam:
    A longitudinal study of new arrivals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1997; 185:39–45.
    DOI: 10.1097/00005053-199701000-00007 [PubMed: 9040532]

    Hoeffel, E.; Rastogi, S.; Kim, MO.; Shahid, H. The Asian population: 2010. 2012. Retrieved from
    http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-11

    Hojat M, Shapurian R, Foroughi D, Nayerahmadi H, Farzaneh M, Shafieyan M, Parsi M. Gender
    differences in traditional attitudes toward marriage and the family. Journal of Family Issues.
    2000; 21(4):419–434. DOI: 10.1177/019251300021004001

    Hölling H, Erhart M, Ravens-Sieberer U, Schlack R. Behavioural problems in children and
    adolescents. First results from the German Health Interview and Examination Survey for
    Children and Adolescents (KiGGS): Erste Ergebnisse aus dem Kinder und
    Jugendgesundheitssurvey (KiGGS). Bundesgesundheitsblatt – Gesundheitsforschung –
    Gesundheitsschutz. 2007; 50(5–6):784–793. DOI: 10.1007/s00103-007-0241-7 [PubMed:
    17514464]

    Holroyd EA, Molassiotis A, Taylor-Pilliae RE. Filipino domestic workers in Hong Kong: Health
    related behaviors, health locus of control and social support. Women and Health. 2001; 33:181–
    205. DOI: 10.1300/j013v33n01_11 [PubMed: 11523638]

    Hondagneu-Sotelo, P. Doméstica: Immigrant Workers Cleaning and Caring in the Shadows of
    Affluence. Berkeley: University of California Press; 2001.

    HoSang DW. Family and community as the cornerstone of civic engagement: Immigrant and youth
    organizing in the southwest. National Civic Review. 2006; 95:58–61. DOI: 10.1002/ncr.160

    Howes C, Obregon NB. Emotional availability in Mexican-heritage low-income mothers and children:
    Infancy through preschool. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2009; 9:260–276. DOI:
    10.1080/15295190902844589

    Hua JM, Costigan CL. The familial context of adolescent language brokering within immigrant
    Chinese families in Canada. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2012; 41:894–906. DOI:
    10.1007/s10964-011-9682-2 [PubMed: 21681583]

    Huddleston, T.; Niessen, J. Migrant Integration Policy Index III. Brussels, Belgium: British Council
    and Migration Policy Group; 2011.

    Huddleston, T.; Niessen, J.; Chaoimh, EN.; White, E. Migrant Integration Policy Index III. Brussels,
    Belgium: British Council and Migration Policy Group; 2011.

    Hughes D, Rodriguez J, Smith EP, Johnson DJ, Stevenson HC, Spicer P. Parents’ ethnic-racial
    socialization practices: A review of research and directions for future study. Developmental
    Psychology. 2006; 42(5):747–770. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.747 [PubMed: 16953684]

    Huh J, Prause JA, Dooley CD. The impact of nativity on chronic diseases, self-rated health, and
    comorbidity status of Asian and Hispanic immigrants. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health.
    2008; 10:103–118. DOI: 10.1007/s10903-007-9065-7 [PubMed: 17546500]

    Humes, K.; Jones, NA.; Ramirez, RR. Overview of Race and Hispanic Origin: 2010. 2011. Retrieved
    from http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-02

    Bornstein Page 53

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://fcd-us.org/resources/children-immigrant-families-essential-americas-future?destination=resources%2Fsearch%3Ftopic%3D0%26authors%3DHernandez%26kkeyword%3D

    http://fcd-us.org/resources/children-immigrant-families-essential-americas-future?destination=resources%2Fsearch%3Ftopic%3D0%26authors%3DHernandez%26kkeyword%3D

    http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-11

    http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-02

    Hunt LM, Schneider S, Comer B. Should “acculturation” be a variable in health research? A critical
    review of research on U.S. Hispanics. Social Science and Medicine. 2004; 59:973–986. DOI:
    10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.12.009 [PubMed: 15186898]

    Hurtado A, Vega LA. Shift happens: Spanish and English transmission between parents and their
    children. Journal of Social Issues. 2004; 60(1):137–155. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    0022-4537.2004.00103.x

    Iceland J, Scopilliti M. Immigrant residential segregation in US metropolitan areas, 1990–2000.
    Demography. 2008; 45(1):79–94. DOI: 10.1353/dem.2008.0009 [PubMed: 18390292]

    Idema H, Phalet K. Transmission of gender-role values in Turkish-German migrant families: The role
    of gender, intergenerational and intercultural relations. Zeitschrift für Familienforschung. 2007;
    19:71–105.

    Ispa JM, Csizmadia A, Rudy D, Fine MA, Krull JL, Bradley RH, Cabrera N. Patterns of maternal
    directiveness by ethnicity among early Head Start research participants. Parenting: Science and
    Practice. 2013; 13:58–75. DOI: 10.1080/15295192.2013.732439

    Jain A, Belsky J. Fathering and acculturation: Immigrant Indian families with young children. Journal
    of Marriage and the Family. 1997; 59:873–883. DOI: 10.2307/353789

    Jensen, LA.; Arnett, JJ.; McKenzie, J. Globalization and cultural identity developments in adolescence
    and emerging adulthood. In: Schwartz, SJ.; Luyckx, K.; Vignoles, VL., editors. Handbook of
    identity theory and research. New York: Springer Publishing Company; 2011. p. 285-301.

    Jia G, Aaronson D. A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the
    United States. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2003; 24(01):131–161. DOI: 10.1017/
    s0142716403000079

    Jin L, Underhill PA, Doctor V, Davis RW, Shen P, Cavalli-Sforza LL, Oefner PJ. Distribution of
    haplotypes from a chromosome 21 region distinguishes multiple prehistoric human migrations.
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 1999; 96:3796–3800. DOI: 10.1073/
    pnas.96.7.3796

    Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH. Behavior problems and mental health referrals of international adoptees.
    Journal of the American Medical Association. 2005; 293:2501–2515. DOI: 10.1001/jama.
    293.20.2501 [PubMed: 15914751]

    Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH. Adoptees do not lack self-esteem: A meta-analysis of studies on self-
    esteem of transracial, international, and domestic adoptees. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;
    133:1067–1083. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1067 [PubMed: 17967094]

    Kao, G. Psychological well-being and educational achievement among immigrant youth. In:
    Hernandez, DJ., editor. Children of immigrants: Health, adjustment, and public assistance.
    Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1999. p. 410-477.

    Kao G, Tienda M. Optimism and achievement: The educational performance of immigrant youth.
    Social Science Quarterly. 1995; 76(1):1–19.

    Kelly, MRF.; Schauffler, R. Divided fates: Immigrant children and the new assimilation. In: Portes, A.,
    editor. The new second generation. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1996. p. 30-53.

    Kennedy, JF. A nation of immigrants. New York: Harper and Row; 1964.

    Kidder LH. Requirements for being “Japanese”: Stories of returnees. International Journal of
    lntercultural Relations. 1992; 16:383–394. DOI: 10.1016/0147-1767(92)90045-v

    Kieffer EC, Martin JA, Herman WH. Impact of maternal nativity on the prevalence of diabetes during
    pregnancy among U.S. ethnic groups. Diabetes Care. 1999; 22:729–735. DOI: 10.2337/diacare.
    22.5.729 [PubMed: 10332673]

    Killen, M.; Rutland, A. Children and social exclusion: Morality, prejudice, and group identity. Oxford,
    UK: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011.

    Killen M, Rutland A, Abrams D, Mulvey KL, Hitti A. Development of intra- and intergroup judgments
    in the context of moral and social-conventional norms. Child Development. 2012; Advance
    online publication. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12011

    Kim SY, Wang Y, Chen Q, Shen Y, Hou Y. Parent–child acculturation profiles as predictors of Chinese
    American adolescents’ academic trajectories. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2015; 44:1263–
    1274. DOI: 10.1007/s10964-014-0131-x [PubMed: 24820295]

    Bornstein Page 54

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Kim SY, Wang Y, Orozco-Lapray D, Shen Y, Murtuza M. Does ‘tiger parenting’ exist? Parenting
    profiles of Chinese Americans and adolescent developmental outcomes. Asian American Journal
    of Psychology. 2013; 4:7–18. DOI: 10.1037/a0030612 [PubMed: 23646228]

    Kim, SY.; Wong, VY. Assessing Asian and Asian American parenting: A review of the literature. In:
    Kurasaki, KS.; Okazaki, S.; Sue, S., editors. Asian American mental health: Assessment theories
    and methods. New York: Kluwer; 2002. p. 185-201.

    King, PE.; Boyatzis, CJ. Religious and Spiritual Development. In: Lamb, ME.; Lerner, RM., editors.
    Socioemotional Processes: Volume 3 of the Handbook of child psychology and developmental
    science. 7. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2015. p. 975-1021.

    Knight GP, Tein JY, Shell R, Roosa M. The cross-ethnic equivalence of parenting and family
    interaction measures among Hispanic and Anglo-American families. Child Development. 1992;
    63:1392–1403. DOI: 10.2307/1131564 [PubMed: 1446558]

    Knight GP, Berkel C, Umaña-Taylor AJ, Gonzales NA, Ettekal I, Jaconis M, Boyd BM. The familial
    socialization of culturally related values in Mexican American families. Journal of Marriage and
    Family. 2011; 73(5):913–925. DOI: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00856.x [PubMed: 22021936]

    Koenig M. Social conditions for the implementation of linguistic human rights through multicultural
    policies: The case of the Kyrgyz Republic. Current Issues in Language & Society. 1999; 6(1):57–
    84. DOI: 10.1080/13520529909615536

    Kosic A. Acculturation attitudes, need for cognitive closure and adaptation of immigrants. Journal of
    Social Psychology. 2002; 2:179–201. DOI: 10.1080/00224540209603894

    Kosic, A. Personality and individual factors in acculturation. In: Sam, DL.; Berry, JW., editors. The
    Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge
    University Press; 2006. p. 113-128.

    Kosic A, Kruglanski AW, Pierro A, Manneti L. The social cognition of immigrants’ acculturation:
    Effects of the need for closure and the reference group at entry. Journal of Personality and Social
    Psychology. 2008; 86:796–813. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.796

    Kosik KS, Muñoz C, Lopez L, Arcila ML, García G, Madrigal L, … &, Lopera F. Homozygosity of
    the autosomal dominant Alzheimer disease presenilin 1 E280A mutation. Neurology. 2015;
    84(2):206–208. DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001130 [PubMed: 25471389]

    Kouider EB, Koglin U, Petermann F. Emotional and behavioral problems in migrant children and
    adolescents in Europe: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2014;
    23(6):373–391. DOI: 10.1007/s00787-013-0485-8 [PubMed: 24132833]

    Kovelman I, Baker SA, Petitto LA. Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: A functional
    magnetic resonance imaging investigation of syntactic processing and a possible “neural
    signature” of bilingualism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008; 20(1):153–169. DOI:
    10.1162/jocn.2008.20011 [PubMed: 17919083]

    Krogstad, JM. 5 facts about Mexico and immigration to the U.S. Pew Research Center. 2016. Retrieved
    from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/02/11/mexico-and-immigration-to-us/

    Krogstad, JM.; Lopez, MH. Hispanic population reaches record 55 million, but growth has cooled.
    Pew Research Center. 2015. Retrieved from http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/06/25/u-
    s-hispanic-population-growth-surge-cools/

    Kurglanski A, Webster DM. Motivated closing of the mind: “seizing” and “freezing. Psychological
    Review. 1996; 103:263–283. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.2.263 [PubMed: 8637961]

    Kwak K. Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations for immigrant
    and non-immigrant families. Human Development. 2003; (46):115–136. DOI:
    10.1159/000068581

    Kwak K, Berry JW. Generational differences in acculturation among Asian families in Canada: A
    comparison of Vietnamese, Korean, and East Indian groups. International Journal of Psychology.
    2001; 36(3):152–162. DOI: 10.1080/00207590042000119

    Kwon SA. Moving from Complaints to Action: Oppositional Conscious and Collective Action in a
    Political Community. Anthropology & Education. 2008; 39:59–76. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1548-1492.2008.00005.x

    Bornstein Page 55

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/02/11/mexico-and-immigration-to-us/

    http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/06/25/u-s-hispanic-population-growth-surge-cools/

    http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/06/25/u-s-hispanic-population-growth-surge-cools/

    Lara M, Akinbami L, Flores G, Morgenstern H. Heterogeneity of childhood asthma among Hispanics:
    Puerto Ricans bear a disproportionate burden. Pediatrics. 2006; 117:43–53. DOI: 10.1542/peds.
    2004-1714 [PubMed: 16396859]

    Larson, RW.; Jensen, LA.; Kang, H.; Griffith, A.; Rompala, V. Peer Groups as a Crucibleof Positive
    Value Development in a Global World. Chapter. In: Trommsdorff, G.; Chen, X., editors. Values,
    Religion, and Culture in Adolescent Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press;
    2012.

    Leidy MS, Guerra NG, Toro RI. Positive parenting, family cohesion, and child social competence
    among immigrant Latino families. Journal of Latina/o Psychology. 2012; 1(S):3–13. DOI:
    10.1037/2168-1678.1.S.3

    Lerner, RM. Concepts and theories of human development. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2002.

    Lerner RM. Promoting positive human development and social justice: Integrating theory, research and
    application in contemporary developmental science. International Journal of Psychology. 2015;
    50(3):165–173. DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12162 [PubMed: 25782450]

    Lerner, RM.; Lerner, JV.; Bowers, E.; Geldhof, GJ. Positive youth development and relational-
    developmental systems. In: Overton, WF.; Molenaar, PCM.; Lerner, RM., editors. Theory and
    method. Volume 1 of the Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. 7. Hoboken,
    NJ: Wiley; 2015. p. 607-651.

    Leyendecker B. Children from immigrant families – adaption, development, and resilience. Current
    trends in the study of migration in Europe. International Journal of Developmental Science. 2011;
    5:3–9. DOI: 10.3233/DEV-2011-002

    Leyendecker, B.; Willard, J.; Agache, A.; Jäkel, J.; Spiegler, O.; Kohl, K. The challenges of diaspora
    migration: Interdisciplinary perspectives on Israel and Germany. In: Silbereisen, RK.; Titzmann,
    PF.; Shavit, Y., editors. The challenges of diaspora migration: Interdisciplinary perspectives on.
    Israel and Germany, Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Co; 2014. p. 289-304.

    Liefbroer, AC.; Mulder, CH. Family obligations. In: Dykstra, PA.; Kalmijn, M.; Komter, AE.; Knijn,
    TCM.; Liefbroer, AC.; Mulder, CH., editors. Family solidarity in the Netherlands. Amsterdam:
    Dutch University Press; 2006. p. 123-146.

    Lin CC, Fu VR. A comparison of child-rearing practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and
    Caucasian-American parents. Child Development. 1990; 61:429–433. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1467-8624.1990.tb02789.x

    Loo SK, Rapport MD. Ethnic variations in children’s problem behaviors: A cross-sectional,
    developmental study of Hawaii school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.
    1998; 39:567–575. DOI: 10.1111/1469-7610.00352 [PubMed: 9599784]

    Lopez V. An exploratory study of Mexican-origin fathers’ involvement in their child’s education: The
    role of linguistic acculturation. School Community Journal. 2007; 17:61–70.

    Lowe L. Heterogeneity, Hybridity, Multiplicity: Marking Asian American Differences. Diaspora: A
    Journal of Transnational Studies. 1991; 1:24–44. DOI: 10.1353/dsp.1991.0014

    Lowe, L. Heterogeneity, hybridity, multiplicity: Marking Asian American differences. In: Ono, KA.,
    editor. A companion to Asian American studies. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing; 2004. p.
    254-275.

    Magnusson, D. The logic and implications of a person-oriented approach. In: Cairns, RB.; Bergman,
    LR.; Kagan, J., editors. Methods and models for studying the individual. Thousand Oaks, CA:
    SAGE; 1998. p. 33-64.

    Magnusson, D.; Allen, VL. Implications and applications of an interactional perspective for human
    development. In: Magnusson, D.; Allen, VL., editors. Human development: An interactional
    perspective. New York: Academic Press; 1983. p. 469-387.

    Marin G, Gamba RJ, Marin BV. Extreme response style and acquiescence among Hispanics. Journal of
    Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1992; 23:498–509. DOI: 10.1177/0022022192234006

    Maisonneuve C, Teste B. Acculturation preferences of a host community: The effects of immigrant
    acculturation strategies on evaluations and impression formation. International Journal of
    Intercultural Relations. 2007; 31:669–688. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.06.001

    Bornstein Page 56

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Malti T, Killen M, Gasser L. Social judgments and emotion attributions about exclusion in
    Switzerland. Child Development. 2012; 83:697–712. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01705.x
    [PubMed: 22182049]

    Marra C. Il monitoraggio dei fenomeni migratori nel Friuli-Venezia Giulia. Una rassegna bibliografica.
    Studi Emigrazione. 2002; 147:702–711.

    Martinez JN, Aguayo-Tellez E, Rangel-Gonzalez E. Explaining the Mexican-American Health
    Paradox Using Selectivity Effects. International Migration Review. 2014; 49:878–906. DOI:
    10.1111/imre.12112

    Massey DS. Hispanic residential segregation: A comparison of Mexicans, Cubans, and Puerto Ricans.
    Sociology and Social Research. 1981; 65:311–322.

    Massey DS, Bitterman B. Explaining the paradox of Puerto Rican segregation. Social Forces. 1985;
    64:306–331. DOI: 10.1093/sf/64.2.306

    Massey DS, Mullan BP. Processes of Hispanic and black spatial assimilation. American Journal of
    Sociology. 1984; 89:836–873. DOI: 10.1086/227946

    Matsumoto, DR.; van de Vijver, FJR., editors. Cross-cultural research methods in psychology. New
    York: Cambridge University Press; 2011.

    McGee WJ. Piratical acculturation. American Anthropologist. 1898; 11:243–249.

    Meisel, JM. Two first languages: Early grammatical development in bilingual children. Dordrecht:
    Foris; 1990.

    Mendenhall ME, Oddou G. The dimension of expatriate acculturation: a review. Academy of
    Management Review. 1985; 10:39–47. DOI: 10.5465/amr.1985.4277340

    Mendoza, FS.; Javier, JR.; Burgos, AE. Health of children in immigrant families. In: Lansford, JE.;
    Deater-Deckard, K.; Bornstein, MH., editors. Immigrant families in contemporary society. New
    York: Guilford Press; 2007. p. 30-50.

    Menjivar C. Family reorganization in a context of legal uncertainty: Guatemalan and Salvadorean
    immigrants in the United States. International Journal of Sociology of the Family. 2006; 32:223–
    245.

    Minoura Y. The psychological reorganization of overseas experience after returning to Japan. Shakai
    Shinrigaku Kenkyuu. 1988; 3:3–11.

    Mirsky J, Barasch M. Adjustment problems among Soviet immigrants at risk, emotional distress
    among elderly Soviet immigrants during the Gulf War. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related
    Sciences. 1993; 30:233–243. [PubMed: 8163360]

    Mistry J, Wu J. Navigating cultural worlds and negotiating identities: A conceptual model. Human
    Development. 2010; 53:5–25. DOI: 10.1159/000268136

    Molcho M, Cristini F, Gabhainn SN, Santinello M, Moreno C, de Matos MG, … Due P. Health and
    well-being among child immigrants in Europe. Eurohealth. 2010; 16(1):20–24.

    Montagliani A, Giacalone R. Impression management in cross-cultural adaptation. Journal of Social
    Psychology. 1998; 138:598–608. DOI: 10.1080/00224549809600415

    Montreuil A, Bourhis RY. Majority acculturation orientations toward values and devalued immigrants.
    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2001; 32:698–719. DOI: 10.1177/0022022101032006004

    Morland L. Promising practices in positive youth development with immigrants and refugees. The
    Prevention Researcher. 2007; 14:18–20. DOI: 10.1037/e717692007-005

    Moscardino U, Bertelli C, Altoè G. Culture, migration, and parenting: A comparative study of mother-
    infant interaction and childrearing patterns in Romanian, Romanian immigrant, and Italian
    families. International Journal of Developmental Science. 2011; 5(1–2):11–25. DOI: 10.3233/
    DEV-2011-11072

    Motti-Stefanidi F, Asendorpf JB, Masten AS. The adaptation and well-being of adolescent immigrants
    in Greek schools: A multilevel, longitudinal study of risks and resources. Development and
    Psychopathology. 2012; 24(2):451.doi: 10.1017/s0954579412000090 [PubMed: 22559124]

    Motti-Stefanidi F, Pavlopoulos V, Obradovi J, Dalla M, Takis N, Papathanassiou A, Masten AS.
    Immigration as a risk factor for adolescent adaptation in Greek urban schools. European Journal
    of Developmental Psychology. 2008; 5:235–261. DOI: 10.1080/17405620701556417

    Bornstein Page 57

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Nagata DK. Assessing Asian American acculturation and ethnic identity. Asian American and Pacific
    Islander Journal of Health. 1994; 2:109–121.

    Nauck, B. Acculturation. In: van de Vijver, FJ.; van Hemert, DA.; Poortinga, YH., editors. Multilevel
    analysis of individuals and cultures. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2008. p. 379-410.

    Nauck B, Lotter V. Parenting styles and perceived instrumentality of schooling in native, Turkish, and
    Vietnamese families in Germany. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswiisenschaft. 2015; 18(4):845–869.
    DOI: 10.1007/s11618-015-0630-x

    Nazario, S. Child migrants, alone in court. The New York Times. 2013 Apr 10. Retrieved from http://
    www.nytimes.com/2013/04/11/opinion/give-lawyers-to-immigrant-children.html?_r=0

    Neblett EW, Rivas-Drake D, Umaña-Taylor AJ. The promise of racial and ethnic protective factors in
    promoting ethnic minority youth development. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6(3):295–
    303. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00239.x

    Nesdale D, Maass A, Durkin K, Griffiths J. Group norms, threat, and children’s racial prejudice. Child
    Development. 2005; 76:652–663. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00869.x [PubMed: 15892784]

    Nigbur C, Brown R, Cameron L, Hossain R, Landau A, Le Touze D, … &, Watters C. Acculturation,
    well-being and classroom behaviour among white British and British Asian primary-school
    children in the south-east of England: Validating a child-friendly measure of acculturation
    attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 2008; 32:493–504. DOI: 10.1016/
    j.ijintrel.2008.03.001

    Nguyen, AMD.; Benet-Martínez, V. Multicultural identity: What it is and why it matters. The
    psychology of social and cultural diversity. In: Crisp, RJ., editor. The psychology of social and
    cultural diversity: Social issues and interventions. New York: Wiley-Blackwell; 2010. p. 87-114.

    Nguyen AMD, Benet-Martínez V. Biculturalism and adjustment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-
    Cultural Psychology. 2013; 44:122–159. DOI: 10.1177/0022022111435097

    Ogbu, JU. Immigrant and involuntary minorities in comparative perspective. In: Gibson, MA.; Ogbu,
    JU., editors. Minority status and schooling: A comparative study of immigrant and involuntary
    minorities. New York: Garland Publishing; 1991. p. 3-33.

    Okamoto, DG.; Gast, MJ.; Feldman, V. Managing diversity in community-based youth organizations.
    Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Associative; Denver, CO.
    2012.

    Ones DS, Viswesvaran C. Relative importance of personality dimensions for expatriate selection: A
    policy capturing study. Human Performance. 1999; 12:275–294. DOI:
    10.1080/08959289909539872

    Osypuk TL, Galea S, McArdle N, Acevedo-Garcia D. Quantifying separate and unequal: Racial-ethnic
    distributions of neighborhood poverty in metropolitan America. Urban Affairs Review. 2009;
    45:25–65. DOI: 10.1177/1078087408331119 [PubMed: 20160903]

    Overton, WF. Process and relational-developmental-systems. In: Overton, WF.; Molenaar, PCM.;
    Lerner, RM., editors. Theory and method. Volume 1 of the Handbook of child psychology and
    developmental science. 7. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2015. p. 9-62.

    Palloni A, Arias E. Paradox lost: Explaining the Hispanic adult mortality advantage. Demography.
    2004; 41:385–415. DOI: 10.1353/dem.2004.0024 [PubMed: 15461007]

    Passel, JS.; Cohn, D. U.S. Population Projections: 2005–2050. Pew Research Center. 2008. Retrieved
    from http://www.pewhispanic.org/2008/02/11/us-population-projections-2005-2050/

    Passel, JS.; Cohn, D.; Gonzalez-Barrera, A. Net Migration from Mexico Falls to Zero – and Perhaps
    Less. Pew Research Center. 2012. Retrieved from http://www.pewhispanic.org/2012/04/23/net-
    migration-from-mexico-falls-to-zero-and-perhaps-less/

    Paul, GL. Behavior modification research: Design and tactics. In: Franks, CM., editor. Behavior
    therapy: Appraisal and status. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1969. p. 29-62.

    Peck SC, Brodish AB, Malanchuk O, Banerjee M, Eccles JS. Racial/ethnic socialization and identity
    development in Black families: The role of parent and youth reports. Developmental Psychology.
    2014; 50(7):1897–1909. DOI: 10.1037/a0036800 [PubMed: 24798504]

    Perreira KM, Chapman MV, Stein GL. Becoming an American parent. Journal of Family Issues. 2006;
    27:1383–1414. DOI: 10.1177/0192513×06290041

    Bornstein Page 58

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.pewhispanic.org/2008/02/11/us-population-projections-2005-2050/

    http://www.pewhispanic.org/2012/04/23/net-migration-from-mexico-falls-to-zero-and-perhaps-less/

    http://www.pewhispanic.org/2012/04/23/net-migration-from-mexico-falls-to-zero-and-perhaps-less/

    Perreira KM, Fuligni A, Potochnick S. Fitting in: The roles of social acceptance and discrimination in
    shaping the academic motivations of Latino youth in the US Southeast. Journal of Social Issues.
    2010; 66(1):131–153. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01637.x [PubMed: 22611286]

    Pessar PR, Mahler SJ. Transnational migration: Bringing gender in. International Migration Review.
    2003; 37:812–846. DOI: 10.1111/j.1747-7379.2003.tb00159.x

    Pew Hispanic Center. A nation of immigrants: A portrait of the 40 million, including 11 million
    unauthorized. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center; 2013.

    Pfeifer JH, Ruble DN, Bachman MA, Alvarez JM, Cameron JA, Fuligni AJ. Social identities and
    intergroup bias in immigrant and nonimmigrant children. Developmental Psychology. 2007;
    43:496–507. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.496 [PubMed: 17352555]

    Phalet K, Schönpflug U. Intergenerational transmission of collectivism and achievement values in two
    acculturation contexts: The case of Turkish families in Germany and Turkish and Moroccan
    families in the Netherlands. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2001; 32:186–201. DOI:
    10.1177/0022022101032002006

    Phinney, JS. Acculturation is not an independent variable: Approaches to studying acculturation as a
    complex process. In: Bornstein, MH.; Cote, LR., editors. Acculturation and parent-child
    relationships: Measurement and development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. p. 79-95.

    Phinney JS, Flores J. “Unpacking” acculturation: Aspects of acculturation as predictors of traditional
    sex role attitudes. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2002; 33:320–331. DOI:
    10.1177/0022022102033003007

    Phinney JS, Horenczyk G, Liebkind K, Vedder P. Ethnic identity, immigration, and well-being: An
    interactional perspective. Journal of Social Issues. 2001; 57:493–510. DOI:
    10.1111/0022-4537.00225

    Phinney JS, Ong A, Madden T. Cultural values and intergenerational value discrepancies in immigrant
    and non-immigrant families. Child Development. 2000; 71:528–539. DOI:
    10.1111/1467-8624.00162 [PubMed: 10834482]

    Phinney, JS.; Vedder, P. Family relationship values of adolescents and parents: Intergenerational
    discrepancies and adaptation. In: Berry, JW.; Phinney, JS.; Sam, DL.; Vedder, P., editors.
    Immigrant youth in cultural transition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. p. 167-184.

    Poppe, E.; Verkuyten, M. Gender differences in prejudice: A study on evaluations of Muslims, Muslim
    men and Muslim women among Dutch adolescents. Utrecht, the Netherlands: Ercomer; 2012.

    Portes, A.; Bach, RL. Latin journey: Cuban and Mexican immigrants in the United States. Berkeley:
    University of California Press; 1985.

    Portes A, Landolt P. The downside of social capital. The American Prospect. 1996; 7:18–23. Retrieved
    from: http://www.prospect.org/print-friendly/print/V7/26/26-cnt2.html.

    Portes, A.; Rumbaut, RG. Legacies: The story of the immigrant second generation. Berkeley:
    University of California Press; 2001.

    Portes, A.; Rumbaut, RG. Immigrant America: A portrait. 3. Berkeley: University of California Press;
    2006.

    Portes, A.; Stepick, A. City on the edge: The transformation of Miami. Berkeley, CA: University of
    California Press; 1993.

    Portes A, Zhou M. The new second generation: Segmented assimilation and its variants. The ANNALS
    of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 1993; 530:74–96. DOI:
    10.1177/0002716293530001006

    Powell, JW. Introduction to the study of Indian languages. 2. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
    Printing Office; 1880.

    Powell JW. Report of the director. Report of the Bureau of American Ethnology. 1900; 19:xi–xxxiii.

    Purzycki BG, Apicella C, Atkinson QD, Cohen E, McNamara RA, Willard AK, Xygalatas D,
    Norenzayan A, Henrich J. Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human
    sociality. Nature. 2016; 530:327–330. DOI: 10.1038/nature16980 [PubMed: 26863190]

    Qin DB, Way N, Mukherjee P. The other side of the model minority story the familial and peer
    challenges faced by Chinese American adolescents. Youth & Society. 2008; 39:480–506.

    Raikes H, Pan L, Luze G, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Brooks-Gunn J, Constantine J, Tarullo LB, …
    Rodriguez ET. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes

    Bornstein Page 59

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.prospect.org/print-friendly/print/V7/26/26-cnt2.html

    during the first three years of life. Child Development. 2006; 77:924–953. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1467-8624.2006.00911.x [PubMed: 16942498]

    Redfield R, Linton R, Herskovits MJ. Memorandum for the study of acculturation. American
    Anthropologist. 1936; 38:149–152. DOI: 10.1525/aa.1936.38.1.02a00330

    Reese L, Gallimore R. Immigrant Latinos’ cultural model of literacy development: An evolving
    perspective on home-school discontinuities. American Journal of Education. 2000; 108:103–134.
    DOI: 10.1086/444236

    Riggs NR, Bohnert AM, Guzman MD, Davidson D. Examining the potential of community-based
    after-school programs for Latino youth. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2010;
    45:417–429. DOI: 10.1007/s10464-010-9313-1 [PubMed: 20300821]

    Riosmena F, Everett BG, Rogers RG, Dennis JA. Negative acculturation and nothing more?
    Cumulative disadvantage and mortality during the immigrant adaptation process among Latinos
    in the United States. International Migration Review. 2015; 49(2):443–478. DOI: 10.1111/imre.
    12102

    Roccas S, Brewer MB. Social identity complexity. Personality and Social professional psychology.
    Research and Practice. 2002; 25:207–213. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0602_01

    Rodríguez SA, Perez-Brena NJ, Updegraff KA, Umaña-Taylor AJ. Emotional closeness in Mexican-
    origin adolescents’ relationships with mothers, fathers, and same-sex friends. Journal of Youth &
    Adolescence. 2014; 43:1953–1968. DOI: 10.1007/s10964-013-0004-8 [PubMed: 23999997]

    Rodriguez N, Myers HF, Mira CB, Flores T, Garcia-Hernandez L. Development of the
    multidimensional acculturative stress inventory for adults of Mexican origin. Psychological
    Assessment. 2002; 14:451–461. DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.451 [PubMed: 12501570]

    Rosenthal D, Ranieri N, Klimidis S. Vietnamese adolescents in Australia: Relationships between
    perceptions of self and parental values, intergenerational conflict, and gender dissatisfaction.
    International Journal of Psychology. 1996; 31:81–91. DOI: 10.1080/002075996401106

    Ross-Sheriff, F.; Tirmazi, MT.; Walsh, TR. Cultural and religious contexts of parenting by immigrant
    South Asian Muslim mothers. In: Lansford, JE.; Deater-Deckard, K.; Bornstein, MH., editors.
    Immigrant families in contemporary society. New York: Guildford Press; 2007. p. 194-211.

    Roy AL, Hughes D, Yoshikawa H. Intersections between nativity, ethnic density, and neighborhood
    SES: Using an ethnic enclave framework to explore variation in Puerto Ricans’ physical health.
    American Journal of Community Psychology. 2013; 51:468–479. DOI: 10.1007/
    s10464-012-9564-0 [PubMed: 23314837]

    Rudmin FW. Critical history of the acculturation psychology of assimilation, separation, integration,
    and marginalization. Review of General Psychology. 2003; 7:3–37. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1037/1089-2680.7.3.250.

    Rudmin FW. Review of Immigrant youth in cultural transition: Acculturation, identity, and adaptation
    across national contexts. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2008; 39:230–233. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022107313860.

    Ryder AG, Alden LE, Paulhus DL. Is acculturation unidimensional or bidimensional? A headto-head
    comparison in the prediction of personality, self-identity, and adjustment. Journal of Personality
    and Social Psychology. 2000; 79:49–65. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.49 [PubMed: 10909877]

    Saad CS, Sue S, Zane N, Cho YI. The relationship between education and ethnic minority factors in
    income among Asian Americans. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2012; 3:66–78. DOI:
    10.1037/a0026867

    Salusky I, Larson RW, Griffith A, Wu J, Raffaelli M, Sugimura N, Guzman M. How Adolescents
    Develop Responsibility: What Can Be Learned From Youth Programs. Journal of Research and
    Adolescence. 2014; 24:417–430. DOI: 10.1111/jora.12118

    Sam, DL.; Virta, E. Social group identity and its effect on the self-esteem of adolescents with
    immigrant background. In: Craven, RG.; Marsh, HW.; McInerney, DM., editors. International
    advances in self research. Sydney: Information Age Publishers; 2003. p. 347-373.

    Samm A, Varnik A, Tooding LM, Sisask M, Kolves K, von Knorring AL. Children’s Depression
    Inventory in Estonia. Single items and factor structure by age and gender. European Child and
    Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008; 17:162–170. DOI: 10.1007/s00787-007-0650-z [PubMed:
    17876502]

    Bornstein Page 60

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.3.250

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.3.250

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022107313860

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022107313860

    Santiago AM. Patterns of Puerto Rican segregation and mobility. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral
    Sciences. 1992; 14:107–133. DOI: 10.1177/07399863920141006

    Schaller A, Rocha LO, Barshinger D. Maternal attitudes and parent education: How immigrant
    mothers support their child’s education despite their own low levels of education. Early
    Childhood Education Journal. 2007; 34:351–356. DOI: 10.1007/s10643-006-0143-6

    Schmidt, S.; Morland, L.; Rose, J. Growing Up in a New Country: A Positive Youth Development
    Toolkit for Working with Refugees and Immigrants. 2006. Retrieved from: http://www.brycs.org/
    documents/upload/GrowingUpInANewCountry-Web

    Schmitz MF. Cultural and acculturation differences in trajectories of Home Environment Inventory
    Scores for Latino children and families. Journal of Family Issues. 2005; 25:568–583. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513×04273586.

    Schmitz, PG. Acculturation and adaptation process among immigrants in Germany. In: Bouvy, AM.;
    van de Vijver, FJ.; Schmitz, PG., editors. Journeys into cross-cultural psychology. Amsterdam:
    Swets & Zeitlinger; 1994. p. 142-157.

    Schwartz SJ, Pantin H, Sullivan S, Prado G, Szapocznik J. Nativity and years in the receiving culture
    as markers of acculturation in ethnic enclaves. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2006a;
    37:345–353. DOI: 10.1177/0022022106286928 [PubMed: 16799700]

    Schwartz SJ, Montgomery MJ, Briones E. The role of identity in acculturation among immigrant
    people: Theoretical propositions, empirical questions, and applied recommendations. Human
    Development. 2006b; 49:1–30. DOI: 10.1159/000090300

    Schwartz SJ, Unger JB, Zamboanga BL, Szapocznik J. Rethinking the oncept of acculturation:
    Implications for theory and research. American Psychologist. 2010; 65:237–251. DOI: 10.1037/
    a0019330 [PubMed: 20455618]

    Schwartz S, Zamboanga B, Jarvis LH. Ethnic identity and acculturation in Hispanic early adolescents:
    Mediated relationships to academic grades, prosocial behavior, and externalizing symptoms.
    Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2007; 13:364–373. DOI:
    10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.364 [PubMed: 17967105]

    Scott, WA.; Scott, R. Adaptation of immigrants: Individual differences and determinants. Pergamon
    Press; Oxford: 1989.

    Scott WA, Stumpf J. Personal satisfaction and role performance: Subjective and social aspects of
    adaptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1984; 47:182–206. DOI:
    10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.812 [PubMed: 6747814]

    Searle W, Ward C. The prediction of psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-cultural
    transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Social Relations. 1990; 14:449–464. DOI:
    10.1016/0147-1767(90)90030-z

    Seelye HN, Brewer MB. Ethnocentrism and acculturation of North Americans in Guatemala. Journal
    of Social Psychology. 1970; 80:147–155. DOI: 10.1080/00224545.1970.9712535 [PubMed:
    5496402]

    Shin J, Bayram-Ozdemir S, Lee J, Cheah CSL. The parenting practices and child outcomes of Korean
    immigrant mothers with different acculturative strategies. Korean Journal of Child Studies. 2010;
    31:1–17.

    Shore B. Taking culture seriously. Human Development. 2002; 45:226–228. DOI: 10.1159/000064982

    Shuval, JT. Migration and stress. In: Goldberger, L.; Breznitz, S., editors. Handbook of stress :
    Theoretical and clinical aspects. Free Press; London: 1982. p. 677-691.

    Simpkins SD, Delgado MY, Price CD, Quach A, Starbuck E. Socioeconomic status, ethnicity, culture,
    and immigration: Examining the potential mechanisms underlying Mexican-origin adolescents’
    organized activity participation. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49:706–721. DOI: 10.1037/
    a0028399 [PubMed: 22545831]

    Song L, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Yoshikawa H, Kahana-Kalman R, Wu I. Language experiences and
    vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years.
    Developmental Psychology. 2012; 48:1106–1123. DOI: 10.1037/a0026401 [PubMed: 22142187]

    Song, YI. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Ohio State University; Canton, OH: 1986. Battered
    Korean women in urban America: The relationship of cultural conflict to wife abuse.

    Bornstein Page 61

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.brycs.org/documents/upload/GrowingUpInANewCountry-Web

    http://www.brycs.org/documents/upload/GrowingUpInANewCountry-Web

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513×04273586

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513×04273586

    Sonuga-Barke EJS, Monocha K, Taylor EA, Sandberg S. Inter-ethnic bias in teachers’ ratings of
    childhood hyperactivity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1993; 11:187–200. DOI:
    10.1111/j.2044-835x.1993.tb00597.x

    South SJ, Crowder K, Chavez E. Exiting and entering high-poverty neighborhoods: Latinos, Blacks
    and Anglos compared. Social Forces. 2005; 84:873–900. DOI: 10.1353/sof.2006.0037

    Spencer, MB. Phenomenology and ecological systems theory: Development of diverse groups. In:
    Damon, W.; Lerner, RM., editors. Theoretical models of human development: Vol. 1 of the
    Handbook of child psychology. 6. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. p. 829-893.

    Spiegler O, Leyendecker B, Kohl K. Acculturation gaps between Turkish immigrant marriage partners:
    Resource or source of distress? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2015; 46(5):667–683.
    DOI: 10.1177/0022022115578686

    Sterba, CM. Good Americans: Italian and Jewish immigrants during the First World War. New York:
    Oxford University Press; 2003.

    Stevens GWJM, Pels T, Bengi-Arslan L, Verhulst FC, Vollebergh WAM, Crijnen AAM. Parent, teacher
    and self-reported problem behaviour in The Netherlands. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
    Epidemiology. 2003; 38:576–585. DOI: 10.1007/s00127-003-0677-5 [PubMed: 14569425]

    Stilling EA. The electronic melting pot hypothesis: The cultivation of acculturation among Hispanics
    through television viewing. Howard Journal of Communications. 1997; 8:77–100. DOI:
    10.1080/10646179709361744

    Stringer C. The dates of Eden. Nature. 1988; 331:565–566. DOI: 10.1038/331565a0 [PubMed:
    3124001]

    Stuart J, Ward C, Adam Z. Current issues in the development and acculturation of Muslim youth in
    New Zealand. International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development Bulletin. 2010;
    2(58):9–13.

    Su TF, Costigan CL. The development of children’s ethnic identity in immigrant Chinese families in
    Canada: The role of parenting practices and children’s perceptions of parental family obligation
    expectations. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2009; 29(5):638–663. DOI:
    10.1177/0272431608325418

    Suárez-Orozco C, Qin DB. Gendered perspectives in psychology: Immigrant origin youth.
    International Migration Review. 2006; 40(1):165–198. DOI: 10.1111/j.1747-7379.2006.00007.x

    Suárez-Orozco, C.; Suárez-Orozco, M. Children of immigration: The developing child. Cambridge,
    MA: Harvard University Press; 2001.

    Suárez-Orozco C, Todorova I, Louie J. Making up for lost time: The experience of separation and
    reunification among immigrant families. Family Process. 2002; 41:625–643. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1545-5300.2002.00625.x [PubMed: 12613121]

    Sy SR, Schulenberg JE. Parent beliefs and children’s achievement trajectories during the transition to
    school in Asian American and European American families. International Journal of Behavioral
    Development. 2005; 29:505–515.

    Szapocznik J, Kurtines WM. Family psychology and cultural diversity. Opportunities for theory,
    research and application. American Psychology. 1993; 48:400–407. DOI: 10.1037//0003-066x.
    48.4.400

    Taft R. The shared frame of reference concept applied to the assimilation of immigrants. Human
    Relations. 1953; 6:45–55.

    Taft R. A psychological model for the study of social assimilation. Human Relations. 1957; 10:141–
    156. DOI: 10.1177/001872675701000204

    Tamis-LeMonda, CS.; Baumwell, L.; Diaz, S. Foundations to school readiness in Latino immigrant
    children in the U.S. To appear. In: Chuang, S., editor. Immigrant Children. Lexington Books;
    2011.

    Tamis-LeMonda CS, Sze I, Ng F, Kahana-Kalman R, Yoshikawa H. Maternal teaching during play
    with 4-year olds: variation by ethnicity and family resources. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2013;
    59:361–398. DOI: 10.1353/mpq.2013.0016

    Tamis-LeMonda CS, Way N, Hughes D, Yoshikawa H, Kalman RK, Niwa EY. Parents’ goals for
    children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individuals.
    Social Development. 2008; 17:183–209. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00419.x

    Bornstein Page 62

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Tang TN, Dion KL. Gender and acculturation in relation to traditionalism: Perceptions of self and
    parents among Chinese students. Sex Roles. 1999; 41:17–29. DOI: 10.1023/A:1018881523745

    Tartakovsky E, Schwartz PG. Motivation for emigration, values, well-being, and identification among
    young Russian Jews. International Journal of Psychology. 2001; 36:88–99. DOI:
    10.1080/00207590042000100

    Taylor ZE, Larsen-Rife D, Conger RD, Widaman KF. Familism, interparental conflict, and parenting in
    Mexican-origin families: A cultural-contextual framework. Journal of Marriage and Family.
    2012; 74:312–327. DOI: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00958.x [PubMed: 22736810]

    Teichman JR, Contreras-Grau JM. Acculturation and teaching styles among young mainland Puerto
    Rican mothers. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2006; 28:84–101. DOI:
    10.1177/0739986305284092

    The next wave: Hmong shamans unite. In Facebook [Community page]. n.d. Retrieved from https://
    www.facebook.com/HmongShamans

    Theokas C, Almerigi JB, Lerner RM, Dowling EM, Benson PL, Scales PC, von Eye A.
    Conceptualizing and modeling individual and ecological asset components of thriving in early
    adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2005; 25(1):113–143.

    Thomas, WI.; Znaniecki, F. The Polish peasant in Europe and America. New York: Dover; 1958.
    Original work published 1918

    Triandis, HC. Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press; 1995.

    Tseng V. Family interdependence and academic adjustment in college youth from immigrant and U.
    S.-born families. Child Development. 2004; 75:966–983. DOI: 10.1111/j.
    1467-8624.2004.00717.x [PubMed: 15144497]

    Unger JB, Ritt-Olson A, Soto DW, Baezconde-Garbanati L. Parent-child acculturation discrepancies as
    a risk factor for substance use among Hispanic adolescents in southern California. Journal of
    Immigrant and Minority Health. 2009; 11(3):149–157. DOI: 10.1007/s10903-007-9083-5
    [PubMed: 17922232]

    United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. International
    Migration Report 2015: Highlights (ST/ESA/SER.A/375). 2016. Retrieved from: http://
    www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/publications/migrationreport/docs/
    MigrationReport2015_Highlights

    United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. UNHCR Mid-Year Trends 2015. 2015. Retrieved
    from http://www.unhcr.org/56701b969.html

    Urban Institute. Children of Immigrants Data Tool. Feb. 2016 http://apps.urban.org/features/children-
    of-immigrants/

    U.S. Census Bureau. 2010 American Community Survey. 2010. Retrieved from
    www.factfinder.census.gov

    U.S. Census Bureau. Estimates of the Components of Resident Population Change by Race and
    Hispanic Origin for the United States: April 1, 2010 to July 1, 2012. 2012 Population Estimates.
    2012a. Retrieved from http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/
    productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    U.S. Census Bureau. Projections of the Population by Sex, Race, and Hispanic Origin for the United
    States: 2015 to 2060. 2012b. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/projections/data/
    national/2012/summarytables.html

    U.S. Census Bureau. Annual Estimates of the Resident Population by Sex, Race Alone or in
    Combination, and Hispanic Origin for the United States, States, and Counties: April 1, 2010 to
    July 1, 2014. 2014 Population Estimates. 2014. Retrieved from http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/
    tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    U.S. Citizenship and Immigration Services. n.d. Retrieved from https://www.uscis.gov/

    U. S. Customs and Border Protection. Southwest Border Unaccompanied Alien Children Statistics FY
    2016. 2016. Retrieved from http://www.cbp.gov/newsroom/stats/southwest-border-
    unaccompanied-children/fy-2016

    Valentine S. Self-esteem, cultural identity, and generation status as determinants of Hispanic
    acculturation. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2001; 23:459–468. DOI:
    10.1177/0739986301234007

    Bornstein Page 63

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    https://www.facebook.com/HmongShamans

    https://www.facebook.com/HmongShamans

    http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/publications/migrationreport/docs/MigrationReport2015_Highlights

    http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/publications/migrationreport/docs/MigrationReport2015_Highlights

    http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/publications/migrationreport/docs/MigrationReport2015_Highlights

    http://www.unhcr.org/56701b969.html

    http://apps.urban.org/features/children-of-immigrants/

    http://apps.urban.org/features/children-of-immigrants/

    http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    http://www.census.gov/population/projections/data/national/2012/summarytables.html

    http://www.census.gov/population/projections/data/national/2012/summarytables.html

    http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk

    https://www.uscis.gov/

    http://www.cbp.gov/newsroom/stats/southwest-border-unaccompanied-children/fy-2016

    http://www.cbp.gov/newsroom/stats/southwest-border-unaccompanied-children/fy-2016

    Valentine S, Mosley G. Acculturation and sex-role attitudes among Mexican Americans: A
    longitudinal analysis. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2000; 22:104–113. DOI:
    10.1177/0739986300221006

    Van Acker K, Vanbeselaere N. Heritage culture maintenance precludes host culture adoption and vice
    versa: Flemings’ perceptions of Turks’ acculturation behaviour. Group Processes and Intergroup
    Relations. 2012; 15:133–145. DOI: 10.1177/1368430211417263

    Van de Vijver, FJR. Acculturation. In: Bornstein, MH.; Arterberry, ME.; Fingerman, KL.; Lansford,
    JE., editors. The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE;
    p. xx-xx.

    Van Geel M, Vedder P. The adaptation of non-western and Muslim immigrant adolescents in the
    Netherlands: An immigrant paradox? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2010; 51:398–402.
    DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00831.x [PubMed: 20602739]

    Van Oudenhoven JP, Prins KS, Buunk BP. Attitudes of minority and majority members towards
    adaptation of immigrants. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1998; 28:995–1013. DOI:
    10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(1998110)28:6<995::AID-EJSP908>3.0.CO;2-8

    Vasileva K. 6.5% of the EU population are foreigners and 9k4% are born abroad. Eurostat: statistics in
    focus 2011. 2011; 34:1–8.

    Velasco Gonzalez KV, Verkuyten M, Weesie J, Poppe E. Prejudice towards Muslims in the
    Netherlands: Testing integrated threat theory. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2008;
    47:667–685. DOI: 10.1348/014466608X284443 [PubMed: 18284782]

    Verdon S, McLeod S, Winsler A. Language maintenance and loss in a population study of young
    Australian children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2014; 29(2):168–181. DOI: 10.1016/
    j.ecresq.2013.12.003

    Verkuyten, M. The social psychology of ethnic identity. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2005.

    Verkuyten M, Kinket B. Social distances in a multi-ethnic society: The ethnic hierarchy among Dutch
    pre-adolescents. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2000; 63:75–85. DOI: 10.2307/2695882

    Verkuyten M, Thijs J. Ethnic and gender bias among Dutch and Turkish children in late childhood:
    The role of social context. Infant and Child Development. 2001; 10:203–217. DOI: 10.1002/icd.
    279

    Verkuyten M, Thijs J. Racist victimization among children in the Netherlands: The effect of ethnic
    group and school. Ethnic and Racial Studies. 2002; 25:310–331. DOI:
    10.1080/01419870120109502

    Verkuyten M, Thijs J, Sierksma J. Majority children’s evaluation of acculturation preferences of
    immigrant and emigrant peers. Child Development. 2013; 85:176–191. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12111
    [PubMed: 23621772]

    Villarruel, FA.; Montero-Sieburth, M.; Dunbar, C.; Outley, CW. Dorothy, there is no yellow brick road:
    The paradox of community youth development approaches for Latino and African American
    urban youth. In: Mahoney, JL.; Larson, R.; Eccles, JS., editors. Organized activities as context of
    development: Extracurricular activities, after-school and community programs. Mahwah, NJ:
    Erlbaum; 2005. p. 111-129.

    von Eye, A.; Bergman, LR.; Hsieh, C. Person-oriented methodological approaches. In: Overton, WF.;
    Molenaar, PCM.; Lerner, RM., editors. Theory and method. Volume 1 of the Handbook of child
    psychology and developmental science. 7. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2015. p. 789-842.

    Wachs, TD. Necessary but not sufficient. Washington, DC: APA Books; 2000.

    Wang Y, Kim SY, Anderson ER, Chen AC, Yan N. Parent-child acculturation discrepancy, perceived
    parental knowledge, peer deviance, and adolescent delinquency in Chinese immigrant families.
    Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2012; 41(7):907–919. DOI: 10.1007/s10964-011-9705-z
    [PubMed: 21833664]

    Ward, C. The A, B, Cs of acculturation. In: Matsumoto, D., editor. The handbook of culture and
    psychology. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2001. p. 411-445.

    Ward, C.; Bochner, S.; Furnham, A. The psychology of culture shock. Hove, United Kingdom:
    Routledge; 2001.

    Bornstein Page 64

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Ward, C.; Chang, W.; Lopez-Nerney, S. Psychological and sociocultural adjustment of Filipina
    domestic workers in Singapore. In: Lasry, JC.; Adair, J.; Dion, K., editors. Latest contributions to
    cultural psychology. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1999. p. 118-134.

    Ward C, Kennedy A. Locus of control, mood disturbance, and social difficulty during cross-Cultural
    transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 1992; 16:175–194. DOI:
    10.1016/0147-1767(92)90017-o

    Ward C, Kennedy A. Where’s the ‘‘culture’’ in cross-cultural transition? Comparative studies of
    sojourner adjustment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1993; 24:221–249. DOI:
    10.1177/0022022193242006

    Ward C, Kennedy A. The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. International Journal of
    Intercultural Relation. 1999; 56:1–19. DOI: 10.1016/s0147-1767(99)00014-0

    Ward C, Leong CH, Low M. Personality and sojourner adjustment: An exploration of the “Big Five”
    and the “Cultural Fit” proposition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2004; 35:137–151.
    DOI: 10.1177/0022022103260719

    Warren R, Kerwin D. The U.S. eligible-to-naturalize population: Detailed social and economic
    characteristics. Journal on Migration and Human Security. 2015; 3:306–329. DOI: 10.14240/
    jmhs.v3i4.54

    Weisskirch RS, Kim SY, Zamboanga BL, Schwartz SJ, Bersamin M, Umaña-Taylor AJ. Cultural
    influences for college student language brokers. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
    Psychology. 2011; 17(1):43–51. DOI: 10.1037/a0021665 [PubMed: 21341896]

    Whiten, A.; McGuigan, N. Conformity. In: Bornstein, MH.; Arterberry, ME.; Fingerman, KL.;
    Lansford, JE., editors. The SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development. Los Angeles,
    CA: SAGE; p. xx-xx.

    Wilson KL, Portes A. Immigrant enclaves—An analysis of the labor-market experiences of Cubans in
    Miami. American Journal of Sociology. 1980; 86:295–319. DOI: 10.1086/227240

    Wissenschaftlicher Beirat für Familienfragen. 2002. http://www.bmfsfj.de/BMFSFJ/Ministerium/
    beiraete,did=9388.html

    Wohlwill, JF. The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press; 1971.

    Wu NH, Kim SY. Chinese American adolescents’ perceptions of the language brokering experience as
    a sense of burden and sense of efficacy. Journal of Youth & Adolescence. 2009; 38(5):703–718.
    DOI: 10.1007/s10964-008-9379-3 [PubMed: 19636765]

    Xu Y, Farver JAM, Krieg A. The home environment and Asian immigrant children’s early literacy
    skills. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2017; 17:xxx–xxx.

    Yagmur K, van de Vijver FJR. Acculturation and language orientations of Turkish immigrants in
    Australia, France, Germany, and the Netherlands. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2011; :
    1–21. DOI: 10.1177/0022022111420145

    Yee, BWK.; Huang, LN.; Lew, A. Families: Life-span socialization in a cultural context. In: Lee, LC.;
    Zane, NWS., editors. Handbook of Asian American Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
    Publications; 1998. p. 83-135.

    Yoo HC, Gee GC, Takeuchi D. Discrimination and health among Asian American immigrants:
    Disentangling racial from language discrimination. Social Science and Medicine. 2009; 68:726–
    732. DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.11.013 [PubMed: 19095340]

    Yoshida T, Matsumoto D, Akiyama T, Moriyoshi N, Furuiye A, Ishii C. Peers’ perceptions of Japanese
    returnees. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 2003; 27(6):641–658. DOI: 10.1016/
    j.ijintrel.2003.08.005

    Yoshikawa H, Kalil A. The effects of parental undocumented status on the developmental contexts of
    young children in immigrant families. Child Development Perspectives. 2011; 5:291–297. DOI:
    10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00204.x

    Yu SM, Singh GK. High parenting aggravation among US immigrant families. American Journal of
    Public Health. 2012; 102(11):2102–2108. DOI: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300698 [PubMed:
    22994171]

    Yzerbyt V, Corneille O, Estrada C. The interplay of subjective essentialism and entitativity in the
    formation of stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2001; 5:141–155. DOI:
    10.1207/s15327957pspr0502_5

    Bornstein Page 65

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    http://www.bmfsfj.de/BMFSFJ/Ministerium/beiraete,did=9388.html

    http://www.bmfsfj.de/BMFSFJ/Ministerium/beiraete,did=9388.html

    Zane, N.; Song, A. Interpersonal effectiveness among Asian Americans: Issues of leadership, career
    advancement, and social competence. In: Leong, FTL.; Ebreo, A.; Kinoshita, L.; Inman, AG.;
    Yang, LH.; Fu, M., editors. Handbook of Asian American psychology. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA:
    SAGE; 2007. p. 283-301.

    Zhou M. Growing up American: The challenge confronting immigrant children and children of
    immigrants. Annual Review of Sociology. 1997; 23:63–95. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.soc.23.1.63

    Zhou Q, Tao A, Chen SH, Main A, Lee E, Ly J, … &, Li X. Asset and protective factors for Asian
    American children’s mental health adjustment. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6(3):312–
    319. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00251.x

    Zhou Y. The Fall of ‘The Other Half of the Sky’? Chinese Immigrant Women in the New York Area.
    Women’s Studies International Forum. 2000; 23(4):445–459. DOI: 10.1016/
    S0277-5395(00)00106-0

    Zsembik BA, Fennell D. Ethnic variation in health and the determinants of health among Latinos.
    Social Science and Medicine. 2005; 61:53–63. DOI: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.040 [PubMed:
    15847961]

    Bornstein Page 66

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

    Figure 1.

    Bornstein Page 67

    Perspect Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 January 01.
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t
    A
    u
    th
    o
    r M
    a
    n
    u
    scrip
    t

      Abstract
      Migration and Acculturation
      What this Article Aims to Do, its Context, and Limitations
      Specificity in Acculturation
      Specificity Principle: Setting Condition
      Setting Condition: Reason to Migrate
      Setting Condition: Place
      Setting Conditions: Experience and Status
      Specificity Principle: People
      Person: Gender
      Person: Personality
      Person: Individual-Difference Characteristics
      Specificity Principle: Time
      Time: Age
      Time: Penetration and Adjustment
      Time: History
      Specificity Principle: Process
      Process: Socialization
      Process: Learning
      Process: Instruction
      Process: Opportunity
      Process: Transaction and Beyond
      Specificity Principle: Domain
      Domain: Multidimensionality
      Domain: Dynamic Adaptability
      Terms of the Specificity Principle Interact
      Setting Condition x Time
      Person x Time
      Process x Time
      Domain x Time
      Is Specificity New to Acculturation?
      The Specificity Principle, Acculturation Science, and Social Policy
      Pertinence of the Specificity Principle to Acculturation Theory and Research
      Pertinence of the Specificity Principle to Acculturation Social Policy
      Conclusions
      References
      Figure 1

    What Will You Get?

    We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

    Premium Quality

    Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

    Experienced Writers

    Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

    On-Time Delivery

    Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

    24/7 Customer Support

    Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

    Complete Confidentiality

    Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

    Authentic Sources

    We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

    Moneyback Guarantee

    Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

    Order Tracking

    You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

    image

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    image

    Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

    From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

    Preferred Writer

    Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

    Grammar Check Report

    Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

    One Page Summary

    You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

    Plagiarism Report

    You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

    Free Features $66FREE

    • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
    • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
    • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
    • Paper Formatting $05FREE
    • Cover Page $05FREE
    • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
    • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
    • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
    • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
    image

    Our Services

    Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

    • On-time Delivery
    • 24/7 Order Tracking
    • Access to Authentic Sources
    Academic Writing

    We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

    Professional Editing

    We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

    Thorough Proofreading

    We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

    image

    Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

    Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

    Check Out Our Sample Work

    Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

    Categories
    All samples
    Essay (any type)
    Essay (any type)
    The Value of a Nursing Degree
    Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
    Nursing
    2
    View this sample

    It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

    Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

    0+

    Happy Clients

    0+

    Words Written This Week

    0+

    Ongoing Orders

    0%

    Customer Satisfaction Rate
    image

    Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

    We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

    See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

    image

    We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

    We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

    • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
    • Customized writing as per your needs.

    We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

    We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

    • Proactive analysis of your writing.
    • Active communication to understand requirements.
    image
    image

    We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

    We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

    • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
    • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
    Place an Order Start Chat Now
    image

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy