Textbook Readings
Kraft & Furlong: ch. 7
Add Abstract
First, I would like for you to look at the sample briefs provided in the course material as this will be extremely helpful. This shows you what the final product should look like, and what material goes into it. You MUST submit your brief in this format!!!
Next, you want to select a piece of legislation related to the topic area we are discussing.
Next, you will analyze your piece of legislations (i.e. policy).
Defining the problem: What problem existed that caused the need for this piece of legislation? Policies are usually created in response to something. Once you find your policy, this information should be included. What is the name of the policy? What is it supposed to do or fix? Who presented it?
Then you will Apply the MAY – CAN & SHOULD Analysis.
May –
Biblical guidelines: Is there anything in the bible related to this policy? What biblical guidelines did Jesus provide regarding the topic? If you find something, quote the text, cite it, and explain why you think it is related.
Constitutional Guidelines: What does the constitution say about this policy? Is it covered? If so, state it here. Be clear. Include the Article number and tell me what it is.
Can-
Political Feasibility: Where does it stand politically? Has it passed? Is it being decided? Do you think it would pass? Why or why not?
Financial Feasibility: How much does it cost? Does the cost make sense? It is worth it? Why or Why not?
Practical Feasibility – Considering why it was set up, do you think it is a practical way to solve the problem? If so why or why not?
Should –
Given the information your presented above, should this policy pass? How do you feel about it? If you support it, state why or why not. Support your position. You want to be detailed when presenting your case. Pretend you are at the round table and you want to support (or reject) this policy. You want to convince your fellow politicians to support you. Make it good. This section does not have to be long. It should be clear, concise, and effective. Does it pass the MAY Can Should Analysis?
MOST IMPORTANTLY! Read the course material and use it to help you analyze your policy. Using the course material is a requirement.
Discussion Board Graduate Rubric
Criteria |
Levels of Achievement |
|||||||||
Content (70%) |
Advanced 92-100% |
Proficient 84-91% |
Developing 1-83% |
Not Present |
Total |
|||||
Biblical Application |
16 to 17.5 points: One separate post (3-5 sentences) thoroughly discusses and applies Biblical principles such as natural law, inalienable rights, sphere sovereignty/covenant, the Sin/Crime distinction and the institutional separation of Church and State as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates a detailed understanding of ideas from required sources. |
14.5 to 15.5 points: One separate post (3-5 sentences) somewhat discusses and applies Biblical principles such as natural law, inalienable rights, sphere sovereignty/covenant, the Sin/Crime distinction and the institutional separation of Church and State as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates a general understanding of ideas from required sources. |
1 to 14 points: Separate post (3-5 sentences) often fails to discuss and apply Biblical principles such as natural law, inalienable rights, sphere sovereignty/covenant, the Sin/Crime distinction and the institutional separation of Church and State as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates an inadequate understanding of ideas from required sources. |
0 points Not present |
||||||
Constitutional Application |
16 to 17.5 points:
One separate post (3-5 sentences) thoroughly references and applies the enumerated powers, Articles and Amendments from the Constitution which are relevant to the assigned policy area as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates a detailed understanding of ideas from required sources. |
14.5 to 15.5 points:
One separate post (3-5 sentences) somewhat references and applies the enumerated powers, Articles and Amendments from the Constitution which are relevant to the assigned policy area as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates a general understanding of ideas from required sources. |
1 to 14 points: One separate post (3-5 sentences) often fails to reference and apply the enumerated powers, Articles and Amendments from the Constitution which are relevant to the assigned policy area as related to the topic of the week. Work demonstrates an inadequate understanding of ideas from required sources. |
|||||||
Structure (30%) |
||||||||||
Sources |
7.5 to 8 points: All required sources from Modules/Weeks 1–2 (must include the “Biblical Principles of Government” article), Scripture, and the required readings and presentations from the assigned module/week are cited. |
6.5 to 7 points: Most of the required sources from Modules/Weeks 1–2 (must include the “Biblical Principles of Government” article), Scripture, and the required readings and presentations from the assigned module/week are cited. |
1 to 6 points: Few of the required sources from Modules/Weeks 1–2 (must include the “Biblical Principles of Government” article), Scripture, and the required readings and presentations from the assigned module/week are cited. |
|||||||
Netiquette, Grammar, Spelling, & APA |
6.5 to 7 points: Sources are cited and listed in current APA format. Proper, netiquette, grammar, and spelling are used throughout all posts, avoids the use of texting and improper abbreviations. |
5.5 to 6 points: Sources are generally cited and listed in current APA format. For the most part, proper, netiquette, grammar, and spelling are used throughout all posts, avoids the use of texting and improper abbreviations. |
1 to 5 points: Numerous deficiencies with respect to proper APA, grammar, spelling, and netiquette. |
|||||||
Professor Comments: |
Total: |
/50 |
Running head: YOUR PAPER TITLE 1
YOUR PAPER TITLE HERE 2
Your Paper Title
Your Name
Date
Class Name and Section
Dr. Kahlib Fischer
Defining the Problem Comment by Fischer, Kahlib: One paragraph.
May
Biblical guidelines and principles
Constitutional guidelines for federal and state involvement
Can
Political Feasibility Comment by Fischer, Kahlib: One brief paragraph for each of these.
Financial feasibility
Practical feasibility
Should Comment by Fischer, Kahlib (Helms School of Government): Provide a summary of the key ideas of your analysis in support of your position. Must be based on the “May” and “Can” analysis. Offer a recommendation based upon the analysis.
References Comment by Fischer, Kahlib: List your sources in APA format below.
Running head: EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1
Effects of Age on Detection of Emotional Information
Christina M. Leclerc and Elizabeth A. Kensinger
Boston College
Author Note
This rese
arch was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BCS 0542694
awarded to Eliza
beth A. Kensinger
.
Correspo
ndence concerning this article should be addressed to Christina M. Leclerc,
Department of Psychology, Boston College, McGuinn Hall, Room 512, 140 Commonwealth
Avenue, Chestn
ut Hill, MA 02467. Email: christina.leclerc.1@bc.edu
Christina
M. Leclerc and Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Department of Psychology,
Boston College.
Author Note
arch was supported by National Science Foundation Grant BCS 0542694
beth A. Kensinger.
ndence concerning this article should be addressed to Christina M. Leclerc,
sychology, Boston College, McGuinn Hall, Room 512, 140 Commonwealth
ut Hill, MA 02467. Email: christina.leclerc.1@bc.edu
M. Leclerc and Elizabeth A. Kensinger, Department of Psychology,
Writing the abstract, 2.04
Establishing a title, 2.01; Preparing the
manuscript for submission, 8.03
Formatting the author name (byline) and
institutional affiliation, 2.02, Table 2.1
Double-spaced manuscript,
Times Roman typeface,
1-inch margins, 8.03
Elements of an author note, 2.03
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION
2
Abstract
Age differences were examined in affective processing, in the context of a visual search task
.
Young and older adults were faster to detect high arousal images compared with low arousal and
neutral items. Younger adults were faster to detect positive high arousal targets compared with
other categories. In contrast, older adults exhibited an overall detection advantage for emotional
images compared with neutral images. Together, these findings suggest that older adults do not
display valence-based effects on affective processing at relatively automatic stages.
Keywords: aging, attention, information processing, emotion, visual search
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (The numbers refer to numbered
sections in the Publication Manual.)
Paper adapted from “Effects of Age on Detection of Emotional Information,” by C. M. Leclerc and E. A. Kensinger,
2008, Psychology and Aging, 23, pp. 209–215. Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association.
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 3
Effects of Age on Detection of Emotional Information
Frequently, people encounter situations in their environment in which it is impossible to
attend to all available stimuli. It is therefore of great importance for one’s attentional processes to
select only the most salient information in the environment to which one should attend. Previous
research has suggested that emotional information is privy to attentional selection in young
adults (e.g.,
& Tapia, 2004; Nummenmaa, Hyona, & Calvo, 2006), an obvious service to evolutionary drives
to approach rewarding situations and to avoid threat and danger (Davis & Whalen, 2001; Dolan
& Vuilleumier, 2003; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997; LeDoux, 1995).
For example, Ohman, Flykt, and Esteves (2001) presented participants with 3 × 3 visual
arrays with images representing four categories (snakes, spiders, flowers, mushrooms). In half
the arrays, all nine images were from the same category, whereas in the remaining half of the
arrays, eight images were from one category and one image was from a different category (e.g.,
eight flowers and one snake). Participants were asked to indicate whether the matrix included a
discrepant stimulus. Results indicated that fear-relevant images were more quickly detected than
fear-irrelevant items, and larger search facilitation effects were observed for participants who
were fe
arful of the stimuli. A similar pattern of results has been observed when examining the
attention-grabbing nature of negative facial expressions, with threatening faces (includ ing those
not atte
nded to) identified more quickly than positive or neutral faces (Eastwood, Smilek, &
Merikl
e, 2001; Hansen & Hansen, 1988). The enhanced detection of emotional information is
not lim
ited to threatening stimuli; there is evidence that any high-arousing stimulus can be
detected rapidly, regardless of whether it is positively or negatively valenced (Anderson, 2005;
Anderson, 2005; Calvo & Lang, 2004; Carretie, Hinojosa, Marin -Loeches, Mecado,
ant stimulus. Results indicated that fearr-rr relevant images were more quickly detected than
elevant items, and larger search facilitation effects were observed for participants who aa
arful of the stimuli. A similar pattern of results has been observed when examining the
n-grabbing nature of negative facial expressions, with threatening faces (includ- ing those
nded to) identified more quickly than positive or neutral faces (Eastwood, Smilek, &
e, 2001; Hansen & Hansen, 1988). The enhanced detection of emotional information is
ited to threatening stimuli; there is evidence that any high-arousing stimulus can be
d rapidly, regardless of whether it is positively or negatively valenced (Anderson,(( 2005;55
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 4
Calvo & Lang, 2004; Carretie et al., 2004; Juth, Lundqvist, Karlsson, & Ohman, 2005;
Nummenmaa et al., 2006).
From this research, it seems clear that younger adults show detection benefits for
arousing information in the environment. It is less clear whether these effects are preserved
across the adult life span. The focus of the current research is on determining the extent to which
aging influences the early, relatively automatic detection of emotio
nal infor
mation.
Regions of the brain thought to be important for emotional detection remain relatively
intact with aging (reviewed by Chow & Cummings, 2000). Thus, it is plausible that the detection
of emotional information remains relatively stable as adults age. However, despite the
preservation of emotion-processing regions with age (or perhaps because of the contrast between
the preservation of these regions and age-related declines in cognitive-processing regions; Good
et al., 2001; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004; Ohnishi, Matsuda, Tabira, Asada, & Uno, 2001; Raz,
2000; West, 1996), recent behavioral research has revealed changes that occur with aging in the
regulation and processing of emotion. According to the socioemotional selectivity theory
(Carstensen, 1992), with aging, time is perceived as increasingly limited, and as a result, emotion
regulation becomes a primary goal (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999). According to
socioemotional selectivity theory, age is associated with an increased motivation to deri
ve
emotional meaning from life and a simultaneous decreasing motivation to expand one’s
knowledge base. As a consequence of these motivational shifts, emotional aspects of the
Writing the introduction, 2.05
Ordering citations within
the same parentheses, 6.16
Selecting
the correct
tense, 3.18
Continuity in presentation
of ideas, 3.05
Citing one
work by six
or more
authors, 6.12
No capitalization in
naming theories, 4.16
Numbers
expressed
in words,
4.32
Numbers that represent
statistical or mathematical
functions, 4.31
Use of hyphenation for
compound words, 4.13,
Table 4.1
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 5
To maintain positive affect in the face of negative age-related change (e.g., limited time
remaining, physical and cognitive decline), older adults may adopt new cognitive strategies. One
such strategy, discussed recently, is the positivity effect (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005), in which
older adults spend proportionately more time processing positive emotional material and less
time processing negative emotional material. Studies examining the influence of emotion on
memory (Charles, Mather, & Carstensen, 2003; Kennedy, Mather, & Carstensen, 2004) have
found that compared with younger adults, older adults recall proportionally more positive
information and proportionally less negative information. Similar results have been found when
examining eye-tracking patterns: Older adults looked at positive images longer than younger
adults did, even when no age differences were observed in looking time for negative stimuli
(Isaacowitz, Wadlinger, Goren, & Wilson, 2006). However, this positivity effect has not gone
uncontested; some researchers have found evidence inconsistent with the positivity effect (e.g.,
Grühn, Smith, & Baltes, 2005; Kensinger, Brierley, Medford, Growdon, & Corkin, 2002).
Based on this previously discussed research, three competing hypotheses exist to explain
age differences in emotional processing associated with the normal aging process . First,
emotional informatio
n may remain important throughout the life span, leading to similarly
facilitated detection
of emotional information in younger and older adults. Second, with aging,
emotional informatio
n may take on additional importance, resulting in older adults’ enhanced
detection of emotion
al information in their environment. Third, older adults may focus
principally on positiv
e emotional information and may show facilitated detection of positive, but
not negative,
emotional information.
The primary goal in the present experiment was to adjudicate among these alternatives.
To do so, we employed a visual search paradigm to assess young and older adults’ abilities to
motional processing associated with the normal aging process . First,
n may remain important throughout the life span, leading to similarly
of emotional information in younger and older adults. Second, with aging,
n may take on additional importance, resulting in older adults’ enhanced
al information in their environment. Third, older adults may focus
e emotional information and may show facilitated detection of positive, but
nal information.
goal in the present experiment was to adjudicate among these alternatives.
ed a visual search paradigm to assess young and older adults’ abilities to
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 6
rapidly detect emotional information. We hypothesized that on the whole, older adults would be
slower to detect information than young adults would be (consistent with Hahn, Carlson, Singer,
& Gronlund, 2006; Mather & Knight, 2006); the critical question was whether the two age
groups would show similar or divergent facilitation effects with regard to the effects of emotion
on item detection. On the basis of the existing literature, the first two previously discussed
hypotheses seemed to be more plausible than the third alternative. This is because there is reason
to think that the positivity effect may be operating only at later stages of processing (e.g.,
strategic, elaborative, and emotion regulation processes) rather than at the earlier stages
of
processing involved in the rapid detection of information (see Mather & Knight, 2005, for
discussion). Thus, the first two hypotheses, that emotional information maintains its importance
across the life span or that emotional information in general takes on greater importance with
age, seemed particularly applicable to early stages of emotional processing.
Indeed, a couple of prior studies have provided evidence for intact early processing of
emotional facial expressions with aging. Mather and Knight (2006) examined young and older
adults’ abilities to detect happy, sad, angry, or neutral faces presented in a complex visual array.
Mather and Knight found that like younger adults, older adults detected threatening faces more
quickly than they detected other types of emotional stimuli. Similarly, Hahn et al. (2006) also
found no age differences in efficiency of search time when angry faces were presented in an
array of neutral faces, compared with happy faces in neutral face displays. When angry faces,
compared with positive and neutral faces, served as nontarget distractors in the visual search
arrays, however, older adults were more efficient in searching, compared with younger adults,
Capitalization of words
beginning a sentence after
a colon, 4.14
Using the colon between
two grammatically
complete clauses, 4.05
Using the semicolon to
separate two independent
clauses not joined by
a conjunction, 4.04
Using the comma between
elements in a series, 4.03
Punctuation with citations
in parenthetical material,
6.21
Citing references in text,
inclusion of year within
paragraph, 6.11, 6.12
Hypotheses and their
correspondence to research
design, Introduction, 2.05
Prefixes and
suffixes that
do not require
hyphens,
Table 4.2
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 7
negative stimuli were not of equivalent arousal levels (fearful faces typically are more arousing
than happy faces; Hansen & Hansen, 1988). Given that arousal is thought to be a key factor in
modulating the attentional focus effect (Hansen & Hansen, 1988; Pratto & John, 1991; Reimann
& McNally, 1995), to more clearly understand emotional processing in the context of aging, it is
necessary to include both positive and negative emotional items with equal levels of arousal.
In the current research, therefore, we compared young and older adults’ detection of four
categories of emotional information (positive high arousal, positive low arousal, negative high
arousal, and negative low arousal) with their detection of neutral information. The positive and
negative stimuli were carefully matched on arousal level, and the categories of high and low
arousal were closely matched on valence to assure that the factors of valence (positive, negative)
and arousal (high, low) could be investigated independently of one another. Participants were
presented with a visual search task including images from these different categories (e.g., snakes,
cars, teapots). For half of the multi-image arrays, all of the images were of the same item, and for
the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining
items was included. Participants were asked to decide whether a different item was included in
the array, and their reaction times
were recorded for each decision. Of primary interest were
differences in response times (RTs
) based on the valence and arousal levels of the target
categories. We reasoned that if yo
ung and older adults were equally focused on emotional
information, then we would expect similar degrees of facilitation in the detection of emotional
stimuli for the two age groups. By contrast, if older adults were more affectively focused than
were younger adults, older adults
should show either faster detection speeds for all of the
emotional items (relative to the neutral items) than shown by young adults or greater facilitation
g y , g ,
single target image of a different type from the remaining
were asked to decide whether a different item was included in
were recorded for each decision. Of primary interest were
) based on the valence and arousal levels of the target
ung and older adults were equally focused on emotional
t similar degrees of facilitation in the detection of emotional
contrast, if older adults were more affectively focused than
should show either faster detection speeds for all of the
utral items) than shown by young adults or greater facilitation
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 8
for the arousing items than shown by the young adults (resulting in an interaction between age
and arousal).
Method
Participants
Younger adults (14 women, 10 men, Mage = 19.5 years, age range: 18–22 years) were
recruited with flyers posted on the Boston College campus. Older adults (15 women, nine men,
Mage = 76.1 years, age range: 68–84 years) were recruited through the Harvard Cooperative on
Aging (see Table 1, for demographics and test scores).1 Participants were compensated $10 per
hour for their participation. There were 30 additional participants, recruited in the same way as
described above, who provided pilot rating values: five young and five old participants for the
assignment of items within individual categories (i.e., images depicting cats), and 10 young and
10 old participants for the assignment of images within valence and arousal categories. All
participants were asked to bring corrective eyewear if needed, resulting in normal or corrected
to normal vision for all participants.
Materials and
Procedure
The visual search task was adapted from Ohman et al. (2001). There were 10 different
types of items (two each of five Valence × Arousal categories: positive high arousal, positive low
arousal, neutral, negative low arousal, negative high arousal), each containing nine individual
exemplars that were used to construct 3 × 3 stimulus matrices. A total of 90 images were used,
each appearing as a target and as a member of a distracting array. A total of 360 matrices were
presented to each participant; half contained a target item (i.e., eight items of one type and one
target item of another type) and half did not (i.e., all nine images of the same type). Within the
Prefixed words that
require hyphens,
Table 4.3
Using abbreviations, 4.22; Explanation
of abbreviations, 4.23; Abbreviations
used often in APA journals, 4.25;
Plurals of abbreviations, 4.29
Elements of the Method
section, 2.06; Organizing
a manuscript with levels
of heading, 3.03
Using numerals to express
numbers representing age, 4.31
Identifying
subsections
within the
Method
section, 2.06
Participant (subject)
characteristics,
Method, 2.06
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
Running head: EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 10
selected such that the arousal difference between positive low arousal and positive high arousal
was equal to the difference between negative low arousal and negative high arousal.
Similarity ratings. Each item was rated for within-category and between-categories
similarity. For within -category similarity, participants were shown a set of exemplars (e.g., a set
of mushrooms) and were asked to rate how similar each mushroom was to the rest of the
mushrooms, on a 1 (entirely dissimilar ) to 7 (nearly identical ) scale. Participants made these
ratings on the basis of overall similarity and on the basis of the specific visual dimensions in
which the objects could differ (size, shape, orientation). Participants also rated how similar
objects of one category were to objects of another category (e.g., how similar the mushrooms
were to the snakes). Items were selected to assure that the categories were equated on within-
category and between-categories similarity of specific visual dimensions as well as for the
overall similarity of the object categories (ps > .20). For example, we selected particular
mushrooms and particular cats so that the mushrooms were as similar to one another as were the
cats (i.e., within-group similarity was held constant across the categories). Our object selection
also assured that the categories differed from one another to a similar degree (e.g., that the
mushrooms were as similar to the snakes as the cats were similar to the snakes).
Procedure
Each trial began with a white fixation cross presented on a black screen for 1,000 ms; the
matrix was then presented, and it remained on the screen until a participant response was
recorded. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible with a button marked yes
if there was a target present, or a button marked no if no target was present. Response latencies
and accuracy for each trial were automatically recorded with E-Prime (Version 1.2) experimental
Running head: EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTIONRR
selected such that the arousal difference between positive low arousal and positi
was equal to the difference between negative low arousal and negative high arou
Similarity ratings. Each item was rated for within-category and between
similarity. For within -category similarity, participants were shown a set of exem
of mushrooms) and were asked to rate how similar each mushroom was to the re
mushrooms, on a 1 (entirely dissimilar ) to 7 (nearly identical(( ) scale. Participants
ratings on the basis of overall similarity and on the basis of the specific visual di
which the objects could differ (size, shape, orientation). Participants also rated h
objects of one category were to objects of another category (e.g., how similar the
were to the snakes). Items were selected to assure that the categories were equate
category and between-categories similarity of specific visual dimensions as well
overall similarity of the object categories (p(( s > .20). For example, we selected pa
h d ti l t th t th h i il t
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 9
matrix. Within the 180 target trials, each of the five emotion categories (e.g., positive high
arousal, neutral, etc.) was represented in 36 trials. Further, within each of the 36 trials for each
emotion category, nine trials were created for each of the combinations with the remaining four
other emotion categories (e.g., nine trials with eight positive high arousal items and one neutral
item). Location of the target was randomly varied such that no target within an emotion category
was presented in the same location in arrays of more than one other emotion category (i.e., a
negative high arousal target appeared in a different location when presented with positive high
arousal array images than when presented with neutral array images).
The items within each category of grayscale images shared the same verbal label (e.g.,
mushroom, snake), and the items were selected from online databases and photo clipart
packages. Each image depicted a photo of the actual object. Ten pilot participants were asked to
write down the name corresponding to each object; any object that did not consistently generate
the intended response was eliminated from the set. For the remaining images, an additional 20
pilot participants rated the emotional valence and arousal of the objects and assessed the degree
of visual similarity among objects within a set (i.e., how similar the mushrooms were to one
another) and between objects across sets (i.e., how similar the mushrooms were to the snakes).
Valence and arousal ratings . Valence and arousal were judged on 7-point scales (1 =
negative valence or low arousal and 7 = positive valence or high arousal). Negative objects
received mean valence ratings of 2.5 or lower, neutral objects received mean valence ratings of
3.5 to 4.5, and positive objects received mean valence ratings of 5.5 or higher. High arousal
objects received mean arousal ratings greater than 5, and low arousal objects (including all
neutral stimuli) received mean arousal ratings of less than 4. We selected categories for which
both young and older adults agreed on the valence and arousal classifications, and stimuli were
Latin abbreviations, 4.26
Numbers expressed in words
at beginning of sentence, 4.32
Italicization of anchors
of a scale, 4.21
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 11
software. Before beginning the actual task, participants performed 20 practice trials to assure
compliance with the task instructions.
Results
Analyses focus on participants’ RTs to the 120 trials in which a target was present and
was from a different emotional category from the distractor (e.g., RTs were not included for
arrays containing eight images of a cat and one image of a butterfly because cats and butterflies
are both positive low arousal items). RTs were analyzed for 24 trials of each target emotion
category. RTs for error trials were excluded (less than 5% of all responses) as were RTs that
were ±3 SD from each participant ’s mean (approximately 1.5% of responses). Median RTs were
then calculated for each of the five emotional target categories, collapsing across array type (see
Table 2 for raw RT values for each of the two age groups). This allowed us to examine, for
example, whether participants were faster to detect images of snakes than images of mushrooms,
regardless of the type of array in which they were presented. Because our main interest was in
examining the effects of valence and arousal on participants’ target detection times, we created
scores for each emotional target category that controlled for the participant’s RTs to detect
neutral targets (e.g., subtracting the RT to detect neutral targets from the RT to detect positive
high arousal targets). These difference scores were then examined with a 2 × 2 × 2 (Age [young,
older] × Valence [positive, negative] × Arousal [high, low]) analysis of variance (ANOVA). This
ANOVA revealed only a significant main effect of arousal, F(1, 46) = 8.41, p = .006, ηp
2 = .16,
with larger differences between neutral and high arousal images (M = 137) than between neutral
and low arousal images (M = 93; i.e., high arousal items processed more quickly across both age
groups compared with low arousal items; see Figure 1). There was no significant main effect for
valence, nor was there an interaction between valence and arousal. It is critical that the analysis
Symbols, 4.45;
Numbers, 4.31
Abbreviations
accepted as
words, 4.24
Numbering and discussing
figures in text, 5.05
Nouns followed
by numerals or
letters, 4.17
Reporting
p values,
decimal
fractions,
4.35
Statistical symbols,
4.46, Table 4.5
Elements of the
Results section, 2.07
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 12
revealed only a main effect of age but no interactions with age. Thus, the arousal-mediated
effects on detection time appeared stable in young and older adults.
The results described above suggested that there was no influence of age on the
influences of emotion. To further test the validity of this hypothesis, we submitted the RTs to the
five categories of targets to a 2 × 5 (Age [young, old] × Target Category [positive high arousal,
positive low arousal, neutral, negative low arousal, negative high arousal]) repeated measures
ANOVA.2 Both the age group, F(1, 46) = 540.32, p < .001, ηp 2 = .92, and the ta rget category,
F(4, 184) = 8.98, p < .001, ηp 2 = .16, main effects were significant, as well as the Age Group ×
Target Category interaction, F(4, 184) = 3.59, p = .008, ηp
2 = .07. This interaction appeared to
reflect the fact that for the younger adults, positive high arousal targets were detected faster than
targets from all other categories, ts(23) < –1.90,p < .001, with no other target categories
differing significantly from one another (although there were trends for negative high arousal
and negative low arousal targets to be detected more rapidly than neutral targets (p < .12). For
older adults, all emotional categories of targets were detected more rapidly than were neutral
targets, ts(23) > 2.56, p < .017, and RTs to the different emotion categories of targets did not
differ significantly from one another. Thus, these results provided some evidence that older
adults may show a broader advantage for detection of any type of emotional information,
whereas young adults’ benefit may be more narrowly restricted to only certain categories of
emotional information.
Discussion
As outlined previously, there were three plausible alternatives for young and older adults’
performance on the visual search task: The two age groups could show a similar pattern of
enhanced detection of emotional information, older adults could show a greater advantage for
Elements of the
Discussion section, 2.08
Statistics
in text, 4.44
Capitalize effects
or variables when
they appear with
multiplication
signs, 4.20
Spacing, alignment,
and punctuation of
mathematical copy, 4.46
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 13
emotional detection than young adults, or older adults could show a greater facilitation than
young adults only for the detection of positive information. The results lent some support to the
first two alternatives, but no evidence was found to support the third alternative.
In line with the first alternative, no effects of age were found when the influence of
valence and arousal on target detection times was examined; both age groups showed only an
arousal effect. This result is consistent with prior studies that indicated that arousing information
can be detected rapidly and automatically by young adults (Anderson, Christoff, Panitz, De
Rosa, & Gabrieli, 2003; Ohman & Mineka, 2001) and that older adults, like younger adults,
continue to display a threat detection advantage when searching for negative facial targets in
arrays of positive and neutral distractors (Hahn et al., 2006; Mather & Knight, 2006). Given the
relative preservatio
n of automatic processing with aging (Fleischman, Wilson, Gabrieli, Bienias,
& Bennett, 2004; Jennings & Jacoby, 1993), it makes sense that older adults would remain able
to take advantage o
f these automatic alerting systems for detecting high arousal information.
However, d
espite the similarity in arousal-mediated effects on detection between the two
age groups, the present study did provide some evidence for age-related change (specifically,
age-related enhance
ment) in the detection of emotional information. When examining RTs for
the five categories
of emotional targets, younger adults were more efficient in detecting positive
high arousal images (as presented in Table 2), whereas older adults displayed an overall
advantage for detec
ting all emotional images compared with neutral images. This pattern
suggests a broader i
nfluence of emotion on older adults’ detection of stimuli, providing support
for the hypothesis t
hat as individuals age, emotional information becomes more salient.
It is interesting that this second set of findings is clearly inconsistent with the hypothesis
that the positivity e
ffect in older adults operates at relatively automatic stages of information
nd neutral distractors (Hahn et al., 2006; Mather & Knight, 2006). Given the 66
n of automatic processing with aging (Fleischman, Wilson, Gabrieli, Bienias,
nnings & Jacoby, 1993), it makes sense that older adults would remain able 33
f these automatic alerting systems for detecting high arousal information.
espite the similarity in arousal-mediated effects on detection between the two
ent study did provide some evidence for age-related change (specifically,
ment) in the detection of emotional information. When examining RTs for
of emotional targets, younger adults were more efficient in detecting positive
(as presented in Table 2), whereas older adults displayed an overall
22
ting all emotional images compared with neutral images. This pattern
nfluence of emotion on older adults’ detection of stimuli, providing support
hat as individuals age, emotional information becomes more salient.
ng that this second set of findings is clearly inconsistent with the hypothesis
ffect in older adults operates at relatively automatic stages of information
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 14
processing, given that no effects of valence were observed in older adults’ detection speed. In the
present study, older adults were equally fast to detect positive and negative information,
consistent with prior research that indicated that older adults often attend equally to positive and
negative stimuli (Rosler et al., 2005). Although the pattern of results for the young adults has
differed across studies—in the present study and in some past research, young adults have shown
facilitated detection of positive information (e.g., Anderson, 2005; Calvo & Lang, 2004; Carretie
et al., 2004; Juth et al., 2005; Nummenmaa et al., 2006), whereas in other studies, young adults
have shown an advantage for negative information (e.g., Armony & Dolan, 2002; Hansen &
Hansen, 1988; Mogg, Bradley, de Bono, & Painter, 1997; Pratto & John, 1991; Reimann &
McNally, 1995; Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod, 1996)—what is important to note is that the
older adults detected both positive and negative stimuli at equal rates. This equivalent detection
of positive and negative information provides evidence that older adults display an advantage for
the detection of emotional information that is not valence-specific.
Thus, although younger and older adults exhibited somewhat divergent patterns of
emotional detection on a task reliant on early, relatively automatic stages of processing, we
found no evidence of an age-related positivity effect. The lack of a positivity focus in the older
adults is in keeping with the proposal (e.g., Mather & Knight, 2006) that the positivity effect
does not arise through automatic attentional influences. Rather, when this effect is observed in
older adults, it is likely due to age-related changes in emotion regulation goals that operate at
later stages of processing (i.e., during consciously controlled processing), once information has
been attended to and once the emotional nature of the stimulus has been discerned.
Although we cannot conclusively say that the current task relies strictly on automatic
processes, there are two lines of evidence suggesting that the construct examined in the current
Clear statement of support or
nonsupport of hypotheses,
Discussion, 2.08
Use of an em dash to
indicate an interruption
in the continuity of a
sentence, 4.06;
Description of an
em dash, 4.13
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 15
research examines relatively automatic processing. First, in their previous work, Ohman et al.
(2001) compared RTs with both 2 × 2 and 3 × 3 arrays. No significant RT differences based on
the number of images presented in the arrays were found. Second, in both Ohman et al.’s (2001)
study and the present study, analyses were performed to examine the influence of target location
on RT. Across both studies, and across both age groups in the current work, emotional targets
were detected more quickly than were neutral targets, regardless of their location. Together,
these findings suggest that task performance is dependent on relatively automatic detection
processes rather than on controlled search processes.
Although further work is required to gain a more complete understanding of the age-
related changes in t
he early processing of emotional information, our findings indicate that
young and older ad
ults are similar in their early detection of emotional images. The current
study provides further evidence that mechanisms associated with relatively automatic processing
of emotional image
s are well maintained throughout the latter portion of the life span
(Fleischman et al., 2004; Jennings & Jacoby, 1993; Leclerc & Hess, 2005). It is critical that,
although there is ev
idence for a positive focus in older adults’ controlled processing of emotional
information (e.g., Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Charles et al., 2003; Mather & Knight, 2005), the
present results sugg
est that the tendency to focus on the positive does not always arise when
tasks require relativ
ely automatic and rapid detection of information in the environment.
he early processing of emotional information, our findings indicate that
ults are similar in their early detection of emotional images. The current
her evidence that mechanisms associated with relatively automatic processing
s are well maintained throughout the latter portion of the life span
2004; Jennings & Jacoby, 1993;33 Leclerc & Hess, 2005). It is critical that,
idence for a positive focus in older adults’ controlled processing of emotional
Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Charles et al., 2003; Mather & Knight, 2005), the
est that the tendency to focus on the positive does not always arise when
ely automatic and rapid detection of information in the environment.
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 16
Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa , E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural
correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. Journal of Neuroscience,
23, 5627– 5633.
Armony, J. L., & Dolan , R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned
stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40 , 817–826.
doi:10.1016/S0028-3932%2801%2900178-6
Beck, A. T., Epstein , N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical
anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56 ,
893– 897. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893
Calvo, M. G., & Lang , P. J. (2004). Gaze patterns when looking at emotional pictures:
Motivationally biased attention. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 221–243. doi:
10.1023/B%3AMOEM.0000040153.26156.ed
Carretie, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Martin-Loeches, M., Mecado, F., & Tapia, M. (2004). Automatic
attention to emotional stimuli: Neural correlates. Human Brain Mapping , 22 , 290–299.
doi:10.1002/hbm.20037
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional
selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331
Carstensen, L. L., Fung , H., & Charles, S. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the
regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion , 27 , 103– 123.
References
Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 154, 258– 281. doi:10.1037/0096-
3445.134.2.258
Use of parallel construction
with coordinating conjunctions
used in pairs, 3.23
Discussion section ending
with comments on
importance of findings, 2.08
Construction of an accurate and
complete reference list, 6.22;
General desciption of references, 2.11
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 17
Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging
and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 , 117–121.
doi:
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x
Charles, S. T., Mather, M. , & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The
forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 132, 310– 324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310
Chow, T. W., & Cummings, J. L. (2000). The amygdala and Alzheimer’s disease. In J. P.
Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: A functional analysis (pp. 656– 680). Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
Davis, M., & Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry ,
6, 13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812
Dolan, R. J., & Vuilleumier, P. (2003). Amygdala automaticity in emotional processing. Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 348–355.
Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Negative facial expression captures
attention and disrupts performance. Perceptual Physiology, 65, 352–358.
Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Bienias , J. L., & Bennett , D. A. (2004). A
longitudinal study of implicit and explicit memory in old persons. Psychology and Aging,
19, 617–625. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.617
Good , C. D., Johnsrude , I. S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R. N. A., Firston , K. J., & Frackowiak, R.
S. J. (2001). A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human
brains. NeuroImage, 14 , 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786
ather, M. , & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: The.
e nat
y: G
Cum
(Ed.
niver
alen
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 18
Grühn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring memory for positive
material: Evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally
toned words. Psychology and Aging, 20, 579–588. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579
Hahn, S., Carlson, C., Singer, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2006). Aging and visual search: Automatic
and controlled attentional bias to threat faces. Acta Psychologica, 123 , 312–336. doi:
10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.01.008
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen , R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority
effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 917–924. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.54.6.917
Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive
neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 87–96. doi:10.1038/nrn1323
Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson , H. R. (2006). Selective preference in
visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychology
of Aging, 21 , 40– 48. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40
Jennings, J. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1993). Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging,
attention, and control. Psychology and Aging , 8, 283–293. doi:10.1037/0882-
7974.8.2.283
Juth, P., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., & Ohman, A. (2005). Looking for foes and friends:
Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd. Emotion, 5, 379– 395.
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379
Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related
positive bias in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15 , 208–214.
doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x
Davis, M., & Wha
6, 13–33 34. d
Dolan, R. J., & Vu
of the New
Eastwood, J. D., S
attention an
Fleischman, D. A.
longitudina
19, 617–77 62
Good , C. D., John
S. J. (2001).
brains. Neu
alen
doi:
uille
w Yo
Smil
nd d
, W
al st
25. d
srud
). A
uroI
Hahn, S., Carlson, C., Singer, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2006). Aging and visual search: Automa
and controlled attentional bias to threat faces. Acta Psychologica, 123 , 312–22 336. doi:
10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.01.008
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen , R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority
effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54, 917–77 924. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.54.6.917
Hedden, T., & Gabrieli,ii J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive
neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 87–77 96. doi:10.1038/nrn1323
Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson , H. R. (2006). Selective preference.
visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-ee tracking study. Psycholo
of Aging, 21 , 40–00 48. doi:10.1037/0882-22 7974.21.1.40
Jennings, J. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1993). Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging,.
attention, and control. Psychology and Aging , 8, 283–33 293. doi:10.1037/0882-
7974.8.2.283
Juth, P., Lundqvist, D., Karlsson, A., & Ohman, A. (2005). Looking for foes and friends:
Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd. Emotion, 5, 379–99 39
doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379
Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age. -relate
positive bias in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15 , 208–88 214.
doi:10.1111/j.0956-66 7976.2004.01503011.x
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 19
Kensinger, E. A., Brierley, B., Medford , N., Growdon, J. H., & Corkin, S. (2002). Effects of
normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on emotional memory. Emotion, 2, 118–
10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.118
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. , & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation,
and action. In P. J. Lang , R. F. Simons, & M. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting:
Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97– 135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leclerc, C. M., & Hess, T. M. (2005, August). Age differences in processing of affectively
primed information. Poster session presented at the 113th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46 , 209–
235. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233
Mather, M ., & Knight, M. (2005). Goal-directed memory: The role of cognitive control in older
adults’ emotional memory. Psychology and Aging, 20, 554–570. doi:10.1037/0882-
7974.20.4.554
Mather, M. , & Knight, M. R. (2006). Angry faces get noticed quickly: Threat detection is not
impaired among older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences,
61B, P54–P57.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., de Bono , J., & Painter, M. (1997). Time course of attentional bias for
threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behavioral Research Therapy, 35 , 297– 303.
Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified Wisconsin card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.
Cortex, 12 , 313–324.
Example of reference to a
book chapter, print verison,
no DOI, 7.02, Example 25
Digital object identifier as
article identifier, 6.31;
Example of reference to a
periodical, 7.01
134. doi:
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 20
Nummenmaa, L., Hyona , J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective
attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6, 257–268. doi:10.1037/1528-
3542.6.2.257
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the
grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037/0096-
3445.130.3.466
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module
of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037/003
3-
295X.108.3.483
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda , H., Tabira, T., Asada, T., & Uno, M. (2001). Changes in brain morphology
in Alzheimer’s disease and exaggerated aging process? American Journal of
Neuroradiology, 22 , 1680–1685.
Petrides , M. , & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject -ordered tasks after frontal- and tempor
al-
lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20 , 249–262. doi: 10.1016/0028-
3932%2882%2990100-2
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative
social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 380–391. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of
structural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), Handbo
ok
of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–90). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reimann, B., & McNally, R. (1995). Cognitive processing of personally relevant information.
Cognition and Emotion, 9, 324–340.
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in
7/0096
d modu
3-
orphol
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 21
Rosler, A., Ulrich, C., Billino, J., Sterzer, P., Weidauer , S., Bernhardt, T., …Kleinschmidt, A.
(2005). Effects of arousing emotional scenes on the distribution of visuospatial attention:
Changes with aging and early subcortical vascular dementia. Journal of the Neurological
Sciences,229, 109–116. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2004.11.007
Shipley, W. C. (1986).Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
Spielberger, C. D.,
Gorsuch, I., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State–Trait Inventory.
Palo Alto,
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale— Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997
). Technical manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence and Memory Scale–
III. New Yo
rk, NY: The Psychological Corporation.
West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging.
Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272
Williams, J. M., Mathews , A., & MacLeod , C., (1996). The emotional Stroop task and
psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3– 24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess , P. W., Emslie, H. C., & Evans , J. J. (1996). The
Behavioura
l Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Flempton, Bury St. Edmunds,
England: T
hames Valley Test Company.
al-
ve
ok
.
empora
negati
doi:
of
andboo
mation.
Gorsuch, I., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State–Trait Inventory.
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
). Wechsler Memory Scaler — Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
.
). Technical manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence and Memory Scale–
rk, NY: The Psychological Corporation.
An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging.
cal Bulletin, 120, 272–22 292. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272
athews , A., & MacLeod , C., (1996). The emotional Stroop task and
ology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–33 24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3
erman, N., Burgess , P. W., Emslie,ee H. C., & Evans , J. J. (1996). The
l Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Flempton, Bury St. Edmunds,
hames Valley Test Company.
22
Footnotes
covariance were conducted with these covariates, with no resulting
influences of these variables on the pattern or magnitude of the results.
2 These data were also analyzed with a 2 × 5 ANOVA to examine the effect of target
category when presented only in arrays containing neutral images, with the results remaining
qualitatively the same. More broadly, the effects of emotion on target detection were not
qualitatively impacted by the distractor category.
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION
Analyses of 1
Article with more than
seven authors, 7.01,
Example 2
Placement and format
of footnotes, 2.12
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
24
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION
Note. Values represent median response times, collapsing across array type and excluding arrays
of the same category as targets (i.e., positive high arousal represents the median RT to respond to
positiv
e high arousal targets, collapsing across positive low arousal, neutral, negative high
arousal, and negative low arousal array categories). The median response time values were
record
ed in milliseconds.
Table 2
Raw R
esponse Time (RT) Scores for Young and Older Adults
Category Young group Older group
Positive high arousal 825 1,580
Positive low arousal 899 1,636
Neutral 912 1,797
Negative high arousal 885 1,578
Negative low arousal 896 1,625
24
CTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION
Values represent median response times, collapsing across array type and excluding arrays
same category as targets (i.e., positive high arousal represents the median RT to respond to
e high arousal targets, collapsing across positive low arousal, neutral, negative high
, and negative low arousal array categories). The median response time values were
ed in milliseconds.
2
esponse Time (RT) Scores for Young and Older Adults
oryy Young groupg g p Older groupg p
ve high arousal 825 1,580
ve low arousal 899 1,636
al 912 1,797
ive high arousal 885 1,578
ive low arousal 896 1,625
23EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION
Note. The Beck Anxiety Inventory is from Beck et al. (1988); the Behavioral Assessment of the
Dysexecutive Syndrome—Dysexecutive Questionnaire (BADS–DEX) is from Wilson et al.
(1996); the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) measures are from Spielberger et al. (1970);
and the Digit Symbol Substitution, Digit Span–Backward, and Arithmetic Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale—III and Wechsler Memory Scale—III measures are from Wechsler (1997).
Generative naming scores represent the total number of words produced in 60 s each for letter
F, A, and S. The Vocabulary measure is from Shipley (1986); the Mental Control measure is
from Wechsler (1987); the Self-Ordered Pointing measure was adapted from Petrides and Milner
(1982); and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) measure is from Nelson (1976).
Table 1
Participant Characteristics
Younger group Older group
Measure M SD M SD F (1, 46) p
Years of education 13.92 1.28 16.33 2.43 18.62 <.001
Beck Anxiety Inventory 9.39 5.34 6.25 6.06 3.54 .066
BADS– DEX 20.79 7.58 13.38 8.29 10.46 .002
STAI–State 45.79 4.44 47.08 3.48 1.07 .306
STAI–Trait 45.64 4.50 45.58 3.15 0.02 .963
Digit Symbol Substitution 49.62 7.18 31.58 6.56 77.52 <.001
Generative naming 46.95 9.70 47.17 12.98 .004 .951
Vocabulary 33.00 3.52 35.25 3.70 4.33 .043
Digit Span– Backward 8.81 2.09 8.25 2.15 0.78 .383
Arithmetic 16.14 2.75 14.96 3.11 1.84 .182
Mental Control 32.32 3.82 23.75 5.13 40.60 <.001
Self-Ordered Pointing 1.73 2.53 9.25 9.40 13.18 .001
WCST perseverative errors 0.36 0.66 1.83 3.23 4.39 .042
All values represent raw, nonstandardized scores.
Selecting effective
presentation, 4.41;
Logical and effective
table layout, 5.08
Elements of
table notes, 5.16
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 25
.
Figure 1. Mean difference values (ms) representing detection speed for each target category
subtracted from the mean detection speed for neutral targets. No age differences were found in the
arousal-mediated effects on detection speed. Standard errors are represented in the figure by the
error bars attached to each column.
Figure legends
and captions, 5.23
Principles of figure use and
construction, types of figures;
standards, planning, and
preparation of figures, 5.20–5.25
Figure 2.1. Sample One-Experiment Paper (continued)
INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY 1
Inhibitory Influences on Asychrony as a Cue for Auditory Segregation
Auditory grouping involves the formation of auditory objects from the sound mixture
reaching the ears. The cues used to integrate or segregate these sounds and so form auditory
objects have been defined by several authors (e.g., Bregman, 1990; Darwin, 1997; Darwin &
Carlyon, 1995). The key acoustic cues for segregating concurrent acoustic elements are
differences in onset time (e.g., Dannenbring & Bregman, 1978; Rasch, 1978) and harmonic
relations (e.g., Brunstrom & Roberts, 1998; Moore, Glasberg, & Peters, 1986). In an example of
the importance of onset time, Darwin (1984a, 1984b) showed that increasing the level of a
harmonic near the first formant (F1) frequency by adding a synchronous pure tone changes the
phonetic quality of a vowel. However, when the added tone began a few hundred milliseconds
before the vowel, it was essentially removed from the vowel percept.… [section
c
ontinues].
General Method
Overview
In the experiments reported here, we used a paradigm developed by Darwin to assess the
perceptual integration of additional energy in the F1 region of a vowel through its effect on
phonetic quality (Darwin, 1984a, 1984b; Darwin & Sutherland, 1984).…[section
continues].
Stimuli
Amplitude and phase values for the vowel harmonics were obtained from the vocal-tract
transfer function using cascaded formant resonators (Klatt, 1980). F1 values varied in 10-Hz
steps from 360–550 Hz—except in Experiment 3, which used values from 350– 540 Hz—to
produce a continuum of 20 tokens.…[section continues].
Listeners
Elements of empirical studies, 1.01
Figure 2.2. Sample Two-Experiment Paper (The numbers refer to num-
bered sections in the Publication Manual. This abridged manu-
script illustrates the organizational structure characteristic of
multiple-experiment papers. Of course, a complete multiple-
experiment paper would include a title page, an abstract page,
and so forth.)
Paper adapted from “Inhibitory Influences on Asychrony as a Cue for Auditory Segregation,” by S. D.
Holmes and B. Roberts, 2006, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32,
pp. 1231–1242. Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association.
INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY 2
Listeners were volunteers recruited from the student population of the University of
Birmingham and were paid for their participation. All listeners were native speakers of British
English who reported normal hearing and had successfully completed a screening procedure
(described below). For each experiment, the data for 12 listeners are presented.…[section
continues].
Procedure
At the start of each session, listeners took part in a warm-up block. Depending on the
number of conditions in a particular experiment, the warm-up block consisted of one block of all
the experimental stimuli or every second or fourth F1 step in that block. This gave between 85
and 100 randomized trials. … [section continues].
Data Analysis
The data for each listener consisted of the number of /I/ responses out of 10 repetitions
for each nominal F1 value in each condition. An estimate of the F1 frequency at the phoneme
boundary was obtained by fitting a probit function (Finney, 1971) to a listener ’s identification
data for each condition. The phoneme boundary was defined as the mean of the probit function
(the 50% point).…[section continues].
Experiment 1
In this exper
iment, we used noise-band captors and compared their efficacy with that of a
pure-tone captor. Each noise-band captor had the same energy as that of the corresponding pure –
tone captor and a ce
nter frequency equal to the frequency of this tonal captor…[section
continues].
Method
pe e t
iment, we used noise-band captors and compared their efficacy with that of a
ach noise-band captor had the same energy as that of the corresponding pure- –
nter frequency equal to the frequency of this tonal captor…[section
INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY 3
There were nine conditions: the three standard ones (vowel alone, incremented fourth,
and leading fourth) plus three captor conditions and their controls. A lead time of 240 ms was
used for
Results and Discussion
Figure 4 shows the mean phoneme boundaries for all conditions and the restoration effect
for each captor type. The restoration effects are shown above the histogram bars both as a
boundary shift in hertz and as a percentage of the difference in boundary position between the
incremented-fourth and leading-fourth conditions.… [section continues].
Experiment 2
This experiment considers the case where the added 500-Hz tone begins at the same time
as the vowel but continues after the vowel ends.… [section continues].
Method
There were five conditions: two of the standard ones (vowel alone and incremented
fourth), a lagging-fourth condition (analogous to the leading-fourth condition used elsewhere),
and a captor condition and its control. A lag time of 240 ms was used for the added 500-Hz
tone.… [section continues]
Results and Discussion
the added 500-Hz tone.… [section continues].
Policy on metrication, 4.39;
Style for metric units, 4.40
Abbreviating units
of measurement,
4.27, Table 4.4
Plural forms of nouns
of foreign origin, 3.19
Multiple Experiments, 2.09
Figure 2.2. Sample Two-Experiment Paper (continued)
Running head: INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY 5
a perceptual group between the leading portion and the captor tone, on the basis of their common
onset time and harmonic relationship, leaving the remainder of the extra energy to integrate into
the vowel percept… .[section continues].
[Follow the form of the one-experiment sample paper to type references, the author note,
footnotes , tables, and figure captions.]
Running head: INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY
a perceptual group between the leading portion and the captor tone, on the basis o
INHIBITORY INFLUENCES ON ASYCHRONY 4
1984; Roberts & Holmes, 2006). This experiment used a gap between captor offset and vowel
onset to measure the decay time of the captor effect …[section continues].
Method
There were 17 conditions: the three standard ones (vowel alone, incremented fourth, and
leading fourth), five captor conditions and their controls, and four additional conditions
(described separately below). A lead time of 320 ms was used for the added 500-Hz tone. The
captor conditions were created by adding a 1.1-kHz pure-tone captor, of various durations, to
each member of the leading-fourth continuum.…[section continues].
Results
Figure 6 shows the mean phoneme boundaries for all conditions. There was a highly
significant effect of condition on the phoneme boundary values, F(16, 176) = 39.10, p < .001.
Incrementing the level of the fourth harmonic lowered the phoneme boundary relative to the
vowel-alone condition (by 58 Hz, p < .001), which indicates that the extra energy was integrated
into the vowel percept.…[section continues].
Discussion
The results of this experiment show that the effect of the captor disappears somewhere
between 80 and 160 ms after captor offset. This indicates that the captor effect takes quite a long
time to decay away relative to the time constants typically found for cells in the CN using
physiological measures (e.g., Needham & Paolini, 2003).…[section continues].
Summary and Concluding Discussion
Darwin and Sutherland (1984) first demonstrated that accompanying the leading portion
of additional energy in the F1 region of a vowel with a captor tone partly reversed the effect of
the onset asynchrony on perceived vowel quality. This finding was attributed to the formation of
Use of statistical term rather
than symbol in text, 4.45
Figure 2.2. Sample Two-Experiment Paper (continued)
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 1
The Sleeper Effect in Persuasion:
A Meta-Analytic Review
Persuasive messages are often accompanied by information that induces suspicions of
invalidity. For instance, recipients of communications about a political candidate may discount a
message coming from a representative of the opponent party because they do not perceive the
source of the message as credible (e.g., Lariscy & Tinkham, 1999). Because the source of the
political message serves as a discounting cue and temporarily decreases the impact of the
message, recipients may not be persuaded by the advocacy immediately after they receive the
communication. Over time, however, recipients of an otherwise influential message may recall
the message but not the noncredible source and thus become more persu aded by the message at
that time than they were immediately following the communication. The term sleeper effect was
used to denote such
a delayed increase in persuasion observed when the discounting cue (e.g.,
noncredible source)
becomes unavailable or “dissociated” from the communication in the
memory of the mes
sage recipients (Hovland, Lumsdaine, & Sheffield, 1949).…[section
Method
Sample of Studies
We retrieve
d reports related to the sleeper effect that were available by March 2003 by
means of multiple p
rocedures. First, we searched computerized databases, including PsycINFO
(1887–2003), Dissertation Abstracts International (1861– 2003), ERIC (1967–2003), and the
Social-Science-Citation -Index (1956–2003), using the keywords sleeper effect, delayed-action,
credibility, source c
redibility, source expertise, attitude change, discounting cue, attitude
persistence, attitude maintenance, persuasion, propaganda, attitude and memory, attitude and
a delayed increase in persuasion observed when the discounting cue (e.g.,
becomes unavailable or “dissociated” from the communication in the
sage recipients (Hovland, Lumsdaine, & Sheffield, 1949).…[section
Method
d reports related to the sleeper effect that were available by March 2003 by
rocedures. First, we searched computerized databases, including PsycINFO
rtation Abstracts International (1861– 2003), ERIC (1967–77 2003), and the
tion -Index (1956–66 2003), using the keywords sleeper effect,tt delayed-action,
redibility, source expertise, attitude change, discounting cue, attitude
maintenance, persuasion, propaganda, attitude and memoryrr , attitude and
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 2
retention, attitude and decay, and persuasion and decay . Because researchers often use the terms
opinion and belief, instead of attitude , we conducted searches using these substitute terms as
well.
Second, … [section continues].
Selection Criteria
We used the following criteria to select studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis .
1. We only included studies that involved the presentation of a communication containing
persuasive arguments. Thus, we excluded studies in which the participants played a role or were
asked to make a speech that contradicted their opinions. We also excluded developmental studies
involving delayed effects of an early event (e.g., child abuse), which sometimes are also referred
to as sleeper effects .…[section continues] .
Moderators
For descriptive purposes, we recorded (a) the year and (b) source (i.e., journal article,
unpublished dissertations and theses , or other unpublished document) of each report as well as
(c) the sample composition (i.e., high-school students, university students, or other) and (d) the
country in which the study was conducted.
We also coded each experiment in terms of .…[section continues].
Studies were coded independently by the first author and another graduate student.
Italicize key terms, 4.21
Description of meta-analysis, 1.02;
Guidelines for reporting meta-analysis,
2.10; see also Appendix
Identification of elements in a
series within a sentence, 3.04
Figure 2.3. Sample Meta-Analysis (The numbers refer to numbered sec-
tions in the Publication Manual. This abridged manuscript illus-
trates the organizational structure characteristic of reports of
meta-analyses. Of course, a complete meta-analysis would
include a title page, an abstract page, and so forth.)
Paper adapted from “The Sleeper Effect in Persuasion: A Meta-Analytic Review,” by G. Kumkale and D.
Albarracin, 2004, Psychological Bulletin, 130, pp. 143–172. Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological
Association.
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 3
was satisfactory (Orwin, 1994). We resolved disagreements by discussion and consultation with
colleagues. Characteristics of the individual studies included in this review are presented in
Table 1. The studies often contained several independent datasets such as different messages and
different experiments. The characteristics that distinguish different datasets within a report
appear on the second column of the table.
Dependent Measures and Computation of Effect Sizes
We calculated effect sizes for (a) persuasion and (b) recall–recognition of the message
content. Calculations were based on the data described in the primary reports as well as available
responses of the authors to requests of further information .…[section continues].
Analyses of Effect Sizes
There are two major models used in meta-analysis: fixed-effects and random-
effects.…[section continues].
To benefit from the strengths of both models, we chose to aggregate the effect sizes and to
conduct analyses us
ing both approaches.…[section continues].
Results
The data analysis included a description of the experiments we summarized, an
estimation of overal
l effects, moderator analyses, and tests of mediation.
Sample of Studies
and Datasets
Descriptive characteristics of the datasets included in the present meta-analysis appear in
Table 2.…[section continues].
Overview of the A
verage Effect Sizes
A thorough
understanding of the sleeper effect requires examining (a) the between-
condition differenc
es at each time point as well as (b) the within-condition changes that take
Analyses of Effect Sizes
wo major models used in meta-aa analysis: fixed-effects and s random-
ontinues].
e strengths of both models, we chose to aggregate the effect sizes and to
ing both approaches.…[section continues].
Results
alysis included a description of the experiments we summarized, an
l effects, moderator analyses, and tests of mediation.
and Datasets
characteristics of the datasets included in the present meta-aa analysis appear in
continues].
verage Effect Sizes
understanding of the sleeper effect requires examining (a) the between-
es at each time point as well as (b) the within-condition changes that take
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 4
place over time.…[section continues].
In light of these requirements, we first examined whether discounting cues led to a decrease in
agreement with the communication (boomerang effect). Next,.…[section continues].
Ruling out a nonpersisting boomerang effect. To determine whether or not a delayed
increase in persuasion represents an absolute sleeper effect, one needs to rule out a nonpersisting
boomerang effect, which takes place when a message initially backfires but later loses this
reverse effect (see panel A of Figure 1).…[section continues].
Average sleeper effect. Relevant statistics corresponding to average changes in
persuasion from the immediate to the delayed posttest appear in Table 4, organized by the
different conditions we considered (i.e., acceptance-cue, discounting-cue, no-message control,
and message-only control). In Table 4, positive effect sizes indicate increases in persuasion over
time, negative effect sizes indicate decay in persuasion, and zero effects denote stability in
persuasion. Confidence intervals that do not include zero indicate significant changes over time.
The first row of Table 4 shows that recipients of acceptance cues agreed with the message less as
time went by (fixed-effects, d + = –0.21; random-effects, d+ = –0.23). In contrast to the decay in
persuasion for recipients of acceptance cues, there was a slight increase in persuasion for
recipients of discounting cues over time (d+ = 0.08). It is i mportant to note that change in
discounting-cue conditions significantly differed from change in acceptance-cue conditions,
(fixed-effects; B = –0.29, SE = 0.04), QB(1) = 58.15, p < .0001; QE(123) = 193.82, p <
.0001.…[section continues].
Summary and variability of the overall effect. The overall analyses identified a relative
sleeper effect in persuasion, but no absolute sleeper effect. The latter was not surprising, because
the sleeper effect was expected to emerge under specific conditions.…[section continues].
Use at least
two subheadings
in a section, 3.02
Figure 2.3. Sample Meta-Analysis (continued)
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 5
Moderator Analyses
Although overall effects have descriptive value, the variability in the change observed in
discounting-cue conditions makes it unlikely that the same effect was present under all
conditions. Therefore, we tested the hypotheses that the sleeper effect would be more likely (e.g.,
more consistent with the absolute pattern in Panel B1 of Figure 1) when…[section continues].
THE SLEEPER EFFECT IN PERSUASION 6
. . . [references continue]
[Follow the form of the one-experiment sample paper to type the author note, footnotes,
tables, and figure captions.]
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
Albarracín, D. (2002). Cognition in persuasion: An analysis of information processing in
response to persuasive communications. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 61–130). doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80004-1
… [references continue]
Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Effects of involvement in persuasion: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin, 106, 290–314. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.290
*Johnson, H. H., Torcivia, J. M., & Poprick, M. A. (1968). Effects of source credibility on the
relationship between authoritarianism and attitude change. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 9, 179–183. doi:10.1037/h0021250
*Johnson, H. H., & Watkins, T. A. (1971). The effects of message repetitions on immediate and
delayed attitude change. Psychonomic Science, 22, 101–103.
Jonas, K., Diehl, M., & Bromer, P. (1997). Effects of attitudinal ambivalence on information
processing and attitude-intention consistency. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 33, 190–210. doi:10.1006/jesp.1996.1317
Format for references included
in a meta-analysis with less
than 50 references, 6.26
Figure 2.3. Sample Meta-Analysis (continued)
Biblical Principles of Government:
Should Government Be Involved?
YES NO
Is this an
issue of
injustice?
Inalienable Rights:
Does the injustice
equate to a violation
of life, liberty, and
property?
YES NO
Government
should be
involved.
Government
should not be
involved.
How can other
spheres in society be
involved to help solve
the problem?
Government
should not be
involved.
TheUnited States Constitution
September 17, 1787
____________________
It quickly became apparent that the Articles of Confederation, ratified by all the states by
March 1781, was insufficient in several areas (Lowman, pp. 121-22). One of the main weaknesses
was that it had no means of enforcing laws, or to settle disputes arising out of national laws. This
placed the states in the position of being independent nations (Lowman, p. 122). The states had no
rights with one another that were easily protected, and neither did their citizens. Shays’ Rebellion,
which occurred in Massachusetts in 1786, magnified this problem and was the event that caused
the founding fathers to discuss plans for a better system of government:
Shays’ Rebellion was limited to Massachusetts, but it threw fear into the hearts of Americans
in general. It rudely awakened them to the truly desperate political and economic conditions
in America. George Washington, in a letter to John Jay, wrote that “our affairs are
drawing rapidly to a crisis. We have errors to correct; we have probably had too good an
opinion of human nature in forming our Confederation. Experience has taught us that
men will not adopt, and carry into execution, measures the best calculated for their own
good, without the intervention of coercive power. I do not conceive we can exist long as a
nation without lodging, somewhere, a power which will pervade the whole Union in as
energetic a manner as the authority of the state governments extends over the several states
[Emphasis added.] (Lowman, p. 124).
A convention was called to revise the Articles of Confederation, but under the leadership of George
Washington, the delegates pushed for a more ambitious plan: creating an entirely new system of
government:
The Convention had been called only for the purpose of revising the Articles of
Confederation. But most of the delegates realized from the beginning of their discussions
that this was not enough to solve the nation’s pressing problems. What was needed was a
new and stronger national government. Since whatever action they took would only result in
a recommendation to the states and would not be binding on anyone, they made the bold
decision to put aside the Articles and draft a brand new Constitution for the United States.
In making the “Great Decision,” they heeded the advice of George Washington, who is
reported to have told the delegates even before the Convention officially began: “It is too
probable that no plan we propose will be adopted. Perhaps another dreadful conflict is to
be sustained. If to please the people, we offer what we ourselves disapprove, how can we
afterwards defend our works? Let us raise a standard to which the wise and honest can
repair. The event is in the hands of God” (Lowman, p. 126).
And so the delegates created and successfully pushed for ratification of the Constitution. The
United States Constitution can be looked at as the culmination of many historical trends, which,
throughout the centuries, led to an understanding of a Biblical framework upon which government
should operate. The Constitution includes references to separation of powers, due process of
law, rule by consent, rule by law, rule by justice, protection of inalienable rights, and
federalism, among other things. Furthermore, it was based upon an understanding of covenantal
principles. Before the Constitution was ratified, the states were practically in a state of nature
[defined by Locke as a situation in which no government existed to ensure basic rights among
various parties; see Section III] with one another, since the Articles of Confederation were so weak.
The Constitution was a means by which the people of America, as one nation, could come together
to ensure that their rights were protected.
____________________
We, the people of the United States, in order to form
a more perfect Union, establish justice, insure
domestic tranquillity, provide for the common
defense, promote the g eneral welfare, and secure
the blessings of liberty to ourselves and our posterity,
do ordain and establish this
Constitution for the United States of America.
Section 1.
[Legislative powers; in whom vested.]
All legislative powers herein granted shall be vested in a Congress of the United States, which shall
consist of a Senate and House of Representatives.
Section 2.
[House of Representatives, how and by whom chosen Qualifications of a Representative.
Representatives and direct taxes, how apportioned. Enumeration. Vacancies to be filled.
Power of choosing officers, and of impeachment.]
1. The House of Representatives shall be composed of members chosen every second year by the
people of the several States, and the elector in each State shall have the qualifications requisite for
electors of the most numerous branch of the State Legislature.
These concepts shouldn’t be confused with an
endorsement of a welfare program, since
socialism wasn’t an idea the Founding Fathers
supported or were even aware of. These terms
refer more to ensuring safety and order. They
cannot be interpreted apart from the
enumerated powers discussed in the proceeding
sections.
2. No person shall be a Representative who shall not have attained the age of twenty-five years, and
been seven years a citizen of the United States, and who shall not, when elected, be an inhabitant of
that State in which he shall be chosen.
3. Representatives [and direct taxes] [Altered by 16th Amendment] shall be apportioned among
the several States which may be included within this Union, according to their respective numbers,
[which shall be determined by adding the whole number of free persons, including those bound to
service for a term of years, and excluding Indians not taxed, three-fifths of all other persons.]
[Altered by 14th Amendment] The actual enumeration shall be made within three years after the
first meeting of the Congress of the United States, and within every subsequent term of ten years, in
such manner as they shall by law direct. The number of Representatives shall not exceed one for
every thirty thousand, but each State shall have at least one Representative; and until such
enumeration shall be made, the State of New Hampshire shall be entitled to choose three,
Massachusetts eight, Rhode Island and Providence Plantations one, Connecticut five, New York six,
New Jersey four, Pennsylvania eight, Delaware one, Maryland six, Virginia ten, North Carolina five,
South Carolina five, and Georgia three.
4. When vacancies happen in the representation from any State, the Executive Authority thereof
shall issue writs of election to fill such vacancies.
5. The House of Representatives shall choose their Speaker and other officers; and shall have the
sole power of impeachment.
Section 3.
[Senators, how and by whom chosen. How classified. State Executive, when to make
temporary appointments, in case, etc. Qualifications of a Senator. President of the Senate,
his right to vote. President pro tem, and other officers of the Senate, how chosen. Power to
try impeachments. When President is tried, Chief Justice to preside. Sentence.]
1. The Senate of the United States shall be composed
of two Senators from ea ch State, [chosen by the
Legislature thereof,] [Altered by 17th Amendment]
for six years; and each Senator shall have one vote.
2. Immediately after they shall be assembled in
consequence of the first election, they shall be divided
as equally as may be into three classes. The seats of the
Senators of the first class shall be vacated at the expiration of the second year, of the second class at
the expiration of the fourth year, and of the third class at the expiration of the sixth year, so that
one-third may be chosen every second year; [and if vacancies happen by resignation, or otherwise,
during the recess of the Legislature of any State, the Executive thereof may make temporary
appointments until the next meeting of the Legislature, which shall then fill such vacancies.]
[Altered by 17th Amendment].
3. No person shall be a Senator who shall not have attained to the age of thirty years, and been nine
In keeping with the concept of federalism and
States’ Rights, the Founders intended that the
State legislators choose the Senators so that
the concerns of each state as a unique
political entity would be acknowledged and
protected. This was changed in the
Amendment.
years a citizen of the United States, and who shall not, when elected, be an inhabitant of that State
for which he shall be chosen.
4. The Vice-President of the United States shall be President of the Senate, but shall have no vote,
unless they be equally divided.
5. The Senate shall choose their other officers, and also a President pro tempore, in the absence of the
Vice President, or when he shall exercise the office of the President of the United States.
6. The Senate shall have the sole power to try all impeachments. When sitting for that purpose,
they shall be on oath or affirmation. When the President of the United States is tried, the Chief
Justice shall preside: and no person shall be convicted without the concurrence of two-thirds of the
members present.
7. Judgment in cases of impeachment shall not extend further than to removal from office, and
disqualification to hold and enjoy any office of honor, trust, or profit under the United States: but
the party convicted shall nevertheless be liable and subject to indictment, trial, judgment and
punishment, according to law.
Section 4.
[Times, etc., of holding elections, how prescribed. One session in each year.]
1. The times, places and manner of holding elections for Senators and Representatives, shall be
prescribed in each State by the Legislature thereof; but the Congress may at any time by law make or
alter such regulations, except as to the places of choosing Senators.
2. The Congress shall assemble at least once in every year, and such meeting shall be [on the first
Monday in December,] [Altered by 20th Amendment] unless they by law appoint a different day.
Section 5.
[Membership, Quorum, Adjournments, Rules, Power to punish or expel. Journal. Time of
adjournments, how limited, etc.]
1. Each House shall be the judge of the elections, returns and qualifications of its own members,
and a majority of each shall constitute a quorum to do business; but a smaller number may adjourn
from day to day, and may be authorized to compel the attendance of absent members, in such
manner, and under such penalties as each House may provide.
2. Each House may determine the rules of its proceedings, punish its members for disorderly
behavior, and, with the concurrence of two-thirds, expel a member.
3. Each House shall keep a journal of its proceedings, and from time to time publish the same,
excepting such parts as may in their judgment require secrecy; and the yeas and nays of the members
of either House on any question shall, at the desire of one-fifth of those present, be entered on the
journal.
4. Neither House, during the session of Congress, shall, without the consent of the other, adjourn
for more than three days, nor to any other place than that in which the two Houses shall be sitting.
Section 6.
[Compensation, Privileges, Disqualification in certain cases.]
1. The Senators and Representatives shall receive a compensation for their services, to be
ascertained by law, and paid out of the Treasury of the United States. They shall in all cases, except
treason, felony and breach of the peace, be privileged from arrest during their attendance at the
session of their respective Houses, and in going to and returning from the same; and for any speech
or debate in either House, they shall not be questioned in any other place.
2. No Senator or Representative shall, during the time for which he was elected, be appointed to
any civil office under the authority of the United States, which shall have increased during such time;
and no person holding any office under the United States, shall be a member of either House during
his continuance in office.
Section 7.
[House to originate all revenue bills. Veto. Bill may be passed by two-thirds of each
House, notwithstanding, etc. Bill, not returned in ten days to become a law. Provisions as
to orders, concurrent resolutions, etc.]
1. All bills for raising revenue shall originate in the House of Representatives; but the Senate may
propose or concur with amendments as on other bills.
2. Every bill which shall have passed the House of Representatives and the Senate, shall, before it
become a law, be presented to the president of the United States; if he approve, he shall sign it, but
if not, he shall return it, with his objections, to that house in which it shall have originated, who shall
enter the objections at large on their journal, and proceed to reconsider it. If after such
reconsideration, two thirds of that house shall agree to pass the bill, it shall be sent, together with the
objections, to the other house, by which it shall likewise be reconsidered, and if approved by two-
thirds of that house, it shall become a law. But in all such cases the votes of both houses shall be
determined by yeas and nays, and the names of the persons voting for and against the bill shall be
entered on the journal of each house respectively. If any bill shall not be returned by the president
within ten days (Sundays excepted) after it shall have been presented to him, the same shall be a law,
in like manner as if he had signed it, unless the Congress by their adjournment prevent its return, in
which case it shall not be a law.
3. Every order, resolution, or vote to which the concurrence of the Senate and House of
Representatives may be necessary (except on a question of adjournment) shall be presented to the
president of the United States; and before the same shall take effect, shall be approved by him, or,
being disapproved by him, shall be re-passed by two-thirds of the Senate and House of
Representatives, according to the rules and limitations prescribed in the case of a bill.
Section 8.
[Powers of Congress.]
The Congress shall have the power
1. to lay and collect taxes, duties, imposts and excises,
to pay the debts and provide for the common defense
and g eneral welfare of the United States; but all
duties, imposts and excises shall be uniform
throughout the United States:
2. To borrow money on the credit of the United States:
3. To regulate commerce with foreign nations, and among the several states, and with the Indian
tribes:
4. To establish an uniform rule of naturalization, and uniform laws on the subject of bankruptcies
throughout the United States:
5. To coin money, regulate the value thereof, and of foreign coin, and fix the standard of weights
and measures:
6. To provide for the punishment of counterfeiting the securities and current coin of the United
States:
7. To establish post-offices and post-roads:
8. To promote the progress of science and useful arts, by securing for limited times to authors and
inventors the exclusive right to their respective writings and discoveries:
9. To constitute tribunals inferior to the supreme court:
10. To define and punish piracies and felonies committed on the high seas, and offenses against the
law of nations:
11. To declare war, grant letters of marque and reprisal, and make rules concerning captures on land
and water:
12. To raise and support armies, but no appropriation of money to that use shall be for a longer
term than two years:
13. To provide and maintain a navy:
14. To make rules for the government and regulation of the land and naval forces:
Here again we have a general overview of the
powers of Congress. The point is that the general
goal of government is order and safety. The powers
listed below explain the reasons for which Congress
can assign taxes.
15. To provide for calling forth the militia to execute the laws of the union, suppress insurrections
and repel invasions:
16. To provide for organizing, arming and disciplining the militia, and for governing such part of
them as may be employed in the service of the United States, reserving to the states respectively, the
appointment of the officers, and the authority of training the militia according to the discipline
prescribed by Congress:
17. To exercise exclusive legislation in all cases whatsoever, over such district (not exceeding ten
miles square) as may, by cession of particular states, and the acceptance of Congress, become the
seat of the government of the United States, and to exercise like authority over all places purchased
by the consent of the legislature of the state in which the same shall be, for the erection of forts,
magazines, arsenals, dock-yards, and other needful buildings: And,
18. To make all laws which shall be necessary and
proper for carrying into execution the foreg oing
powers, and all other powers vested by this constitution
in the government of the United States, or in any
department or officer thereof.
Section 9.
[Provision as to migration or importation of certain persons. H abeas Corpus, Bills of
attainder, etc. Taxes, how apportioned. No export duty. No commercial preference.
Money, how drawn from Treasury, etc. No titular nobility. Officers not to receive presents,
etc.]
1. The migration or importation of such persons as any of the states now existing shall think proper
to admit, shall not be prohibited by the Congress prior to the year 1808, but a tax or duty may be
imposed on such importations, not exceeding 10 dollars for each person.
2. The privilege of the writ of habeas corpus shall not be suspended, unless when in cases of rebellion
or invasion the public safety may require it.
3. No bill of attainder or ex post facto law shall be passed.
4. [No capitation, or other direct tax shall be laid unless in proportion to the census or enumeration
herein before directed to be taken.] [Altered by 16th Amendment]
5. No tax or duty shall be laid on articles exported from any state.
6. No preference shall be given by any regulation of commerce or revenue to the ports of one state
over those of another: nor shall vessels bound to, or from one state, be obliged to enter, clear, or
pay duties in another.
This is known as the Necessary and Proper
clause. It does not exist in a vacuum and can
only be in effect as a means of carrying out one
of the enumerated powers above. Thus, it is
actually very limited, even though it is often used
to justify any number of Congressional over-
reaches.
7. No money shall be drawn from the treasury but in consequence of appropriations made by law;
and a regular statement and account of the receipts and expenditures of all public money shall be
published from time to time.
8. No title of nobility shall be granted by the United States: And no person holding any office or
profit or trust under them, shall, without the consent of the Congress, accept of any present,
emolument, office, or title, of any kind whatever, from any king, prince, or foreign state.
Section 10.
[States prohibited from the exercise of certain powers.]
1. No state shall enter into any treaty, alliance, or confederation; grant letters of marque and reprisal;
coin money; emit bills of credit; make any thing but gold and silver coin a tender in payment of
debts; pass any bill of attainder, ex post facto law, or law impairing the obligation of contracts, or grant
any title of nobility.
2. No state shall, without the consent of the Congress, lay any imposts or duties on imports or
exports, except what may be absolutely necessary for executing its inspection laws; and the net
produce of all duties and imposts, laid by any state on imports or exports, shall be for the use of the
treasury of the United States; and all such laws shall be subject to the revision and control of the
Congress.
3. No state shall, without the consent of Congress, lay any duty of tonnage, keep troops, or ships of
war in time of peace, enter into any agreement or compact with another state, or with a foreign
power, or engage in a war, unless actually invaded, or in such imminent danger as will not admit of
delay.
Section 1.
[President: his term of office. Electors of President; number and how appointed. Electors
to vote on same day. Qualification of President. On whom his duties devolve in case of his
removal, death, etc. President’s compensation. His oath of office.]
1. The Executive power shall be vested in a President of the United States of America. He shall
hold office during the term of four years, and together with the Vice President, chosen for the same
term, be elected as follows
2. [Each State] [Altered by 23rd Amendment] shall appoint, in such manner as the Legislature
may direct, a number of electors, equal to the whole number of Senators and Representatives to
which the State may be entitled in the Congress: but no Senator or Representative, or person
holding an office of trust or profit under the United States, shall be appointed an elector [The
electors shall meet in their respective States, and vote by ballot for two persons, of whom one at
least shall not be an inhabitant of the same State with themselves. And they shall make a list of all
the persons voted for each; which list they shall sign and certify, and transmit sealed to the seat of
Government of the United States, directed to the President of the Senate. The President of the
Senate shall, in the presence of the Senate and House of Representatives, open all the certificates,
and the votes shall then be counted. The person having the greatest number of votes shall be the
President, if such number be a majority of the whole number of electors appointed; and if there be
more than one who have such majority, and have an equal number of votes, then the House of
Representatives shall immediately choose by ballot one of them for President; and if no person have
a majority, then from the five highest on the list the said House shall in like manner choose the
President. But in choosing the President, the votes shall be taken by States, the representation from
each State having one vote; a quorum for this purpose shall consist of a member or members from
two-thirds of the States, and a majority of all the States shall be necessary to a choice. In every case,
after the choice of the President, the person having the greatest number of votes of the electors shall
be the Vice President. But if there should remain two or more who have equal votes, the Senate
shall choose from them by ballot the Vice President.] [Altered by 12th Amendment]
3. The Congress may determine the time of choosing the electors, and the day on which they shall
give their votes; which day shall be the same throughout the United States.
4. No person except a natural born citizen, or a citizen of the United States, at the time of the
adoption of this Constitution, shall be eligible to the office of President; neither shall any person be
eligible to that office who shall not have attained to the age of thirty-five years, and been fourteen
years a resident within the United States.
5. [In case of the removal of the President from office, or of his death, resignation, or inability to
discharge the powers and duties of the said office, the same shall devolve on the Vice President, and
the Congress may by law provide for the case of removal, death, resignation, or inability, both of the
President and Vice President, declaring what officer shall then act as President, and such officer shall
act accordingly, until the disability be removed, or a President shall be elected.] [Altered by 25th
Amendment].
6. The President shall, at stated times, receive for his services, a compensation, which shall neither
be increased nor diminished during the period for which he shall have been elected, and he shall not
receive within that period any other emolument from the United States, or any of them.
7. Before he enter on the execution of his office, he shall take the following oath or affirmation:
I do solemnly swear (or affirm) that I will faithfully execute the office
of the President of the United States, and will to the best of my ability,
preserve, protect and defend the Constitution of the United States.
Section 2.
[President to be Commander-in-Chief. He may require opinions of cabinet officers, etc.,
may pardon. Treaty-making power. Nomination of certain officers. When President may
fill vacancies.]
1. The President shall be Commander-in-Chief of the Army and Navy of the United States, and of
the militia of the several States, when called into the actual service of the United States; he may
require the opinion, in writing, of the principal officer in each of the executive departments, upon
any subject relating to the duties of their respective offices, and he shall have power to grant
reprieves and pardons for offenses against the United States, except in cases of impeachment.
2. He shall have power, by and with the advice and consent of the Senate, to make treaties,
provided two-thirds of the Senators present concur; and he shall nominate, and by and with the
advice and consent of the Senate, shall appoint ambassadors, other public ministers and consuls,
judges of the Supreme Court, and all other officers of the United States, whose appointments are
not herein otherwise provided for, and which shall be established by law: but the Congress may by
law vest the appointment of such inferior officers, as they think proper, in the President alone, in the
courts of law, or in the heads of departments.
3. The President shall have the power to fill up all vacancies that may happen during the recess of
the Senate, by granting commissions, which shall expire at the end of their next session.
Section 3.
[President shall communicate to Congress. He may convene and adjourn Congress, in case
of disagreement, etc. Shall receive ambassadors, execute laws, and commission officers.]
He shall, from time to time give to the Congress information of the state of the Union, and
recommend to their consideration such measures as he shall judge necessary and expedient; he may,
on extraordinary occasions, convene both Houses, or either of them, and in case of disagreement
between them, with respect to the time of adjournment, he may adjourn them to such time as he
shall think proper; he may receive ambassadors, and other public ministers; he shall take care that
the laws be faithfully executed, and shall commission all the officers of the United States.
Section4.
[All civil offices forfeited for certain crimes.]
The President, Vice President, and all civil officers of the United States, shall be removed from
office on impeachment for, and conviction of, treason, bribery, or other high crimes and
misdemeanors.
Section 1.
[Judicial powers. Tenure. Compensation.]
The judicial power of the United States, shall be vested in one supreme court, and in such inferior
courts as the Congress may, from time to time, ordain and establish. The judges, both of the
supreme and inferior courts, shall hold their offices during good behaviour, and shall, at stated
times, receive for their services a compensation, which shall not be diminished during their
continuance in office.
Section 2.
[Judicial power; to what cases it extends. Original jurisdiction of Supreme Court Appellate.
Trial by Jury, etc.]
1. The judicial power shall extend to all cases, in law and equity, arising under this constitution, the
laws of the United States, and treaties made, or which shall be made under their authority; to all
cases affecting ambassadors, other public ministers and consuls; to all cases of admiralty and
maritime jurisdiction; to controversies to which the United States shall be a party; [to controversies
between two or more states, between a state and citizens of another state, between citizens of
different states, between citizens of the same state, claiming lands under grants of different states,
and between a state, or the citizens thereof, and foreign states, citizens or subjects.] [Altered by 11th
Amendment]
2. In all cases affecting ambassadors, other public ministers and consuls, and those in which a state
shall be a party, the supreme court shall have original jurisdiction. In all the other cases before-
mentioned, the supreme court shall have appellate jurisdiction, both as to law and fact, with such
exceptions, and under such regulations as the Congress shall make.
3. The trial of all crimes, except in cases of impeachment, shall be by jury; and such trial shall be
held in the state where the said crimes shall have been committed; but when not committed within
any state, the trial shall be at such place or places as the Congress may by law have directed.
Section 3.
[Treason defined. Proof of Punishment.]
1. Treason against the United States shall consist only in levying war against them, or in adhering to
their enemies, giving them aid and comfort. No person shall be convicted of treason unless on the
testimony of two witnesses to the same overt act, or on confession in open court.
2. The Congress shall have power to declare the punishment of treason, but no attainder of treason
shall work corruption of blood, or forfeiture, except during the life of the person attained.
Section 1.
[Each State to give credit to the public acts, etc. of every other State.]
Full faith and credit shall be given in each state to the public acts, records and judicial proceedings of
every other state. And the Congress may by general laws prescribe the manner in which such acts,
records and proceedings shall be proved, and the effect thereof.
Section 2.
[Privileges of citizens of each State. Fugitives from Justice to be delivered up. Persons held
to service having escaped, to be delivered up.]
1. The citizens of each state shall be entitled to all privileges and immunities of citizens in the
several states. [See the 14th Amendment].
2. A person charged in any state with treason, felony, or other crime, who shall flee justice, and be
found in another state, shall, on demand of the executive authority of the state from which he fled,
be delivered up, to be removed to the state having jurisdiction of the crime.
3. [No person held to service or labour in one state, under the laws thereof, escaping into
another, shall, in consequence of any law or regulation therein, be discharged from such service or
labour, but shall be delivered up on claim of the party to whom such service or labour may be due.]
[Altered by 13th Amendment.]
Section 3.
[Admission of new States. Power of Congress over territory and other property.]
1. New states may be admitted by the Congress into this union; but no new state shall be formed or
erected within the jurisdiction of any other state, nor any state be formed by the junction of two or
more states, without the consent of the legislatures of the states concerned, as well as of the
Congress.
2. The Congress shall have power to dispose of and make all needful rules and regulations
respecting the territory or other property belonging to the United States; and nothing in this
constitution shall be so construed as to prejudice any claims of the United States, or of any particular
state.
Section 4.
[Republican form of government guaranteed. Each State to be protected.]
The United States shall guarantee to every state in this union, a republican form of government, and
shall protect each of them against invasion; and on application of the legislature, or of the executive
(when the legislature cannot be convened), against domestic violence.
[Amendments.]
The Congress, whenever two-thirds of both houses shall deem it necessary, shall propose
amendments to this constitution, or on the application of the legislatures of two-thirds of the several
states, shall call a convention for proposing amendments, which, in either case, shall be valid to all
intents and purposes, as part of this constitution, when ratified by the legislatures of three-fourths of
the several states, or by conventions in three-fourths thereof, as the one or the other mode of
ratification may be proposed by the Congress: Provided, that no amendment which may be made
prior to the year 1808, shall in any manner affect the first and fourth clauses in the ninth section of
the first article; and that no state, without its consent, shall be deprived of its equal suffrage in the
Senate.
[Constitution as the supreme law of the land.]
1. All debts contracted and engagements entered into, before the adoption of this constitution, shall
be as valid against the United States under this constitution, as under the confederation.
2. This constitution, and the laws of the United States which shall be made in pursuance thereof;
and all treaties made, or which shall be made, under the authority of the United States shall be the
supreme law of the land; and the judges in every state shall be bound thereby, any thing in the
constitution or laws of any state to the contrary notwithstanding.
3. The senators and representatives before-mentioned, and the members of the several state
legislatures, and all executive and judicial officers, both of the United States and of the several states,
shall be bound by oath or affirmation, to support this constitution; but no religious test shall ever be
required as a qualification to any office or public trust under the United States.
[Ratification.]
The ratification of the conventions of nine states, shall be sufficient for the establishment of this
constitution between the states so ratifying the same.
____________________
www.constitution.org/usconsti.htm
The Constitution Society,
(www.constitution.org/default.htm)
Accessed March 18, 1998.
____________________
December 15, 1791
____________________
Ratifying the Bill of Rights was an important step in assuring that the Constitution itself
was ratified and supported by the American people. The Bill of Rights was a culmination of a
tradition of specifically enumerated rights. The Massachusetts Body of Liberties (1641); the
English Bill of Rights (1689); the Virginia Declaration of Rights (1776); and the Virginia
Statute of Religious Freedom were documents which influenced the people’s call for an American
bill of rights (Hicks, p. 236).
One of the key proponents in the call for a bill of rights was James Madison:
James Madison finally won ratification of the Constitution in Virginia by gaining the support
of the numerous Baptists in that state. He won their confidence in the Constitution by
incorporating a plea for a bill of rights within Virginia’s ratification and by promising to
propose a bill of rights at his earliest convenience if elected to Congress. Virginia’s
convention ratified the Constitution on June 25, 1788, and the voters subsequently sent
Madison to the House of Representatives. Madison proposed the Bill of Rights in
Congress on September 25, 1789, and ten of the twelve amendments that he proposed
became the first ten amendments to the Constitution when they were ratified by the states
by December 15, 1791 [Emphasis added.] (Hicks, p. 236).
The Bill of Rights makes specific provisions for upholding justice and rule of law, while at the
same time stipulating that it was not the sole source of the American people’s rights.
____________________
[Liberty of conscience and of press.]
Congress shall make no law respecting an establishment of religion, or prohibiting the free exercise
thereof; or abridging the freedom of speech, or of the press; or the right of the people peaceably to
assemble, and to petition the Government for a redress of grievances.
[Right to bear arms.]
A well-regulated militia, being necessary to the security of a free State, the right of the people to
keep and bear arms, shall not be infringed.
[Quartering of soldiers in private houses forbidden without consent.]
No soldier shall, in time of peace be quartered in any house, without the consent of the owner, nor
in time of war, but in a manner to be prescribed by law.
[Searches and seizures must be done lawfully.]
The right of the people to be secure in their persons, houses, papers, and effects, against
unreasonable searches and seizures, shall not be violated, and no warrants shall issue, but upon
probable cause, supported by oath or affirmation, and particularly describing the place to be
searched, and the persons or things to be seized.
[Criminal proceedings must be just.]
No person shall be held to answer for a capital, or otherwise infamous crime, unless on a
presentment or indictment of a Grand Jury, except in cases arising in the land or naval forces, or in
the militia, when in actual service in time of war or public danger; nor shall any person be subject for
the same offense to be twice put in jeopardy of life or limb; nor shall be compelled in any criminal
case to be a witness against himself, nor be deprived of life, liberty, or property, without due process
of law; nor shall private property be taken for public use without just compensation.
[Criminal proceedings must be just.]
In all criminal prosecutions, the accused shall enjoy the right to a speedy and public trial, by an
impartial jury of the State and district wherein the crime shall have been committed, which district
shall have been previously ascertained by law, and to be informed of the nature and cause of the
accusation; to be confronted with the witnesses against him; to have compulsory process for
obtaining witnesses in his favor, and to have the assistance of counsel for his defense.
[Trial by Jury.]
In suits at common law, where the value in controversy shall exceed twenty dollars, the right of trial
by jury shall be preserved, and no fact tried by a jury shall be otherwise reexamined in any court of
the United States, than according to the rules of the common law.
[Excessive bail and fines and cruel and unusual punishment are prohibited.]
Excessive bail shall not be required, nor excessive fines imposed, nor cruel and unusual punishments
inflicted.
[Protection of unenumerated rights ensured.]
The enumeration in the Constitution, of certain rights, shall not be construed to deny or disparage
others retained by the people.
[States’ rights.]
The powers not delegated to the United States by the Constitution, nor prohibited by it to the
States, are reserved to the States respectively, or to the people.
____________________
www.constitution.org/usconsti.htm
The Constitution Society
(www.constitution.org/default.htm)
[Internet]
Accessed March 18, 1998.
____________________
PREAMBLE
ARTICLE I
Section 1.
Section 2.
Section 3.
Section 4.
Section 5.
Section 6.
Section 7.
Section 8.
Section 9.
Section 10.
ARTICLE II
Section 1.
Section 2.
Section 3.
Section4.
ARTICLE III
Section 1.
Section 2.
Section 3.
ARTICLE IV
Section 1.
Section 2.
Section 3.
Section 4.
ARTICLE V
ARTICLE VI
ARTICLE VII
The Bill of Rights of the United States of America
AMENDMENT I
AMENDMENT II
AMENDMENT III
AMENDMENT IV
AMENDMENT V
AMENDMENT VI
AMENDMENT VII
AMENDMENT VIII
AMENDMENT IX
AMENDMENT X
The Congressional Budget Office provides financial estimates of proposed bills. It is recommend
that you start with this site because it provides you with a summary of the bill and provides
financial analysis that you will need for your “Financial Feasibility” study.
You can use its search feature to find bills you would be interested in using for your policy brief.
From the main page, scroll down to the “Find Analyses” section and click the “Cost Estimates” link:
Use the search tool in the upper left hand corner to put in your search topic. It can be a specific bill,
or a general issue, like “abortion”.
Note that the search tool only works on a year-by-year basis, so if you do not know exactly what are
looking for, you will need to use the drop down menu to select different years. The CBO site
contains analyses as far back as 1998.
FINDING LEGISLATION
http://www.cbo.gov/
CONGRESS.GOV, provided by the Library of Congress: allows you to type in a key word (such as
abortion) and it will list any bills that are dealing with that issue and where it is in the approval
process.
OTHER RESOURCES
Access the Government Databases page for
Lexis-Nexis Legal Search (provides federal and state case law)
ProQuest Congressional
Many other policy related sites
Government Research Guide
GovEngine: a privately run but free web site directory to the governments of all 50 states, including
their legislatures. From there students can find state laws & statutes.
https://www.congress.gov/
https://www.congress.gov/
http://www.liberty.edu/newlibrary/databases/?s=16
http://libguides.liberty.edu/content.php?pid=548824&sid=4519481
http://www.govengine.com/
We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.
Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.
Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.
Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.
Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.
Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.
We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.
Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.
You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.
Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.
Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.
You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.
You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.
Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.
We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.
We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.
We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.
Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!
Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality
Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.
We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.
We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.
We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.
We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.