Discussion 1: Schemas

 Most everyone has seen criminal trials (both fictitious and real) on TV. You know, then, that the trial starts with opening statements and ends with closing arguments by the prosecution and defense, respectively.

For this Discussion, you will examine the advantages and disadvantages of schemas.

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
Discussion 1: Schemas
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

To Prepare

  • Review the Learning Resources for this week and consider the advantages and disadvantages of the schema.
  • Imagine you are a juror. Exactly how do opening statements and closing arguments function as schemas? In particular, what impact would opening statements have on how you, as a juror, would interpret evidence proffered at trial?

 Post how your preconceptions (i.e., your schema) might supersede the schema (i.e., theory) of the case as presented by attorneys for the state and for the defendant during opening statements. How might the strength of your preconceptions cause you to reject the attorneys’ attempt to provide a schema for the case?  

 

During jury selection, attorneys for the prosecution and defense question prospective jurors to determine who may already have decided the defendant’s guilt or innocence before hearing the attorneys present the case. As an attorney, it is your job to expose and then counter juror preconceptions that may make them resistant to your schema for the case. Your class colleagues have described how their preconceptions might override the schema you, as an attorney, would advance during opening statements. Describe the strategy you might use to persuade the resistant juror (i.e., the class colleague you are responding to) to set aside her or his preconceptions and consider what you have to say.

Be sure to support your postings and responses with specific references to the social psychology theory and research. In addition to the Learning Resources, search the Walden Library and/or Internet for peer-reviewed articles to support your post and responses. Use proper APA format and citations, including those in the Learning Resources.

 https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ghNlOsy5DBQ

Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., Akert, R. M., & Sommers, S. R. (Eds.). (2019). Social psychology (10th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.

  • Chapter 3, “Social Cognition: How We Think About the Social World” 

Standard Form Contracts and
Contract Schemas: A
Preliminary Investigation of the
Effects of Exculpatory Clauses
on Consumers’ Propensity to Sue

Dennis P. Stolle, M.A., J.D.,1* and
Andrew J. Slain, B.S.1*

This study investigated the extent to which exculpatory

clauses deter consumers from pursuing their legal rights.

Undergraduate participants (N=101) were presented with

two written vignettes and asked to imagine themselves as a

consumer harmed by a contracted for service.

Participants

then read a contract and responded to questions assessing

their likelihood of seeking compensation and their

perceptions of the contract. The presence of exculpatory

clauses, the severity of the harm, and the nature of the

harm were varied. The data suggest that exculpatory

clauses, if read, have a deterrent effect on propensity to

seek compensation. Development of a psychological

definition of contract schemas and implications for legal

policy are discussed. # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Behav. Sci. Law Vol. 15, 83±94 1997.

No. of Figures: 0. No. of Tables: 1. No. of Refs: 27.

In 1963, Stewart Macaulay asked a few empirical questions about contract law:

“What good is contract law? who uses it? when and how?” (p. 55). Using survey

and interview methodologies, Macaulay set out to answer those questions.

Interestingly, Macaulay found that formal contract doctrine often takes a back

seat to extra-legal conceptions of fair dealing and “common honesty and decency”

(p. 58). Macaulay’s heavy reliance on empirical data was viewed by some legal

scholars as contributing to the demise of traditional contract theory and doctrine.

CCC 0735±3936/97/010083±12$17.50
#1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Behavioral Sciences and the Law, Vol. 15, 83±94 (1997) RESEARCH REPORT

* Correspondence to: either author at 1University of Nebraska-Lincoln, 209 Burnett Hall, Lincoln,
Nebraska 68588-0308 USA. Electronic mail may be directed to the first author at
dstolle@unlgrad1.unl.edu or the second author at aslain@unlgrad1.unl.edu.

Dennis P. Stolle, M.A., J.D. (expected May 1997), and Andrew J. Slain, B.A., are J.D.-Ph.D. students
in the Law/Psychology Program at the University Nebraska-Lincoln. The authors are grateful to
professors Mark Fondacaro, Steven Penrod, Alan Tomkins, and Robert Works for their helpful
suggestions and comments, and Michelle Dreesen for her assistance in collecting and coding the data. A
version of this article was presented at the biennial conference of the American Psychology-Law Society,
Hilton Head, South Carolina, February, 1996.

This research was made possible in part by a University of Nebraska-Lincoln Warden Research Grant
awarded to the first author. This article was completed while the first author was in receipt of a National
Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Fellowship. The financial support of NIMH is gratefully
acknowledged.

Macaulay was even dubbed the “Lord High Executioner of the Contract is Dead

School” (Gilmore, 1974, p. 105). Nonetheless, Macaulay’s findings did not toll the

death knell for contract doctrine and theory. Rather, they offered a new and more

empirically accurate way of conceptualizing contracts.

In the years since Macaulay’s initial work, Law and Society researchers have

continued to offer empirical views of contractual behavior that challenge the

assumptions of traditional contract doctrine (Macaulay, 1985). Although the Law

and Society research has produced findings relevant to both legal and sociolegal

scholars, the research has not emphasized inquiry into the implicit psychology of

contract. Furthermore, the Social Science in Law movement, which places a strong

emphasis on using psychological theory to “sharpen the legal scholar’s insights”

(Monahan & Walker, 1993, p. v), has been largely absent from the empirical

contract literature. Perhaps the most psychologically sophisticated studies of

contractual behaviors are those studies investigating the importance of the

“psychological contract” in employment contexts (e.g., Morrison & Robinson, in

press; Robinson, in press; Robinson & Rousseau, 1994; Rousseau, 1989; Rousseau

& Aquino, 1993; Schmedemann & McLean Parks, 1994). However, most of the

employment studies emphasize the management implications of the psychological

contract over the legal implications.

Altogether there exists little more than a handful of empirical studies exploring

the psycholegal dimensions of contract (e.g., Masson & Waldron, 1994). Indeed,

numerous legal scholars have noted the need for sophisticated empirical research

and the application of psychological theory in the context of contract law

(Eisenberg, 1995; Harrison, 1994; Hasen, 1990; MacNeil, 1985; Rubin, 1995;

Speidel, 1995; Stratman, 1988; White, 1988). As the domain of contract widens

and shifts, it has become clear that to truly understand the nature of contract,

“empirical theories dealing with the use and abuse of contract behavior in the

shadow of contract law and beyond will be required” (Speidel, 1995, p. 255). The

purpose of the present study was to provide a preliminary attempt at exploring

empirical theories of contract from a psycholegal perspective.

THE PRESENCE OF EXCULPATORY CLAUSES
IN FORM CONTRACTS

The vast majority of consumer transactions are conducted via standardized

agreements presented to consumers on preprinted forms with little or no

opportunity for the consumer to negotiate the terms of the agreement. The

widespread use of standard form contracts is largely a result of their efficiency

(Farnsworth, 1990). Because the costs involved in negotiating individual contracts

would often exceed the potential profit arising from many routine transactions,

standard form contracts are an essential element of modern commercial life

(Farnsworth, 1990).

Unfortunately, traditional contract doctrine, which contemplates a bargained-for

exchange between parties of relatively equal power, often falls short of providing an

adequate analytic framework for resolving disputes involving standard form

contracts (Farnsworth, 1991). Form contracts seldom involve parties of equal

bargaining power, they are typically offered on a take-it-or-leave-it basis, and the

84 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

terms embodied in such forms sometimes seek to alter the base-line legal rights of

the consumer (Eisenberg, 1995). For example, it is not uncommon for form

contracts to include terms that, if enforced, would relieve the drafting party of

liability for their own negligence. The enforceability of such terms, commonly

referred to as exculpatory clauses, is often unclear (Morant, 1995). Many

jurisdictions have held these types of disclaimers unenforceable because the

terms are either unconscionable, in violation of public policy, or are beyond

the range of the consumer’s reasonable expectations (Morant, 1995). Although the

enforceability of exculpatory clauses is often suspect, they remain commonplace in

consumer form contracts.

Some commentators have suggested that the knowing inclusion of unenforceable

disclaimers is unethical because it creates a facade of legality that may deter

consumers from bringing otherwise legitimate claims against the drafting party

(Kuklin, 1988; Vukowich, 1993). In response to such concerns, the discussion

draft of the Model Rules of Professional Conduct contained a provision prohibiting

lawyers from drafting agreements containing “legally prohibited terms,” or terms

that “would be held to be unconscionable as a matter of law” (Vukowich, 1993,

p. 776). However, strong objections from the legal community to a rule that would

penalize the inclusion of overly broad disclaimers caused the rule to be eliminated,

leaving the current Model Rules without any reference to a lawyer’s responsibility

as a contract drafter (Vukowich, 1993). At least part of the reasoning behind the

objections was that a rule restricting the terms that a lawyer may include in a

contract would infringe upon the lawyer’s ability to fully protect his client’s

interests under a broad range of possible contingencies.

The current state of the common law is such that the inclusion of exculpatory

clauses is prohibited only under limited circumstances. Furthermore, the

enforceability of exculpatory clauses varies greatly between jurisdictions, and as a

function of the specific circumstances, leaving the consumer with little ability to

predict whether any particular disclaimer clause will be enforceable (Morant,

1995). Of course, the extent to which exculpatory clauses actually deter consumers

from bringing legal actions is an empirical question. If such terms do have an effect

on consumers’ propensity to sue, it seems likely that those effects may interact with

other variables, such as the type and severity of the harm suffered by the consumer.

If such terms in fact have no effect on consumers’ propensity to pursue their legal

rights, then some of the concern regarding the presence of such clauses in consumer

form contracts may be unwarranted.

CONTRACT SCHEMAS AND EXCULPATORY CLAUSES

In the present context, a discussion of the effects of exculpatory language on

consumer’s propensity to sue is largely a discussion of “expectations and their

effects” (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 97). Such expectations may form the basis of

cognitive schemasГgeneric knowledge that holds across many particular

instances” (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 98). Indeed, researchers have found support

for the notion of contract schemas in the employment context (Schmedemann &

McLean Parks, 1994; Morrison & Robinson, in press). Like employees, consumers

Standard form contracts 85

may base some of their expectations about contractual transactions upon cognitive

schemas.

Schmedemann and McLean Parks suggest that contract schemas may include

such attributes as legal jargon and the presence of a signature block (1994). Early

empirical work on contract disclaimers suggests that consumers’ contract schemas

may also include a belief that terms in a written contract are generally enforceable.

In 1970, Warren Mueller conducted a study to examine tenant perceptions of

residential leases (1970). Mueller found that most participants in his study believed

that the exculpatory clauses presented in the mock lease would be enforceable and

that there would be little variation between states in terms of enforceability (1970).

These beliefs were erroneous. In fact, Mueller had presented his participants with

clauses unlikely to be upheld in the state where the study was conducted (1970).

Furthermore, at the time the study was conducted, there was wide variability

among states in the enforceability of the clauses presented.

Although Mueller’s study was not intended as an investigation of contract

schemas, his data suggests that, at least in 1970, consumers held a general belief

that provisions contained in the residential leases they sign are enforceable.

However, Mueller’s study suffers from several limitations. Mueller’s participants

responded on a dichotomous, yes/no, scale; consequently, Mueller had no

measurement of the magnitude of his participants’ beliefs. Furthermore,

Mueller’s study was conducted in a survey format with no experimental

manipulations, and Mueller’s participants were not presented with circumstances

in which the exculpatory language would be relevant. Previous schema research

suggests that increasing the costs of being wrong may decrease reliance on schemas

and increase the desire to seek out new data (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Consequently,

a remaining question is whether participants placed in a relevant context in which

an exculpatory clause would have particularly detrimental effects on their

immediate future would abandon their contract schema and seek out new

information. In this context, methods of seeking out relevant information might

include talking to a representative of the drafting party or talking to a lawyer.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were 101 undergraduate students recruited from psychology courses at

a mid-western university. Most participants volunteered in exchange for class

credit. The final sample consisted of 51% women and 45% men; 4% of the

participants failed to indicate their gender. Participants’ mean age was 21, and the

median age was 19.

Materials

Vignettes

Two separate vignettes were utilized, one involving a personal injury and the other

involving damage to personal property. Both vignettes asked participants to

86 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

imagine themselves in the place of a consumer who was harmed as a result of a

contracted for service. In the personal injury vignette, a consumer received either a

small cut or a spinal injury while using exercise equipment at a health club. In the

property damage vignette, a consumer’s car was either scratched or stolen while in

the care of an automotive repair shop. To avoid the complications of insurance,

participants were asked to imagine that they did not have relevant insurance

coverage.

Both vignettes included a copy of the contract that the consumer signedÐa

membership agreement in the health club vignette and an estimate form in the

automotive repair vignette. The contract language was adopted from actual

contracts, with company names and other identifying information altered. The two

contracts were comparable in terms of length, organization, and complexity.1 The

apparent intended legal effect of the exculpatory clause in both contracts was to

relieve the drafting parties of liability even for their own negligence.2

Questionnaire

Each questionnaire contained 6 manipulation check items, 12 items measured on

7-point Likert-type scales, and two open-ended questions. The Likert-type items

were designed to assess the participants’ self-reported likelihood of seeking

compensation and reading the contract, and the participants’ perceptions of the

enforceability, fairness, and difficulty of the contract language. In order to control

for the possibility that some effects may be a function of participants’

predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs, a final Likert-type

item designed to directly assess perceptions of reputability was included. The

questionnaire items are reproduced in the Appendix.

Design and Procedure

The design was a 2 (severity: minor vs. severe) 62 (presence of exculpatory clause:
present vs. not present) 62 (damage type: personal injury vs. property damage)
design in which the last factor was a repeated measures variable and was used as a

conceptual replication to assess generalization across legal contexts. After

completing an informed consent procedure, each participant received a pack

containing the two contract vignettes and two questionnaires. The order in which

the vignettes appeared was randomly assigned, and the between-subjects conditions

were randomly assigned. The participants were instructed to read through the

Standard form contracts 87

1 Both contracts received a Flesch reading ease score of 23, indicating that the contracts contained
difficult reading appropriate for readers at the 11±13th grade level (Felsenfeld & Siegel, 1981). Both
contracts also received a Gunning’s Fog Index rating of 24. The average word length was 1.68 syllables
in the health club contract and 1.72 syllables in the automotive repair contract. The average sentence
length was 40.8 words and 37.9 words respectively, and the average paragraph length was 1.7 sentences
and 1.5 sentences respectively. Copies of the contracts are available from the first author upon request.
2 The following is an example of the exculpatory language: Buyer specifically agrees that Gym, its
officers, employees and agents shall not be liable for any claim, demand, or cause of action of any kind
whatsoever for, or on account of death, personal injury, property damage or loss of any kind resulting
from or related to Member’s use of the facilities or participation in any sport, exercise or activity within or
without the club premises, and Buyer agrees to hold Gym harmless for same.

materials and to answer the questions following each vignette. Participants were

free to refer back to the vignettes, including the written contract, as they completed

the questionnaires.

Hypotheses

Based on Mueller’s finding that tenants tend to believe that the provisions in

the leases they sign are enforceable, we reasoned that participants would rely

on a contract schema that includes a belief that terms in a written contract are

generally enforceable. Consequently, we expected to find a main effect for the

presence of an exculpatory clause such that participants assigned to conditions

in which an exculpatory clause was present would be less likely than

participants assigned to conditions in which such a clause was not present to

express a strong propensity to seek legal advice or to demand compensation on

their own behalf. Based upon schema theory, we reasoned that as the cost of

being wrong increased, participants’ reliance on their contract schema would

decrease and that participants would show a greater propensity to seek out

additional information. Consequently, we expected to find a main effect for the

severity of the harm such that participants assigned to conditions in which the

harm is minor would be less likely than participants assigned to conditions in

which the harm is severe to express a strong propensity to seek legal advice or

to demand compensation on their own behalf. Furthermore, we expected to

find an interaction between presence of an exculpatory clause and severity of

harm such that when the harm was severe, the presence or absence of an

exculpatory clause would become less relevant to participants’ propensity to

seek legal advice or to demand compensation on their own behalf. Finally, we

expected results would generalize across the two vignettes presented to the

participants.

RESULTS3

Manipulation Checks

Initial analyses were performed on the manipulation check items to determine

whether participants had identified and understood the exculpatory clauses in both

the auto-repair contract and the health club contract. For the auto-repair contract,

a chi-square analysis revealed that almost two-thirds of the participants (65%) were

able to correctly identify whether or not a clause was present that may prevent their

recovery in a lawsuit, w2 (1, N=99)=8.49, p=0.004. Furthermore, most participants
(82%) who correctly indicated that a clause was present were able to correctly

88 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

3 Prior to analysis, each item was examined for accuracy of data entry, missing values, and fit between
their distributions and the assumptions of multivariate analysis. Square root transformations were
performed on four of the items to address their unacceptable skewness. With the transformed variables in
the variable set, one case was identified through Mahalanobis distance as a multivariate outlier and was
deleted from the data set.

identify the paragraph containing the exculpatory clause, w2 (5, N=53)=41.98,
p50.001.4

For the health club contract, a chi-square analysis again revealed that almost

two-thirds of the participants (66%) were able to correctly identify whether or not a

clause was present that may prevent their recovery in a lawsuit, w2 (1,
N=100)=10.35, p50.001. Furthermore, an examination of the presence of
clause by paragraph cross-classification table revealed that of those participants

who correctly indicated a clause was present over 70% correctly identified

paragraph five as containing the clause. However, twelve participants in conditions

containing no relevant exculpatory language also indicated that paragraph five

contained relevant exculpatory language. These false positive identifications led to

a non-significant chi-square statistic, w2 (6, N=58)=6.97, p=0.32.5

Principal Components Analysis

A principal components factor analysis with varimax rotation was performed on

all subjects’ responses to the 12 Likert-type questions (collapsed across the eight

conditions, N=100). The purpose of this procedure was to simplify analysis and
clarify measurements by developing a small set of uncorrelated components

representing the larger number of questionnaire items. Four components were

extracted based upon the eigenvalue greater than one criteria. However, an

examination of the initial statistics revealed that a fifth component had an

eigenvalue of 0.95. Consequently, a five component solution was attempted.

When five components were extracted, the fifth component was unique, with

only the reputability item loading on that component. To be considered a part of a

component, an item was required to have a loading of 0.64 on only that

component. This five component solution was retained because it increased the

interpretability of the components by separating the reputability item, which was

intended to assess predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs

rather than to assess the effects of the experimental manipulations. The five

components accounted for 74.7% of the total measurement variance. The

constituent items and their loadings are presented in Table 1.

The labeled components in order of percentage of explained variance were 1)

propensity to seek compensation, 2) likelihood of reading, 3) fairness/readability, 4)

similarity/enforceability, and 5) reputability. It was reasoned that propensity to seek

compensation, fairness, and similarity/enforceability would be significantly related

to the variables manipulated in the vignettes. In contrast, it was reasoned that the

experimental manipulations would be unlikely to effect likelihood of reading, and

Standard form contracts 89

4 Interestingly, among those participants who mistakenly identified an exculpatory clause when none
was present, 68% identified paragraph 3 as containing the exculpatory clause. Paragraph 3 of the auto-
repair contract did contain disclaimers regarding a parts warranty, perhaps making paragraph 3 the best
choice in the absence of the experimental exculpatory clause. However, the warranty language was not
legally relevant under the circumstances described to the participants.
5 Paragraph five did contain some exculpatory type language even when the experimental clause was not
present. However, the remaining exculpatory language was not relevant under the circumstances
described. Yet, the language may have appeared relevant to the naive legal actor, accounting for the
false±positive identifications.

that any difference in perceptions of reputability would likely be the result of

predetermined attitudes toward car dealers and health clubs. To test these

hypotheses, a new set of composite variables was created to represent the

components, each variable being the average of the set of variables that loaded on a

component.

Analysis of Variance

An ANOVA was performed on each of the orthogonal composite variables, with

follow-up analyses of simple effects where appropriate. First, regarding the effects

of presence of an exculpatory clause, analysis of variance revealed a main effect

such that when the clause was present participants indicated that they would be less

likely to demand compensation or seek legal advice than when the clause was not

present, F(1,95)=4.09, p=0.046, Z2=0.04.
Second, regarding the effects of severity of the harm, analysis of variance revealed

a significant interaction between severity of the harm and damage type for

propensity to seek compensation, F(1,95)=4.75, p=0.032, Z2=0.05. Analysis of
simple effects for propensity to seek compensation revealed that in the property

damage vignette participants indicated that they would be more likely to seek

compensation when the harm was severe (M=3.37) than when the harm was minor
(M=2.95), F(1,98)=5.18, p=0.025, Z2=0.05. Furthermore, when the harm was
minor, participants indicated that they would be more likely to seek compensation

in the personal injury vignette (M=3.31) than in the property damage vignette
(M=2.95), F(1,50)=3.21, p=0.057, Z2=0.06.

Finally, regarding the effects of damage type, analysis of variance revealed a

significant main effect such that participants indicated that in general health clubs

are more reputable than auto dealerships, F(1,95)=44.18, p50.001, Z2=0.32.

90 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

Table 1. Results of the principal components analysis on contract questionnaire items

Component Variance
(%)

Constituent Items Loadings

1. Propensity to seek 23.6 Probability that a lawsuit would be successful. 0.87
compensation Probability that demand will be successful. 0.84

Likelihood of demanding payment for losses. 0.76
Likelihood of seeking advice from an attorney. 0.69

2. Likelihood of reading 20.7 Likelihood of reading the agreement. 0.91
contract Likelihood of reading the agreement carefully. 0.88

3. Fairness and 12.5 Likelihood that contract violates legal rights. 0.79
Readability Difficulty understanding the contract

language.
0.70

Fairness of the contract as a whole. 0.68
4. Similarity and 10.1 Similarity of contract to those commonly used. 0.81

Enforceability Likelihood a court would enforce the contract. 0.64
5. Reputability 7.9 Reputability of most garages/gyms in general. 0.95

Total 74.7

Qualitative Responses

The qualitative results allowed for an examination of more psychologically rich

responses. The first open-ended question provides some additional insight into

participants’ initial reaction to the events described in the vignettes. A total of 94

out of 100 participants answered the following question: “What would you do in

similar circumstances?” Three coders independently categorized the responses with

an inter-rater reliability of 87%.6 Collapsed across all of the conditions, participants

indicated that their likely courses of action might include seeking legal help,

engaging in self-help, absorbing the costs themselves, ending their business

relationship with the drafting party, or seeking help from insurance agents, police,

friends, or family. However, most responses indicated an intention to either seek

legal help (46%) or engage in self-help (29%).

DISCUSSION

The primary focus of this study was the effect of exculpatory clauses on consumers’

propensity to seek compensation when harmed in a setting governed by a form

contract. As indicated by the qualitative results, participants’ initial reaction to the

described situations was to seek compensation, either through legal representation

or by dealing directly with the representatives of the business. However, consistent

with the concerns of many legal scholars, the presence of exculpatory language did

have a deterrent effect on participants’ propensity to seek compensation. This main

effect for presence of an exculpatory clause is also consistent with previous research

suggesting that consumers’ contract schema includes a general belief that written

contract terms are enforceable (Mueller, 1970), thus accounting for their decreased

propensity to seek compensation in the face of an exculpatory clause. Surprisingly,

the presence of an exculpatory clause did not impact participants’ perceptions of

the fairness of the contracts.

We expected that when the costs of being wrong were great (i.e., the harm was

severe), participants would be less likely to rely on their contract schemas (Fiske &

Taylor, 1991). This hypothesis received partial support. There was no main effect

for severity of the harm. However, a significant interaction between damage type

and severity of the harm revealed that in the property damage vignette participants

indicated a greater propensity to seek compensation when the harm was severe than

when the harm was minor. In contrast, severity of the harm had no effect on

propensity to seek compensation in the personal injury vignette. Furthermore,

participants indicated a greater propensity to seek compensation when the harm

was minor in the personal injury vignette than in the property damage vignette.

These effects may be the result of participants having a stronger affective reaction to

Standard form contracts 91

6 The first coder grouped the responses in a manner such that seven representative categories developed.
Using the category definitions developed by the first coder, two more coders independently categorized
the responses. The assessment of inter-rater reliability was calculated by dividing the number of
agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements between each possible pairing of researchers
on a large sample of the coded data. The three percentages of agreement between the three indepedent
coders (84%, 90%, 86%) were then averaged to produce the overall inter-rater reliability of 87%.

personal injuries than to property damage, leading them to be more likely to seek

compensation for a personal injury regardless of the specific circumstances.

The type of damage repeated measures variable was included to determine

whether the effects of presence of an exculpatory clause and severity of the harm

would generalize across contexts. Thus, we did not expect any main effects of type

of damage for propensity to seek compensation. However, because the property

damage condition involved a car dealership and the personal injury condition

involved a health club, we were concerned that participants may bring

predetermined attitudes regarding the reputability of car dealers and health

clubs. Rather than allow this potential confound, we directly measured

participants’ attitudes towards car dealerships and health clubs. Consistent with

our concerns, participants rated health clubs as being more reputable than car

dealerships. However, the only main effect of damage type was for reputability,

suggesting that participants’ attitudes towards car dealerships and health clubs had

little effect on their ratings on the other dependent variables.

Limitations and Implications

This study represents a novel, but preliminary, attempt at expanding empirical

psychological inquiry into an area of contract law that has been relatively untapped

by psycholegal researchers. Although it is quite likely that undergraduates are

regularly exposed to the types of contracts presented in our vignettes, the use of

undergraduate participants and a vignette methodology does limit the

generalizability of these results. Replications with differing methods and

populations are needed. Furthermore, the high number of false positive

identifications of exculpatory language revealed in the manipulation checks

suggest that the participants had considerable difficulty comprehending the

contractual language,7 and this may have contributed to the relatively small

effect size (n2=0.04) for presence of the clause. Such difficulties are not inconsistent
with previous findings (Masson & Waldron, 1994) and may be exacerbated in

research using less well educated samples. It must also be remembered that many

consumers may not read form contracts at the time of the transaction.8

Furthermore, it can be assumed that not all consumers would refer back to the

language of their contract after incurring an injury as a result of the contracted for

service.

Although preliminary, the present data do raise several interesting implications

for the ongoing legal debate over the inclusion of potentially unenforceable

exculpatory clauses and the ongoing development of psychological theories of

contractual behaviors. First, consistent with the concerns of many legal scholars,

the presence of exculpatory language in form contracts does appear to have some

deterrent effect on consumers’ propensity to seek compensation. Second,

consistent with the findings of Schmedemann and McLean Parks (1994), the

92 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

7 Participants in this study indicated that the language contained in the contracts was “somewhat
difficult” to understand (M=3.8).
8 Participants in this study indicated that they would be “somewhat likely” to read the contract
(M=4.00) but would be less likely to read the contract closely (M=3.53), t(99)=4.51, p50.001.

data supports the notion of a contract schema. Third, consistent with the findings

of Mueller (1970), the data suggest that consumers’ contract schemas may include

a general belief that all contract terms are enforceable. Finally, the results suggest

that in the face of increased costs of being wrong, consumers may abandon their

contract schema. In cases involving damage to property, when the severity of the

harm was great participants showed a greater propensity to seek compensation

rather than rely on their general belief in the enforceability of contract terms.

Additional research should further investigate the nature and content of contract

schemas and their impact on decision making across a variety of subject

populations and transactional contexts.

APPENDIX

PLEASE ANSWER THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS.

FEEL FREE TO REFER BACK TO THE SCENARIO

YOU JUST READ.

1. What would you do in similar circumstances? Please explain briefly:

2. How similar do you think this [agreement/estimate] is to those used by most

[health clubs/garages]?

3. How difficult do you find the language of the [membership/estimate] form to

understand?

4. Under the circumstances described above, how likely do you think you

would have been to read the agreement?

5. Under the circumstances described above, how likely do you think you

would have been to read the agreement closely?

6. How likely would you be to demand that the [health club/garage] pay for

your losses?

7. How successful do you think you would be if you demanded that the [health

club/garage] pay for your losses?

8. Do you think that there are any clauses in the [membership/estimate] form

that might prevent you from recovering on your demand?

8a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might prevent

your recovery:

9. How likely would you be to seek advice from an attorney?

10. Assuming the attorney advised you a lawsuit was possible, how successful do

you think your lawsuit might be?

11. Do you think that there are any clauses in the [membership/estimate] form

that might prevent you from recovering in a lawsuit?

11a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might prevent

your recovery:

12. How likely do you think it is that the [membership/estimate] form you signed

violates your legal rights?

13. How likely do you think it is that a court of law would uphold the

[membership/estimate] form that you signed?

14. How fair do you think the [membership/estimate] form as a whole is?

Standard form contracts 93

15. Do you think that any of the paragraphs in the [membership/estimate] form

are unethical?

15a. If so, please circle the number of the paragraph(s) that might be

unethical:

16. Have you ever had an experience similar to that in this scenario? Explain:

17. How reputable do you think most [health clubs/garages] are in general?

REFERENCES

Eisenberg, M. A. (1995). The limits of cognition and the limits of contract. Stanford Law Review, 47,
211±259.

Farnsworth, E. A. (1991). United States contract law.
Farnsworth, E. A. (1990). Contracts. Boston: Little, Brown and Company.
Felsenfeld, C., & Siegel, A. (1981). Writing contracts in plain English. St. Paul: West Publishing.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gilmore, G. (1974). The death of contract. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press.
Harrison, J. L. (1994). Class, personality, contract, and unconscionability. William & Mary Law

Review, 35, 445±501.
Hasen, R. L. (1990). Efficiency under informational asymmetry: The effect of framing on legal rules.

University of California Los Angeles Law Review, 38, 391±438.
Kuklin, B. (1988). On the knowing inclusion of unenforceable contract and lease terms. University of

Cincinnati Law Review, 56, 845±918.
Macaulay, S. (1963). Non-contractual relations in business: A preliminary study. American Sociological

Review, 28, 55±67.
Macaulay, S. (1985). An empirical view of contract. Wisconsin Law Review, 1985, 465±482.
MacNeil, I. R. (1985). Relational contract: What we do and do not know. Wisconsin Law Review,

1985, 483±525.
Masson, M. E. J., & Waldron, M. A. (1994). Comprehension of legal contracts by non-experts:

Effectiveness of plain language redrafting. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 67±85.
Monahan, J. & Walker, L. (1993). Social science in law: Cases and materials (3rd ed.). Westbury, New

York: The Foundation Press.
Morant, B. D. (1995). Contracts limiting liability: A paradox with tacit solutions. Tulane Law Review,

69, 715±779.
Morrison, E. W. & Robinson, S. L. (in press). When employees feel betrayed: A model of how

psychological contract violation develops. Academy of Management Journal.
Mueller, W. (1970). Residential tenants and their leases: An empirical study. Michigan Law Review,

69, 247±298.
Robinson, S. L. (in press). Trust and breach of the psychological contract. Administrative Science

Quarterly.
Robinson, S. L., & Rousseau, D. M. (1994). Violating the psychological contract: not the exception

but the norm. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 245±259.
Rousseau, D. M. (1989). Psychological and implied contracts in organizations. Employee

Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 2, 121±139.
Rousseau, D. M., & Aquino, K. (1993). Fairness and implied contract obligations in job terminations:

the role of remedies, social accounts, and procedural justice. Human Performance, 6, 135±149.
Rubin, E. L. (1995). The nonjudicial life of contract: Beyond the shadow of the law. Northwestern

University Law Review, 90, 107±131.
Schmedemann, D. A., & McLean Parks, J. (1994). Contract formation and employee handbooks:

Legal, psychological, and empirical analyses. Wake Forest Law Review, 29, 647±718.
Speidel, R. W. (1995). The shifting domain of contract. Northwestern University Law Review, 90,

254±266.
Stratman, J. F. (1988). Contract disclaimers in ERISA summary plan documents: A deceptive

practice? Industrial Relations Law Journal, 10, 350±380.
Vukowich, W. T. (1993). Lawyers and the standard form contract system: A model rule that should

have been. Georgetown Journal of Legal Ethics, 6, 799±859.
White, J. J. (1988). Promise fulfilled and principle betrayed. Annual Survey of American Law, 1988,

7±42.

94 D. P. Stolle and A. J. Slain

Copyright of Behavioral Sciences & the Law is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not

be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder’s express written

permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

  • The Accumulation of Stereotype-Based Self-Fulfilling Prophecies
  • Stephanie Madon
    Iowa State University

    Lee Jussim
    Rutgers University

    Max Guyll
    Iowa State University

    Heather Nofziger and Elizabeth R. Salib
    Rutgers University

    Jennifer Willard
    Kennesaw State University

    Kyle C. Scherr
    Central Michigan University

    A recurring theme in the psychological literature is that the self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes can
    accumulate across perceivers. This article provides the first empirical support for this long-standing
    hypothesis. In three experiments (Ns � 123–241), targets more strongly confirmed a stereotype as the
    number of perceivers who held stereotypic expectations about them increased. A fourth experiment (N �
    121) showed that new perceivers judged targets according to the stereotypic behaviors they had
    previously been channeled to adopt, an effect that even occurred among perceivers who were privy to the
    fact that targets’ behavior had been shaped by the actions of others. The authors discuss ways in which
    these effects may contribute to group inequalities.

    Keywords: accumulation, behavioral confirmation, self-fulfilling prophecy, stereotypes

    Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    New Look in Perception of the 1940s and 1950s initiated a
    revolution in approaches to perception within social psycholog

    y.

    Departing from the prevailing view that perception is veridical,
    New Look in Perception research promoted the idea that percep-
    tion is influenced by the goals, needs, and motives of perceivers.
    An influential perspective that emerged from this movement was a
    weak form of social constructionism. According to this perspec-
    tive, social beliefs can alter reality and shape behavior. The self-
    fulfilling prophecy is central to this perspective because it involves
    a perceiver’s false expectation about a target initiating a sequence
    of events that causes the target to exhibit expectancy-consistent
    behavior, thereby making the initially false expectation true. This
    research tested a core tenet of social constructionism within social
    psychology—the idea that self-fulfilling prophecy effects can ac-

    cumulate across perceivers. Moreover, it tested this hypothesis
    with respect to stereotypes, which psychological theory proposes
    contribute to group inequalities through their cumulative self-
    fulfilling effects.

    Self-Fulfilling Prophecies and Their
    Cumulative Effects

    The idea that false expectations can lead to their own fulfillment
    originated in the writings of Merton (1948). Merton proposed that
    the self-fulfilling prophecy was a powerful process capable of
    producing profound social problems including war, economic
    downturns, academic underachievement, and racial disparities in
    employment and wealth. Research bearing on Merton’s analysis
    clearly supported the existence of self-fulfilling prophecies, but not
    the idea that self-fulfilling prophecy effects are powerful. Both
    experimental and naturalistic research have converged on the con-
    clusion that perceivers’ false expectations have only modest self-
    fulfilling effects on the behavior of targets (Jussim, 2012;
    Rosenthal, 1994, 2003). However, these modest effects should not
    be interpreted to mean that self-fulfilling prophecies can never be
    powerful. Even small self-fulfilling prophecy effects can become
    powerful if they accumulate across perceivers (e.g., Jussim,
    Eccles, & Madon, 1996; Klein & Snyder, 2003; Madon, Guyll,
    Spoth, & Willard, 2004; Merton, 1948).

    The potential for self-fulfilling prophecy effects to accumulate
    across perceivers represents a central theme within social psychol-
    ogy (Ross, Lepper, & Ward, 2010). Yet, only one study has
    empirically supported the hypothesized effect. In the context of a

    Stephanie Madon, Psychology Department, Iowa State University; Lee
    Jussim, Psychology Department, Rutgers University; Max Guyll, Psychol-
    ogy Department, Iowa State University; Heather Nofziger and Elizabeth R.
    Salib, Psychology Department, Rutgers University; Jennifer Willard, Psy-
    chology Department, Kennesaw State University; Kyle C. Scherr, Psychol-
    ogy Department, Central Michigan University.

    Heather Nofziger is now at The NPD Group. Elizabeth R. Salib is now
    at Heidrick Consulting.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephanie
    Madon, Psychology Department, Iowa State University, W112 Lagomar-
    cino Hall, 901 Stange Road, Ames, IA 50011-1041. E-mail: madon@
    iastate.edu

    THIS ARTICLE HAS BEEN CORRECTED. SEE LAST PAGE

    T
    hi

    s
    do

    cu
    m

    en
    t

    is
    co

    py
    ri

    gh
    te

    d
    by

    th
    e

    A
    m

    er
    ic

    an
    P

    sy
    ch

    ol
    og

    ic
    al

    A
    ss

    oc
    ia

    ti
    on

    or
    on

    e
    of

    it
    s

    al
    li

    ed
    pu

    bl
    is

    he
    rs

    .
    T

    hi
    s

    ar
    ti

    cl
    e

    is
    in

    te
    nd

    ed
    so

    le
    ly

    fo
    r

    th
    e

    pe
    rs

    on
    al

    us
    e

    of
    th

    e
    in

    di
    vi

    du
    al

    us
    er

    an
    d

    is
    no

    t
    to

    be
    di

    ss
    em

    in
    at

    ed
    br

    oa
    dl

    y.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology:
    Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes

    © 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 115, No. 5,

    825

    – 844
    0022-3514/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142

    825

    mailto:madon@iastate.edu

    mailto:madon@iastate.edu

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142

    longitudinal study involving parents and their adolescent children,
    Madon et al. (2004) found that adolescents drank the greatest
    amount of alcohol when mothers and fathers both held negative
    expectations about their future alcohol use. However, because
    Madon et al. demonstrated this effect with correlational data, they
    could not rule out predictive accuracy as an alternative explanation
    of the findings. Put differently, their data could not exclude the
    possibility that parents’ negative expectations were accurate from
    the outset, in which case they could not have been self-fulfilling,
    a limitation that characterizes all correlational self-fulfilling proph-
    ecy research.

    The only way to eliminate predictive accuracy as an alternative
    to a self-fulfilling prophecy interpretation is to experimentally
    manipulate perceivers’ expectations. Accordingly, there is a need
    to examine the accumulation of self-fulfilling prophecy effects
    across perceivers in tightly controlled laboratory experiments. It is
    especially important to examine this process with respect to ste-
    reotypes, which for 70 years have been hypothesized to contribute
    to group inequalities via their cumulative self-fulfilling effects
    (Jussim et al., 1996; Klein & Snyder, 2003; Merton, 1948; Word,
    Zanna, & Cooper, 1974).

    There is good evidence that stereotypes can have self-fulfilling
    effects on targets’ behavior (Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977;
    Word et al., 1974). However, research demonstrating this effect
    has focused exclusively on dyadic relations involving one per-
    ceiver and one target. Although this focus is often warranted, it
    may underestimate the power of stereotypes because it does not
    account for the possibility that their self-fulfilling effects may
    accumulate across multiple perceivers. Specifically, because ste-
    reotypes can be consensual, different perceivers may hold similar
    expectations about members of stereotyped groups (Madon et al.,
    2001). To the extent that these expectations are false for a partic-
    ular target, each perceiver may have a self-fulfilling effect that
    combines with the self-fulfilling effects of other perceivers to
    ultimately cause a target to confirm the stereotype more strongly
    than would have been the case had only one perceiver held the
    false stereotypic expectation.

    Situational Affordances

    One mechanism through which stereotypes may have cumula-
    tive self-fulfilling effects is situational affordances. Generally
    speaking, affordances are the properties of a stimulus that encour-
    age a particular behavioral response (Gibson, 1979). In the words
    of Koffka (1935), for example, “a fruit says, ‘Eat me’; water says,
    ‘Drink me’.” (p. 7). Although typically applied to the properties of
    objects, perceivers’ treatment of targets can construct situations
    with opportunities or constraints that may likewise be viewed as
    affordances. If these opportunities and constraints channel a target
    to behaviorally confirm a false expectation, then the self-fulfilling
    prophecy occurs because the situation afforded such behavior.

    Classic considerations of the self-fulfilling prophecy highlight
    situational affordances as a key mechanism through which stereo-
    types can become self-fulfilling. For example, Merton (1948)
    argued that the early 20th century practice of barring Blacks from
    labor unions on grounds that they were strike breakers encouraged
    Black laborers to cross picket lines by restricting their job oppor-
    tunities. Similarly, Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) and Rist (2000)
    hypothesized that negative expectations caused teachers to limit

    the educational opportunities available to disadvantaged students,
    thereby undermining those students’ academic achievement. These
    examples illustrate how perceivers’ treatment of targets can con-
    struct situational affordances that encourage targets to behaviorally
    confirm a stereotype.

    The potential for a stereotype’s self-fulfilling effect to accumu-
    late across perceivers arises when multiple perceivers provide
    similar situational affordances to the same target. For example,
    consider a scenario in which two educators independently provide
    a female student with a situational affordance that encourages
    confirmation of sex stereotypes: A math teacher inappropriately
    tracks her into a low-ability math class, and a guidance counselor
    encourages her to take home economics or typing as the required
    elective, never suggesting alternatives such as computer program-
    ming or woodshop. Although these situational affordances do not
    prevent the student from excelling in math or enrolling in a
    male-dominated elective, they will tend to channel her in the
    direction of confirming sex stereotypes. Moreover, because two
    educators each constructed the situation in this way, the overall
    effect on the student stands to be greater than if only one educator
    had done so. This is because multiple perceivers who share, and
    independently act upon, a false expectation generate multiple
    vectors of influence that can combine to shape the totality of the
    situation faced by a target in a way that more strongly affords
    behavioral confirmation of a stereotype than is the case with only
    one perceiver.

    Research Overview

    The primary objective of the present research was to test the
    hypothesis that the self-fulfilling effects of stereotypes can accu-
    mulate across perceivers. Experiment 1 tested this hypothesis with
    respect to the overweight stereotype, whereas Experiments 2 and 3
    tested this hypothesis with respect to sex stereotypes. Consistent
    with classic considerations of the self-fulfilling prophecy, the
    experiments focused on situational affordances as the underlying
    mechanism of the hypothesized accumulation effect.

    Experiment 4

    tested whether targets’ confirmatory behavior— behavior that was
    caused by a stereotype’s prior cumulative self-fulfilling effect—
    influences new perceivers’ judgments of them. This issue is im-
    portant because it addresses the possibility that once the cumula-
    tive self-fulfilling effect of a stereotype has been set in motion, it
    can contribute to a cycle whereby prior self-fulfilling prophecy
    effects lead new perceivers to develop false expectations, which
    may themselves become self-fulfilling.

    Analytic Strategy

    Although the methods of the experiments differed, the same
    analytic strategy was used to test the accumulation hypothesis.
    First, the analyses tested whether perceivers developed stereotypic
    expectations about a target. Second, the analyses tested whether
    perceivers’ stereotypic expectations led them to provide targets
    with situational affordances that encouraged a stereotypic re-
    sponse. Third, the analyses examined whether the number of
    perceivers who held a stereotypic expectation about a target influ-
    enced how strongly the target confirmed the stereotype. Fourth, a
    series of planned contrasts tested for dyadic self-fulfilling proph-
    ecy effects involving one perceiver and one target, and then for the

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    826 MADON ET AL.

    hypothesized accumulation effect which, in this research, involved
    two perceivers and one target. Finally, the analyses explored
    whether the accumulation effects reflected concurrent or synergis-
    tic accumulation. Concurrent accumulation occurs when multiple
    perceivers each have a unique, additive self-fulfilling effect on a
    target’s behavior (Jussim et al., 1996). Synergistic accumulation
    occurs when the self-fulfilling effects of multiple perceivers are
    stronger in combination than their additive effects would suggest
    (Madon et al., 2004).

    Type I Error

    Bonferroni corrections and Tukey’s HSD contrasts controlled
    for Type I error when there were multiple comparisons, any one of
    which would support a predicted effect (e.g., testing whether
    perceivers developed stereotypic expectations). LSD contrasts
    were used when Type I error was not an issue because the pre-
    dicted effect required only a single significant comparison (e.g.,
    testing whether a stereotype had a cumulative self-fulfilling effect)
    or required multiple significant comparisons to support a predicted
    effect (e.g., testing for concurrent or synergistic accumulation).
    Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to exam-
    ine the effect of experimental factors on multiple dependent vari-
    ables of the same underlying construct.

    Power and Effect Sizes

    Meta-analyses of interpersonal self-fulfilling prophecies report
    an average effect size of d � .60 (Rosenthal, 1994). Because the
    current research focused on accumulation, we increased this effect
    size by 10% when performing power analyses. The results indi-
    cated that Experiments 1 and 2 each required a sample size of 93
    to achieve a power of .80 for detecting the anticipated accumula-
    tion effect of d � .66, whereas Experiment 3 required a sample
    size of 115. When calculating the sample size of Experiment 4, we
    used an effect size of d � .80 on grounds that the meta-analytic
    effect of individuating information on person perception is large
    (Kunda & Thagard, 1996). Using this estimate, a power analysis
    indicated that Experiment 4 required a sample of 80 to achieve a
    power of .80. All sample sizes exceeded these minimums. Effect
    sizes and their confidence intervals (CI) are reported in terms of d,
    �2, and �p2, which were calculated with scripts developed by
    Wuensch (2012). A 95% CI is reported for d, and a 90% CI for �2

    and �p2 (Steiger, 2004).

    Experiment 1

    Experiment 1 tested whether the self-fulfilling effect of the
    overweight stereotype can accumulate across perceivers. The pro-
    cedures involved two phases. Phase 1 manipulated a target’s
    weight to examine whether it influenced the situational affor-
    dances that individual perceivers provided a target. Phase 2 ex-
    posed a new group of participants (designated as targets) to the
    combined situational affordances that two randomly paired per-
    ceivers had provided the target in Phase 1. Targets’ behavior
    during the experimental session indexed the degree to which they
    confirmed the overweight stereotype.

    Method

    Participants. Undergraduates (N � 723) at Iowa State Uni-
    versity participated to fulfill a course requirement, including 403
    women, 319 men, and one participant who omitted a response.
    There were 19 African Americans, 26 Asian/Asian Americans,
    626 European Americans, 5 Native Americans, 5 Indians, and 42
    participants who self-described as multiethnic.

    Experimental design. Participants were randomly assigned to
    triads (N � 241 triads) each consisting of two perceivers and one
    target. Each triad was randomly assigned to one of three expecta-
    tion conditions. In the no-overweight expectation condition (n �
    83 triads), both perceivers in a triad believed the target was thin. In
    the single-overweight expectation condition (n � 76 triads), one
    perceiver in a triad believed the target was thin, whereas the other
    believed the target was heavy. In the double-overweight expecta-
    tion condition (n � 82 triads), both perceivers in a triad believed
    the target was heavy. Although triad was the unit of analysis,
    participants completed the experimental procedures independent-
    ly; hence, the data were not nested. The targets’ weight was
    manipulated with a bogus profile and photograph that were ran-
    domly assigned to perceivers.

    Phase 1: Materials and measures.
    Profile. A handwritten profile that appeared to have been

    completed by the target at an earlier session reported the target’s
    sex, age, height, weight, and personality. The profile always de-
    scribed the target as female, European American, 21 years old,
    “between 5 feet 4 inches and 5 feet 6 inches tall”, and as having a
    constellation of personality traits that was held constant. The
    profile systematically varied the target’s weight to be “between
    101 and 120 pounds” (n � 242) or “between 181 and 200 pounds”
    (n � 240).

    Photograph. A photograph of a heavy or thin European Amer-
    ican woman in her early twenties accompanied each profile. For
    stimulus sampling purposes, two photographs depicted a heavy
    woman and two depicted a thin woman.

    Perceiver behavior. Perceivers predicted the target’s attitudes
    and behaviors on a variety of issues by responding to a series of
    questions. Included among these was a critical item that assessed
    the extent to which perceivers provided the target with a situational
    affordance that encouraged confirmation of the overweight stereo-
    type. Specifically, perceivers were shown colored pictures of four
    bins that contained 2 (Bin 1), 4 (Bin 2), 20 (Bin 3), and 40 (Bin 4)
    pieces of candy, and asked “Which bin should be given to the
    person in the next phase? Choose the bin that you think contains
    the approximate amount of candy the person would eat if nobody
    was around.” Four types of candy were used: Kisses, Butterfingers,
    Peanut Butter Cups, and Kit-Kats, the last three of which were all
    fun-size. Except for Bin 1, which contained one Butterfinger and
    one Kit-Kat, equal numbers of each type of candy were included
    in each bin.

    Although the amount of candy in the bins was arbitrary, the
    spread was by design. We did not want perceivers to have the
    option of selecting an intermediate amount of candy because such
    an option would have encouraged them to hedge. We wanted to
    compel perceivers to make a clear choice: choose a little or choose
    a lot. We included some variation on each side so that perceivers
    did not feel overly restricted in their choices. To reduce suspicion,
    the critical item was embedded among fillers each accompanied by

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    827CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    a colored picture (i.e., how much the target liked popular TV
    shows; target’s likelihood of using an electronic voting machine
    and taking an online college course; how favorably the target
    viewed DNA testing).

    Manipulation checks. To assess whether perceivers noticed
    the target’s weight, they reported the target’s weight category as
    indicated on the profile. This question was embedded among fillers
    that instructed perceivers to report the target’s sex and height,
    which were also indicated on the profile. To assess whether the
    target’s weight activated perceivers’ stereotypes, perceivers judged
    the target with respect to five traits, including two that are stereo-
    typic of heavy people (willpower, self-control) and three that are
    not strongly associated with weight (outgoing, intelligent, reli-
    gious; Crandall, 1994; Puhl & Brownell, 2001). Perceivers made
    these trait judgments on 7-point scales with endpoints 1 (not at all)
    and 7 (very much).

    Suspicion check for perceivers. Perceivers reported their be-
    liefs about the experiment’s purpose, research questions under
    investigation, and any prior knowledge they had of the study.

    Phase 2: Materials and measures.
    Candy. Each target received one bowl of candy that included

    the exact amount and type of candy that the two perceivers in the
    target’s triad had selected for the target in Phase 1. For example, if one
    perceiver selected Bin 1 (i.e., one Butterfinger and one Kit-Kat) and
    the other perceiver selected Bin 2 (i.e., one Kiss, one Butterfinger,
    one Peanut Butter Cup, and one Kit-Kat), then the target in this
    triad would have received one bowl of candy that included one
    Kiss, two Butterfingers, one Peanut Butter Cup, and two Kit-Kats.
    Thus, each perceiver independently selected a bin of candy for the
    target in Phase 1, and the candy they selected was combined and
    given to a target in Phase 2. This procedure created a situation that
    is analogous to real-world circumstances in which the independent
    actions of multiple perceivers (e.g., teachers, employers, parents)
    can combine to affect how strongly the situation faced by a target
    (e.g., student, employee, child) encourages behavioral confirma-
    tion of a stereotype.

    Target behavior. The amount of candy targets took from the
    bowl provided an explicit behavior with which to determine how
    strongly they confirmed the overweight stereotype.

    Filler questions and props. To support the cover story used in
    Phase 2—that the study was designed to examine the relationship
    between personality and taste preferences—all targets completed
    filler questions that assessed their personality traits and expecta-
    tions about the candy they expected to eat. In addition, several
    props encouraged targets to take candy home. A printed sign read:
    “Feel free to take as much candy home with you as you like. We
    have plenty.” There was a large stack of brown paper lunch bags
    provided for the purpose of carrying the candy. A garbage can
    filled with candy wrappers was placed on the floor near targets. At
    the end of the session, a computer message encouraged targets to
    take candy home.

    Suspicion check for targets. Targets described their beliefs
    about the experiment’s purpose and reported any prior knowledge
    they had of the study.

    Procedures.
    Phase 1. Perceivers were run in group sessions, but provided

    independent responses. After obtaining informed consent, the ex-
    perimenter described the study as examining how accurately peo-
    ple can predict other people’s attitudes and behaviors from their

    personalities. Perceivers then received a packet that included the
    target’s profile and photograph, plus a survey. Although perceivers
    believed the target was another participant in the study and unique
    to them, multiple copies of the four different photographs were
    distributed at each experimental session, but ordered in a way that
    prevented adjacent perceivers from receiving the same one.

    To substantiate the target’s authenticity, the experimenter asked
    perceivers to indicate whether they knew the person assigned to
    them. A stooge planted in the group publicly stated knowing the
    person in her or his packet. In response, the experimenter gave the
    stooge a new packet and asked if s/he knew this second person.
    The stooge always indicated that s/he did not, at which point the
    experimenter again asked if there were others who knew the
    person in their respective packets. When it was confirmed that no
    one did, the experimenter explained that the individuals in the
    packets had completed a profile and provided a photograph at an
    earlier session, and would return for another session to have their
    attitudes and behaviors assessed.

    The experimenter then directed perceivers to use the target’s
    profile to complete the accompanying survey, which instructed
    them to predict the target’s attitudes and behaviors on the same
    issues they believed the target would provide information about at
    the later session. Perceivers believed that their predictions would
    be compared with the target’s actual responses. Perceivers retained
    the profile and photograph while making their predictions. After-
    ward, participants completed the manipulation and suspicion
    checks and provided demographic information. Debriefing fol-
    lowed. The stooge completed all materials along with perceivers.

    Phase 2. After the data from Phase 1 had been collected, the
    perceivers were randomly paired to create the expectation condi-
    tions. The candy selected by each pair of perceivers was combined
    and given to a third participant who was randomly assigned to be
    the target in the triad. After providing informed consent, the
    experimenter escorted each target to a private room equipped with
    a computer, bowl of candy, and props. The experimenter explained
    that the study was designed to examine how personality relates to
    taste preferences, and informed targets that they would perform
    a taste test of candy immediately after answering survey questions.
    The target was further informed that the experimenter would wait
    for the target to complete the experiment in a separate lab, and that
    the target should come to that location at the end of the session to
    receive research credit. This procedure provided targets with com-
    plete privacy, thereby reducing any inhibitions they might have
    had taking candy. After completing the survey, which included
    filler questions and suspicion checks, the computer informed tar-
    gets that they would not engage in a taste test of the candy after all,
    but were free to take as much as they wanted. Targets then met the
    experimenter as instructed, and were debriefed. Upon their depar-
    ture, the experimenter returned to the testing room to record the
    amount of candy taken.

    Results

    Preliminary analyses.
    Descriptive statistics. Supplemental Tables 1 and 2 report

    descriptive statistics for the variables in Phase 1 and Phase 2,
    respectively.

    Manipulation checks. There were 480 perceivers (�99%)
    who correctly reported the target’s weight and 2 (�1%) who

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    828 MADON ET AL.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    omitted a response. Independent sample t tests, with a Bonferroni
    correction criterion of p � .01, indicated that perceivers judged the
    target as having significantly less willpower and self-control when
    they believed the target was heavy versus thin, ts � 3.98, ps �
    .001; perceivers’ judgments of the target’s outgoingness, intelli-
    gence, and religiosity did not differ significantly as a function of
    the target’s weight, ts � 1.46, ps � .146. These results show that
    perceivers developed stereotypic expectations about the target.
    Table 1 reports the means, standard deviations, and effect sizes
    associated with these results.

    Suspicion checks. There were five suspicious perceivers and
    two suspicious targets, no two from the same triad. Excluding their
    data slightly increased the expectation’s effect, but did not mean-
    ingfully alter the results. No participant’s data were excluded from
    the analyses.

    Main analyses. The manipulation checks demonstrated that
    perceivers developed stereotypic expectations about the target.
    Therefore, the main analyses tested for a series of effects that are
    relevant to the overweight stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling
    effect.

    Situational affordances. First, the analyses tested whether
    perceivers’ stereotypic expectations caused them to provide the
    target with a situational affordance that encouraged confirmation
    of the overweight stereotype. A Mann–Whitney U test examined
    whether the target’s weight (heavy vs. thin) influenced the bin of
    candy that individual perceivers selected for the target in Phase 1.
    The results showed that perceivers chose bins containing signifi-
    cantly more candy when they believed the target was heavy
    (Mdn � Bin 3, 20 pieces of candy) versus thin (Mdn � Bin 2, 4
    pieces of candy), U � 12,279, p � .001. Thus, individual perceiv-
    ers provided the target with a situational affordance that more
    strongly encouraged confirmation of the overweight stereotype
    when they believed the target was heavy versus thin.

    In addition, a one-way ANOVA tested whether the number of
    perceivers who believed the target was heavy influenced the
    amount of candy targets received in Phase 2. The independent
    variable was the expectation (no-overweight vs. single-overweight
    vs. double-overweight). The dependent variable was the amount of
    candy given to targets in Phase 2. The results showed that targets
    received the least candy in the no-overweight expectation condi-
    tion (M � 17, SD � 13.14), an intermediate amount of candy in
    the single-overweight expectation condition (M � 27, SD 14.82),

    and the most candy in the double-overweight expectation condi-
    tion (M � 42, SD � 15.74), F(2, 238) � 59.01, p � .001, �2 �
    .33, 90% CI [0.25, 0.40]. Three Tukey’s HSD contrasts indicated
    that these amounts all differed significantly from one another,
    ts(238) � 3.98, ps � .001, ds � .63. These results demonstrate that
    the situation constructed by two independent perceivers afforded
    targets more opportunity to confirm the overweight stereotype than
    did the situation constructed by individual perceivers singly.

    Target behavior. Second, the analyses tested whether the
    number of perceivers who held a stereotypic expectation about
    the target influenced how strongly the target confirmed the stereo-
    type. A one-way ANOVA indicated differences in the effect of the
    expectation (no-overweight vs. single-overweight vs. double-
    overweight) on the amount of candy taken by targets in Phase 2,
    F(2, 238) � 5.32, p � .005, �2 � .043, 90% CI [0.01, 0.09]. A
    mediational path model showed that the amount of candy targets
    received explained 86% of the expectation’s effect, b � .744;
    SE � .228, p � .001, suggesting nearly full mediation.

    Self-fulfilling prophecy effect. Third, an LSD contrast tested
    for a dyadic self-fulfilling effect involving one perceiver and
    one target by comparing the amount of candy taken by targets
    in the no- and single-overweight expectation conditions. No
    significant difference emerged, indicating that individual percei-
    vers’ stereotypic expectations did not have a self-fulfilling effect
    (Mno-overweight � 3.04, SDno-overweight � 2.85 vs. Msingle-overweight �
    2.99, SDsingle-overweight � 3.62), t(238) � 0.08, p � .94, 95% CI
    [�1.19, 1.29], d � 0.01, 95% CI [�0.30, 0.32].

    Accumulation. Despite no evidence of a dyadic self-fulfilling
    prophecy effect, there was still the possibility of an accumulation
    effect. In fact, accumulation may sometimes be necessary for a
    stereotype to have any self-fulfilling effect at all. In the current
    data, for example, it was possible that the signal communicated by
    the situational affordances was too weak to elicit a self-fulfilling
    effect when only the expectations of individual perceivers were
    considered, in which case targets might have ignored or discounted
    the signal. To address this, a LSD contrast compared the amount of
    candy taken by targets in the single- and double-overweight ex-
    pectation conditions. It showed that targets took significantly more
    candy in the double-overweight (M � 4.77, SD � 5.07) than
    single-overweight (M � 2.99, SD � 3.62) expectation condition,
    t(238) � 2.83, p � .005, 95% CI [0.54, 3.02], d � 0.45, 95% CI
    [0.14, 0.76]. This result supports an accumulation effect because it

    Table 1
    Experiment 1: Perceivers’ Trait Judgments of the Target

    Trait judgment

    Expectation condition

    Heavy Thin
    Mean

    difference Effect size

    M SD M SD 95% CI d 95% CI

    Willpower 3.56 1.08 3.95 1.09 0.20, 0.59 0.36 0.18, 0.54
    Self-control 4.18 1.20 4.67 1.08 0.29, 0.70 0.43 0.25, 0.61
    Outgoing 3.24 1.30 3.19 1.22 �0.27, 0.18 0.04 �0.14, 0.21
    Intelligent 4.67 0.81 4.79 0.93 �0.04, 0.27 0.13 �0.05, 0.31
    Religious 4.39 0.95 4.34 1.15 �0.23, 0.14 0.04 �0.14, 0.22

    Note. The df was 479 for willpower, self-control, outgoing, and intelligent, and 478 for religious. M � mean,
    SD � standard deviation, d � Cohen’s d. Cohen’s d and the corresponding confidence intervals were calculated
    with a script developed by Wuensch (2012).

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    829CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    shows that targets more strongly confirmed the overweight stereo-
    type when two perceivers believed the target was heavy than when
    only one did.

    Pattern of accumulation. Finally, the analyses explored the
    pattern of the accumulation effect by testing for concurrent and
    synergistic accumulation. Concurrent accumulation occurs when
    multiple perceivers each have a unique, additive self-fulfilling
    effect on a target’s behavior (Jussim et al., 1996). The data would
    support concurrent accumulation if the degree to which targets
    confirmed the overweight stereotype increased across the no-,
    single-, and double-overweight expectation conditions, respec-
    tively. As reported above, however, there was no significant dif-
    ference in the amount of candy taken by targets in the no- and
    single-overweight expectation conditions, thereby indicating no
    support for concurrent accumulation.

    Synergistic accumulation occurs when the self-fulfilling effects
    of multiple perceivers are stronger in combination than their ad-
    ditive effects would suggest (Madon et al., 2004). The data would
    support synergistic accumulation if there was a larger increase in
    targets’ confirmatory behavior between the single- and double-
    overweight expectation conditions than between the no- and
    single-overweight expectation conditions. The results reported
    above confirmed this pattern. The difference between the amount
    of candy taken in the double- versus the single-overweight expec-
    tation conditions was larger in magnitude (d � 0.45, 95% CI [0.14,
    0.76]) than the difference between the single- versus the no-
    overweight expectation conditions (d � 0.01, 95% CI [�0.30,
    0.32]), with neither effect size included within the confidence
    interval of the other.

    These results were verified with a stepwise regression analysis
    using forward selection with two predictors: a linear term that
    corresponded to the number of perceivers in a triad who believed
    the target was heavy (i.e., 0, 1, and 2), and a quadratic term that
    was the square of the linear term (i.e., 0, 1, and 4). A significant
    and positive linear effect would support the presence of concurrent
    accumulation. A significant and positive quadratic effect would
    support the presence of synergistic accumulation. Because forward
    selection enters variables into the model one at a time, leading with
    the one that explains the greatest variance in the dependent vari-
    able, it permitted the quadratic term to be considered prior to the
    linear term, which is an appropriate analytic strategy under some
    conditions (Rawlings, Pantula, & Dickey, 2001). In the current
    research it was appropriate because neither accumulation process
    had theoretical priority and each process could occur in combina-
    tion with, or in the absence of, the other. In addition, because of
    multicollinearity, artificially forcing the linear term as the first
    predictor in the model would bias the test against the quadratic
    term. The results indicated that the quadratic term explained a
    significant proportion of variance in the amount of candy taken,
    t � 3.15, p � .002, whereas the linear term did not meet the entry
    criterion, t � 0.85, p � .398. This result confirms the presence of
    synergistic, but not concurrent, accumulation.

    Discussion

    The results of Experiment 1 supported the hypothesis that the
    self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes can accumulate across perceiv-
    ers. In Phase 1, individual perceivers selected bins of candy that
    afforded the target a greater opportunity to confirm the overweight

    stereotype when they believed the target was heavy versus thin. In
    Phase 2, targets received a single bowl of candy that combined the
    candy selections of two independent perceivers whose beliefs
    about the target’s weight had been systematically varied. Because
    of the way individual perceivers treated the target in Phase 1, this
    procedure created a situation in which targets’ opportunity to
    confirm the stereotype in Phase 2 was lowest in the no-overweight
    expectation condition, intermediate in the single-overweight ex-
    pectation condition, and greatest in the double-overweight expec-
    tation condition. However, targets did not more strongly confirm
    the stereotype in the single- than no-overweight expectation con-
    dition despite a greater affordance to do so, thus providing no
    evidence of the stereotype’s self-fulfilling effect within dyadic
    relations.

    Nevertheless, there was evidence of the stereotype’s cumulative
    self-fulfilling effect. Targets behaviorally confirmed the over-
    weight stereotype more strongly in the double- than single-
    overweight expectation condition. One might wonder, however,
    whether this result truly reflects the accumulation of the stereo-
    type’s self-fulfilling effect or a mere artifact of the procedures.
    That is, didn’t the tendency for perceivers to disproportionately
    select bins that contained two (Bin 1) and four (Bin 2) pieces of
    candy for a target whom they believed was thin create a ceiling
    effect that limited how much candy targets in the no- and single-
    overweight expectation conditions could take? And, if this hap-
    pened, doesn’t it mean that these targets had no choice but to
    confirm the overweight stereotype less than targets in the double-
    overweight expectation condition? The answer to both questions
    is no.

    The greatest potential for a ceiling effect was in the no-
    overweight expectation condition because it was here that targets
    received the least amount of candy overall. However, even in this
    condition targets received more candy than they elected to take. On
    average, these targets took about three pieces of candy even though
    nearly half of them received 22 pieces of candy or more, and none
    received fewer than 6 pieces. Hence, the amount of candy targets
    in the no-overweight expectation condition received did not se-
    verely restrict how much candy they could take. In fact, had they
    wanted to, they all could have taken the average amount of candy
    taken by targets in the double-overweight expectation condition
    (4.99 pieces), though generally they did not do so, and nearly half
    of them could have taken the maximum amount of candy taken by
    any target in the double-overweight expectation (20 pieces),
    though none did.

    In addition, if a ceiling effect had been responsible for the
    results, then targets in the no-overweight expectation condition
    should have taken less candy than targets in the single-overweight
    expectation condition. But, that did not happen either. Despite that
    targets in the no-overweight expectation condition received an
    average of 10 fewer pieces of candy than targets in the single-
    overweight expectation condition, the amount of candy targets
    took in these conditions was nearly identical. Finally, in no case
    were targets compelled to confirm the stereotype. Even in the
    double-overweight expectation condition targets could have cho-
    sen not to take any candy. The fact that few targets made this
    choice indicates that targets in this condition confirmed the ste-
    reotype even though they did not have to. On empirical grounds,
    therefore, we conclude that ceiling effects cannot explain away the
    support we obtained for accumulation.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    830 MADON ET AL.

    Theoretical considerations also lead us to conclude that the
    results reflected a bona fide accumulation effect. The amount of
    candy targets received across the expectation conditions mattered.
    Perceivers, acting on their stereotypic expectations, constructed a
    situation that afforded some targets greater opportunity to confirm
    the stereotype than others. Although this means that targets’ con-
    firmatory behavior was somewhat dependent on perceivers’ be-
    havior, this is no artifact; this is precisely the process hypothesized
    to create a self-fulfilling prophecy. According to all theoretical
    accounts, perceivers treating targets in line with their false expec-
    tations is a necessary step of the self-fulfilling prophecy process
    (e.g., Darley & Fazio, 1980). Thus, the dependency that existed
    between perceivers’ and targets’ behavior in this experiment is an
    inherent component of the underlying self-fulfilling prophecy pro-
    cess and does not invalidate the support we found for accumula-
    tion.

    It is possible, however, that the dependency contributed to the
    pattern of synergistic accumulation that characterized the data. The
    amount of candy targets received increased across the no-, single-,
    and double-overweight expectation conditions, but differences
    between the conditions were not equal. On average, 10 pieces of
    candy separated the no- and single-overweight expectation condi-
    tions, whereas 15 pieces separated the single- and double-
    overweight expectation conditions. Because of the inherent depen-
    dency between perceivers’ and targets’ behavior, it is possible that
    the upward trend in perceivers’ behavior caused the synergistic
    pattern of accumulation that was present in targets’ confirmatory
    behavior. Stated differently, it is conceivable that targets’ confir-
    matory behavior might have evidenced concurrent accumulation
    had perceivers’ behavior been linear.

    The implication of this interpretation is that targets were passive
    recipients of the situational affordances with which they were
    provided. Although the present data cannot rule out this possi-
    bility, targets are typically conceptualized as active players in
    the self-fulfilling prophecy process (Snyder & Stukas, 1999). In
    the current research, targets may have actively contributed to the
    stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect through their con-
    strual of the situational affordance. For example, targets in the
    double-overweight expectation condition may have presumed that
    they could take many pieces of candy without detection, or rea-
    soned that there was no need to refrain since there was plenty of
    candy to go around. Conversely, targets in the no- and single-
    overweight expectation conditions might have assumed that any
    candy they took would likely be noticed, or felt that they should
    take only a few pieces so that others could have some too. Though
    speculative, the idea that targets contributed to the stereotype’s
    synergistic effects via construal processes fits current conceptual-
    izations of the self-fulfilling prophecy, as well as a large body of
    psychological research showing that people actively interpret so-
    cial reality (Darley & Fazio, 1980; Fiske, 2004; Snyder & Stukas,
    1999).

    Finally, even though we observed synergistic accumulation with
    a laboratory procedure that involved count-based behaviors, we are
    disinclined to attribute the effect solely to the method. The reason
    for our skepticism is that Madon et al. (2004) observed similar
    results in a naturalistic study involving parents and their adolescent
    children. We are not arguing that the same underlying process
    necessarily operated in both studies. Because the two studies used
    different methods, several processes that were prevented from

    operating in the current research could have operated in Madon et
    al.’s study. Nevertheless, the fact that both studies found evidence
    of synergistic accumulation does suggest that the effect is not
    merely a laboratory phenomenon dependent on count-based be-
    haviors.

    Overview of Experiments 2 and 3

    Experiments 2 and 3 tested the accumulation hypothesis with
    respect to sex stereotypes, and did so with a method first used in
    the self-fulfilling prophecy research of Word et al. (1974). To
    summarize, Word et al. performed two experiments that tested the
    self-fulfilling effect of racial stereotypes on the interview perfor-
    mance of job applicants. In the first experiment, White participants
    interviewed either an African American or a White confederate
    who posed as a job applicant. Consistent with the idea that stereo-
    types can bias perceivers’ behavior toward targets, participants
    used a less favorable interview style when interviewing the Afri-
    can American than White confederate.

    In the second experiment, White participants played the role of
    the job applicant and White confederates played the role of the
    interviewer. The interviewers were trained to use either the less
    favorable interview style that participants in the first experiment
    used when interviewing the African American confederate, or the
    more favorable interview style that participants in the first exper-
    iment used when interviewing the White confederate. The results
    showed that participants in the second experiment performed
    worse during the interview when they were subjected to the less
    favorable than more favorable interview style. Thus, the way that
    the African American confederates had been treated in the first
    experiment undermined the interview performance of participants
    in the second experiment.

    The method Word et al. (1974) used is referred to as a double-
    randomization design because it involved the sequential manipu-
    lation of two variables (MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007).
    First, Word et al. manipulated a job applicant’s race to test whether
    racial stereotypes influenced perceivers’ interview style. Second,
    they manipulated perceivers’ interview style to test whether it
    influenced the interview performance of job applicants. Double-
    randomization designs are valued because they experimentally test
    the effect of a mediator variable, thereby ruling out alternative
    explanations for the link between a prior manipulation and a
    subsequently measured dependent variable (MacKinnon, Cheong,
    & Pirlott, 2012; MacKinnon et al., 2007). With this strength in
    mind, we used a double-randomization design in Experiments 2
    and 3. In Experiment 2, we manipulated whether perceivers de-
    veloped stereotype-consistent expectations about target to test
    whether it influenced their tendency to provide the target with a
    situational affordance that encouraged sex-typed behavior. In Ex-
    periment 3, we manipulated the number of perceivers who pro-
    vided a target with a situational affordance that encouraged sex-
    typed behavior to test whether sex stereotypes had cumulative
    self-fulfilling effects.

    Experiment 2

    Experiment 2 constituted the first half of our double-
    randomization design. It tested whether sex stereotypes can lead
    perceivers to provide targets with situational affordances that en-

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    831CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    courage confirmation of the stereotype. Participants read about a
    fictitious target who was described as a sex-typed woman, a
    gender-neutral individual, or a sex-typed man. Afterward, partic-
    ipants selected articles for the target to read from an article pool
    that included a mix of sex-typed and gender-neutral articles. Par-
    ticipants’ article selections indexed the degree to which they
    provided the target with a situational affordance that encouraged
    sex-typed behavior.

    Method

    Participants. Undergraduates (N � 123) at Rutgers Univer-
    sity participated to fulfill a course requirement. There were 52
    women and 71 men, including 10 African Americans, 29 Asians/
    Asian Americans, 64 European Americans, 9 Latina/os, and 11
    participants who self-described as “other.”

    Experimental design. Participants were randomly assigned to
    one of three expectation conditions. In the feminine expectation
    condition (n � 42), participants read a profile about a stereotypical
    woman. In the gender-neutral expectation condition (n � 43),
    participants read a profile about a gender-neutral, sex-unidentified
    person. In the masculine expectation condition (n � 38), participants
    read a profile about a stereotypical man. Although participants
    believed the profile described another participant who would be
    their partner during the experiment, the partner was actually fic-
    titious. Accordingly, participants acted as perceivers, and the fic-
    titious partner was the target.

    Materials and measures.
    Profile. A handwritten profile described the target’s name,

    age, hobbies, and employment. In the feminine expectation con-
    dition, the profile described a sex-typed target named Jessica who
    enjoyed gymnastics and going shopping with friends, and who
    worked as a babysitter in high school and at a cosmetics counter in
    a department store during the summer. In the gender-neutral ex-
    pectation condition, the profile described a gender-neutral target
    named Jesse who enjoyed swimming and hanging out with friends,
    and who worked as a cashier at a local grocery store in high school
    and as a lifeguard during the summer. In the masculine expectation
    condition, the profile described a sex-typed target named Michael
    who enjoyed playing lacrosse and video games, and who worked
    as a landscaper in high school and at a gas station during the
    summer.

    Perceiver behavior. To assess whether perceivers provided
    the target with a situational affordance that encouraged confirma-
    tion of sex stereotypes, perceivers selected three magazine and
    newspaper articles for the target to read. Perceivers selected these
    articles from one of two article pools. We used two article pools
    rather than one to prepare for the procedures of

    Experiment 3

    which, as we describe later, required two separate and different
    sets of articles to simulate the behavior of two independent per-
    ceivers. The article pools both presented the titles and brief sum-
    maries of nine articles, including three stereotypically feminine
    articles, three gender-neutral articles, and three stereotypically
    masculine articles that were matched for length. The stereotypi-
    cally feminine articles dealt with fashion, relationships, and health
    and beauty. The gender-neutral articles dealt with travel, food, and
    entertainment. The stereotypically masculine articles dealt with
    mechanics, science, and space exploration. We assigned a value
    of �1 to each selected stereotypically feminine article, a value of

    0 to each selected gender-neutral article, and a value of �1 to each
    selected stereotypically masculine article, and then summed these
    coded values to create a single value per perceiver. Higher values
    indicated that perceivers selected a greater number of stereotypical
    feminine articles for the target to read. We subsequently refer to
    this variable as article selection.

    Manipulation check. Two trait judgments assessed whether
    the expectation manipulation induced stereotype-consistent expec-
    tations: “How masculine vs. feminine is your partner?” and “How
    manly vs. womanly is your partner?” Perceivers responded on
    7-point scales with anchors 1 (very masculine; very manly), 4
    (gender-neutral), and 7 (very feminine; very womanly). To reduce
    suspicion, these questions were embedded among fillers that as-
    sessed non-sex-typed trait judgments and interests. Responses to
    the two sex-typed judgments were averaged to create a new
    variable (� � .68). Higher values indicated greater perceived
    femininity.

    Suspicion check. Perceivers reported what they believed the
    experiment was about.

    Procedures. After obtaining informed consent, the experi-
    menter described the study as an exploration into how well people
    can predict the likes, dislikes, and abilities of another person about
    whom they know little. Perceivers expected to meet with another
    participant who would be their partner during the study. In antic-
    ipation of the meeting, perceivers completed a survey that assessed
    their name, age, hobbies, and employment under the guise that
    their responses would be given to their partner (i.e., the fictitious
    target) whom they believed was completing the same survey in
    another room. After completing the survey, the experimenter col-
    lected it, exited the room, and returned with what appeared to be
    their partner’s survey, but which was actually the target’s profile.
    Perceivers were told to study their partner’s survey because they
    would use it to select activities for their partner to perform.
    Perceivers then received an article pool from which they selected
    three articles that they believed their partner would most enjoy
    reading. The experimenter explained that the perceiver and partner
    would later discuss how much the partner actually enjoyed reading
    the selected articles. Perceivers subsequently completed the ma-
    nipulation and suspicion checks, reported demographic informa-
    tion, and were debriefed.

    Results

    Preliminary analyses.
    Descriptive statistics. Supplemental Table 3 presents descrip-

    tive statistics.
    Manipulation check. A one-way ANOVA followed by three

    Tukey’s HSD contrasts tested whether the expectation manipula-
    tion induced perceivers to hold stereotype-consistent expectations
    about the target. The independent variable was the expectation
    (feminine vs. gender-neutral vs. masculine). The dependent vari-
    able was perceivers’ judgments of the target’s sex-typed traits. The
    ANOVA results and pattern of means supported the manipula-
    tion’s effectiveness. Perceivers judged the target as most feminine
    in the feminine expectation condition (M � 4.88, SD � 1.12),
    intermediately feminine in the gender-neutral expectation condi-
    tion (M � 4.25, SD � 0.94), and least feminine in the masculine
    expectation condition (M � 2.86, SD � 1.01), F(2, 120) � 42.90,
    p � .001, �2 � .42, 90% CI [.30, .50]. These values all differed

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    832 MADON ET AL.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    significantly from one another, ts(120) � 2.74, ps � .019, ds �
    .61.

    Suspicion check. There was one suspicious perceiver. Re-
    moving this perceiver’s data did not meaningfully alter the results,
    and the data were retained in all of the analyses.

    Article pools. A 2 (article pool: pool one vs. pool two) 3
    (expectation condition: feminine vs. gender-neutral vs. masculine)
    ANOVA tested whether the two article pools differentially influ-
    enced perceivers’ article selections. Because neither the main
    effect of article pool nor the interaction between article pool and
    expectation was significant, Fs(1, 117) � 1.59, ps � .209, both
    �p

    2 � 0.03, we omitted article pool as a factor in the main analyses.
    Main analyses. A one-way ANOVA followed by three

    Tukey’s HSD contrasts tested whether perceivers afforded the
    target a greater opportunity to confirm sex stereotypes when the
    target was sex-typed versus gender-neutral. The independent vari-
    able was the expectation (feminine vs. gender-neutral vs. mascu-
    line). The dependent variable was article selection. The results
    showed that the number of stereotypically feminine articles
    selected by perceivers was highest in the feminine expectation
    condition (M � 1.64, SD � 1.48), intermediate in the gender-
    neutral expectation condition (M � 0.16, SD � 1.28), and lowest
    in the masculine expectation condition (M � �2.00, SD � 0.87),
    F(2, 120) � 93.70, p � .001, �2 � .61, 90% CI [.52, .67], with
    these values all differing significantly from one another: Feminine
    versus gender-neutral expectation conditions, t(120) � 5.38, p �
    .001, 95% CI [0.83, 2.14], d � 1.20, 95% CI [0.73, 1.67]; Femi-
    nine versus masculine expectation conditions, t(120) � 13.61, p �
    .001, 95% CI [3.01, 4.28], d � 2.95, 95% CI [2.38, 3.51]; Gender-
    neutral versus masculine expectation conditions, t(120) � 7.85,
    p � .001, 95% CI [1.51, 2.81], d � 1.75, 95% CI [1.26, 2.23].
    Table 2 reports the average number of stereotypically feminine,
    gender-neutral, and stereotypically masculine articles selected in
    each expectation condition.

    Because the number of women and men was not equal across
    the expectation conditions, we also examined whether perceivers’
    sex could account for the results, but found no evidence to support
    its influence. Neither the main effect of perceiver sex nor the
    Expectation Perceiver sex interaction had a significant effect on
    perceivers’ article selections, Fs � 2.36, ps � .127, all �p2 � .03,
    and in no case did the inclusion of perceiver sex meaningfully alter
    either the pattern or significance of the expectation’s effect. We
    also analyzed the data separately for women and men and found
    virtually identical results to those produced by the full sample.

    Discussion

    Perceivers in this experiment selected the greatest number of
    stereotypically feminine articles for Jessica, the sex-typed femi-
    nine target, the greatest number of stereotypically masculine arti-
    cles for Michael, the sex-typed masculine target, and an interme-
    diate number of sex-typed articles for Jesse, the gender-neutral
    target. These results clearly show that perceivers afforded the
    sex-typed targets greater opportunity to confirm sex stereotypes
    than they afforded the gender-neutral target. However, the cause of
    this effect is less clear. Because the procedures paired a sex-typed
    name with stereotype-consistent attributes in the feminine and
    masculine expectation conditions, perceivers’ article selections
    could have been influenced by sex stereotypes, or the target’s
    stereotype-consistent attributes. If it is the latter, then perhaps
    Experiment 2 is more about target-based expectations than
    stereotype-based expectations. For three reasons, we do not be-
    lieve this to be the case.

    First, the congruence between the target’s sex and the target’s
    stereotype-consistent attributes likely provided perceivers with
    strong justification to apply their stereotypes when making their
    article selections (Crandall & Eshleman, 2003; Fazio, 1990; Ma-
    don, Guyll, Hilbert, Kyriakatos, & Vogel, 2006). Second, the
    amount of stereotype-consistent attributes provided to perceivers
    was rather modest, which likely limited its influence even if
    perceivers were motivated to form target-based impressions.
    Third, we observed the same pattern in Experiment 1 even though
    the target’s attributes were held constant across the expectation
    conditions. These considerations suggest that perceivers in the
    current experiment likely relied on sex stereotypes to make their
    article selections. However, because this experiment cannot tease
    apart the relative influence of the target’s sex and the target’s
    attributes, it remains possible that different results might have
    emerged had the procedures varied only the target’s sex.

    Experiment 3

    Experiment 3 constituted the second half of our double-
    randomization design. Whereas Experiment 2 showed that indi-
    vidual perceivers provided sex-typed targets with a situational
    affordance that encouraged confirmation of sex stereotypes, Ex-
    periment 3 examined whether these affordances, if imposed by
    multiple perceivers, can cause the stereotype to have cumulative
    self-fulfilling effects. The experiment manipulated the number of
    perceivers who provided participants with a sex-typed situational

    Table 2
    Experiment 2: Average Number of Stereotypically Feminine, Gender-Neutral, and Stereotypically
    Masculine Articles Perceivers Selected for the Target

    Expectation
    condition

    Article type selected

    Stereotypically feminine Gender-neutral Stereotypically masculine

    M Mode SD M Mode SD M Mode SD

    Feminine 2.00 2 0.96 0.64 0 0.73 0.36 0 0.66
    Gender-neutral 0.68 0 0.84 1.79 2 0.84 0.53 0 0.69
    Masculine 0.07 0 0.26 0.86 0 0.86 2.07 2 0.83

    Note. M � mean; SD � standard deviation.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    833CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    affordance. Participants received a set of articles that they were
    told had been chosen for them by two participants who would be
    their partners during the experiment. The number of sex-typed and
    gender-neutral articles in the sets was varied to simulate the
    behavior of two perceivers from Experiment 2. The number of
    sex-typed articles participants selected to read indexed the degree
    to which they confirmed sex stereotypes.

    Method

    Participants. Undergraduates (N � 121) at Rutgers Univer-
    sity participated to fulfill a course requirement. There were 65
    women and 56 men. Race was not assessed.

    Design. Participants were randomly assigned to one of five
    conditions that manipulated the number of perceivers who pro-
    vided them with a sex-typed situational affordance. In two condi-
    tions, two perceivers provided participants with a situational af-
    fordance that encouraged confirmation of the stereotype of women
    (double-feminine, n � 26) or men (double-masculine, n � 25). In
    two other conditions, one perceiver provided participants with a
    situational affordance that encouraged confirmation of the ste-
    reotype of women (single-feminine, n � 25) or men (single-
    masculine, n � 20), whereas the other perceiver provided a situ-
    ational affordance that discouraged confirmation of sex
    stereotypes. In the gender-neutral condition (n � 25), two per-
    ceivers provided participants with a situational affordance that
    discouraged confirmation of sex stereotypes. To manipulate these
    affordances, participants received one of five article sets that
    included different numbers of sex-typed and gender-neutral arti-
    cles, and selected three to read. Hence, the participants acted as
    targets, and even though the two perceivers with whom they were
    paired were fictitious, the number of sex-typed and gender-neutral
    articles included in the article sets were based on the average
    article selections of perceivers in Experiment 2.

    Materials and measures.
    Article sets. Five article sets manipulated the situational af-

    fordances perceivers provided targets. Each article set showed the
    titles and brief summaries of six magazine and newspaper articles.
    The six articles in each set simulated the behavior of two perceiv-
    ers, each of whom ostensibly had selected three articles for the
    target to read. We varied the number of sex-typed articles in the
    sets based on the results of Experiment 2 (see Table 2). Specifi-
    cally, we rounded the average article selections of perceivers in the
    feminine and masculine expectation conditions of Experiment 2 to
    the nearest integer, and then paired these rounded values in dif-
    ferent permutations to simulate the behavior of two perceivers
    combined. We set the remaining articles to be gender-neutral.

    For example, consider the article sets used in the double- and
    single-feminine conditions. Table 2 reports that perceivers in the
    stereotypically feminine expectation condition of Experiment 2 se-
    lected an average of 2 stereotypically feminine articles and 0.36
    stereotypically masculine articles. To create the article set for the
    double-feminine condition in Experiment 3, we rounded these values
    to two and zero, respectively, and then doubled them to create an
    article set that included four stereotypically feminine articles and zero
    stereotypically masculine articles. We added two gender-neutral arti-
    cles to complete the set of six. This article set simulated the behavior
    of two independent perceivers, both of whom held a stereotypic
    expectation about the target.

    We did not double the rounded values when creating the article
    set for the single-feminine condition because it was intended to
    simulate the behavior of a single perceiver who held a stereotypic
    expectation about the target. Thus, it included two stereotypically
    feminine articles, zero stereotypically masculine articles, plus four
    gender-neutral articles to complete the set of six. The same pro-
    cedures created the remaining article sets: (a) double-masculine:
    zero stereotypically feminine articles, four stereotypically mascu-
    line articles, and two gender-neutral articles; (b) single-masculine:
    zero stereotypically feminine articles, two stereotypically mascu-
    line articles, and four gender-neutral articles; and (c) gender-
    neutral: one stereotypically feminine article, one stereotypically
    masculine article, and four gender-neutral articles.

    We used these procedures for three reasons. First, because
    perceivers in Experiment 2 could not select a fraction of an article
    for the target to read, rounding the average values to the nearest
    integer best represented the behavior a single perceiver acting
    independently. Likewise, combining the rounded values best rep-
    resented the combined behavior of two perceivers acting indepen-
    dently. Second, because the rounded values always equaled the
    modal response, they also best represented the typical behavior of
    individual perceivers in the stereotypically feminine and stereo-
    typically masculine expectation conditions of Experiment 2. Fi-
    nally, we set all of the remaining articles in the article sets to be
    gender-neutral so that all targets in the current experiment, even
    those in the double-feminine and double-masculine conditions,
    could predominantly disconfirm sex stereotypes.

    In considering these procedures, it might seem that targets in the
    current experiment should have had stereotypically masculine ar-
    ticles to select in the double- and single-feminine conditions, and
    stereotypically feminine articles to select in the double- and single-
    masculine conditions. In fact, however, the article sets represented
    a faithful transmission of the restricted situational affordances
    perceivers in Experiment 2 had provided the sex-typed targets.
    Although we could have used less restricted situational affor-
    dances than did perceivers in Experiment 2, doing so would have
    disrupted a key step of the self-fulfilling prophecy process—
    perceivers treating targets in line with their false expectations—
    and undermined the effect that we were trying to capture in the
    second half of our double-randomization design. The procedure we
    used to create the article sets avoided this problem.

    Target behavior. Targets selected three articles to read from
    the six article set.

    Profile. Targets completed a profile about themselves (name,
    age, hobbies, employment) that they believed would be given to
    their partners, reinforcing the cover story that the six articles in the
    set had been given to them by their partners.

    Suspicion check. Targets described what they believed the
    experiment was about.

    Procedures. The procedures matched those from Experiment
    2 with these exceptions. Targets expected to be paired with two
    other participants who would be their partners during the experi-
    ment. Targets were told that their partners were using their profiles
    to design activities for them, the first of which involved reading
    three magazine and newspaper articles from a set of six articles
    that had been chosen for them by their partners from a larger pool
    of articles on a variety of topics. Targets expected to discuss how
    much they enjoyed reading the articles with their partners later in
    the experiment. Targets then received the six articles that their

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    834 MADON ET AL.

    partners had supposedly chosen for them, and selected three to
    read. Afterward, targets completed the suspicion check, provided
    demographic information, and were then debriefed.

    Results

    Preliminary analyses.
    Descriptive statistics. Supplemental Table 4 presents descrip-

    tive statistics.
    Suspicion check. There were four suspicious targets. Remov-

    ing their data from the analyses did not meaningfully alter the
    results, and their data were retained in all of the analyses.

    Main analyses. Perceivers in Experiment 2 provided sex-
    typed targets with a situational affordance that encouraged confir-
    mation of sex stereotypes. In the current research, a series of
    analyses tested whether these situational affordances, if imposed
    by multiple perceivers, can cause sex stereotypes to have cumu-
    lative self-fulfilling effects on targets’ behavior. To allow for the
    possibility that the stereotypes of women and men might evidence
    different effects, the analyses separately analyzed the feminine and
    masculine conditions.

    Participant behavior. Two, separate, one-way ANOVAs
    tested whether the number of perceivers who provided targets with
    a sex-typed situational affordance influenced how strongly targets
    confirmed sex stereotypes. One ANOVA focused on the stereotype
    of women. It examined how many stereotypically feminine articles
    targets selected in the double-feminine, single-feminine, and gender-
    neutral conditions. The other ANOVA focused on the stereotype of
    men. It examined how many stereotypically masculine articles
    targets selected in the double-masculine, single-masculine, and
    gender-neutral conditions. Both ANOVAs were significant: for the
    stereotype of women, F(2, 73) � 23.28, p � .001, �2 � .39, 90%
    CI [.23, .49]; for the stereotype of men, F(2, 67) � 50.11, p �
    .001, �2 � .60, 90% CI [.46, .68]. These results indicate that the
    number of perceivers who provided targets with a sex-typed situ-
    ational affordance influenced how strongly targets confirmed sex
    stereotypes.

    Self-fulfilling prophecy effect. Next, two LSD contrasts
    tested whether the stereotypes of women and men had dyadic
    self-fulfilling effects. The contrast relevant to the stereotype of
    women compared the number of stereotypically feminine articles
    selected by targets in the gender-neutral and single-feminine con-
    ditions. The contrast relevant to the stereotype of men compared

    the number of stereotypically masculine articles selected by targets
    in the gender-neutral and single-masculine conditions. The results
    indicated that targets selected significantly more stereotypically
    feminine articles in the single-feminine than gender-neutral con-
    dition, t(73) � 4.41, p � .001, 95% CI [0.44, 1.16], d � 1.25, 95%
    CI [0.65, 1.83], and significantly more stereotypically masculine
    articles in the single-masculine than gender-neutral condition,
    t(69) � 2.34 p � .022, 95% CI [0.06, 0.80], d � 0.70, 95% CI
    [0.10, 1.30]. Table 3 presents the means. Thus, targets confirmed
    sex stereotypes more strongly when one perceiver provided them
    with a sex-typed situational affordance than when no perceiver had
    done so. These results replicate prior research showing that ste-
    reotypes can have self-fulfilling effects within dyadic relations
    (Snyder et al., 1977; Word et al., 1974).

    Accumulation. Two additional LSD contrasts tested whether
    the stereotypes of women and men had cumulative self-fulfilling
    effects. One contrast compared the number of stereotypically fem-
    inine articles selected by targets in the single- and double-feminine
    conditions. The other contrast compared the number of stereotypi-
    cally masculine articles selected by targets in the single- and
    double-masculine conditions. The results showed that targets se-
    lected significantly more stereotypically feminine articles in the
    double-feminine than single-feminine condition, t(73) � 2.27, p �
    .026, 95% CI [0.05, 0.77], d � 0.64, 95% CI [0.08, 1.19], and
    significantly more stereotypically masculine articles in the double-
    masculine than single-masculine condition, t(67) � 6.80, p � .001,
    95% CI [0.88, 1.62], d � 2.04, 95% CI [1.35, 2.72]. Table 3
    presents the means. These results support the accumulation hy-
    pothesis because they show that targets confirmed sex stereotypes
    more strongly when two perceivers provided them with a sex-
    typed situational affordance than when only one did.

    Pattern of accumulation. Additional analyses explored the
    pattern of these accumulation effects. For the stereotype of
    women, the data would support concurrent accumulation if targets
    selected an increasingly greater number of stereotypical feminine
    articles across the gender-neutral, single-feminine, and double-
    feminine conditions, respectively. Parallel increases in targets’
    selection of stereotypically masculine articles across the gender-
    neutral, single-masculine, and double-masculine conditions would
    support concurrent accumulation for the stereotype of men. The
    fact that the contrasts reported above supported the significance of

    Table 3
    Experiment 3: Average Number of Stereotypically Feminine, Gender-Neutral, and Stereotypically
    Masculine Articles Targets Selected to Read

    Experimental
    condition

    Article type selected

    Stereotypically
    feminine Gender-neutral

    Stereotypically
    masculine

    M SD M SD M SD

    Double-feminine 1.81 0.69 1.15 0.72 0.00 0.00
    Single-feminine 1.40 0.71 1.54 0.76 0.00 0.00
    Gender-neutral 0.60 0.50 1.88 0.53 0.52 0.51
    Single-masculine 0.00 0.00 1.95 0.80 0.95 0.69
    Double-masculine 0.00 0.00 0.77 0.65 2.20 0.65

    Note. M � mean; SD � standard deviation.
    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    835CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    these comparisons suggests that concurrent accumulation was
    present for both stereotypes.

    Because concurrent and synergistic accumulation can occur
    simultaneously, the analyses also tested whether the stereotypes
    evidenced synergistic accumulation. The results would support
    synergistic accumulation for the stereotype of women if the dif-
    ference between the number of stereotypically feminine articles
    selected by targets in the double-feminine versus single-feminine
    conditions was larger in magnitude than the difference between the
    number of stereotypically feminine articles selected by targets in
    the single-feminine versus gender-neutral conditions. A parallel
    pattern of differences in targets’ selection of masculine articles in
    the gender-neutral, single-masculine, and double-masculine con-
    ditions would support the presence of synergistic accumulation for
    the stereotype of men.

    The results did not support synergistic accumulation for the
    stereotype of women. In fact, the pattern was in the opposite
    direction. As reported above, the magnitude of the difference
    between the double- and single-feminine conditions was smaller
    (d � 0.64, 95% CI [0.08, 1.19]) than the magnitude of the
    difference between the single-feminine and gender-neutral condi-
    tions (d � 1.25, 95% CI [0.65, 1.83]). A forward selection regres-
    sion analysis confirmed this result. Whereas a linear term corre-
    sponding to the number of perceivers who provided targets with a
    feminine sex-typed situational affordance (i.e., 0, 1, and 2) ex-
    plained a significant proportion of variance in the number of
    stereotypically feminine articles selected by targets, t � 6.68, p �
    .001, a quadratic term that was the square of the linear term (i.e.,
    0, 1, and 4) did not meet the entry criterion, t � 1.25, p � .214.
    Thus, the stereotype of women evidenced concurrent, but not
    synergistic, accumulation.

    By contrast, the stereotype of men did evidence synergistic
    accumulation. As reported above, the magnitude of the difference
    between the double- and single-masculine conditions was larger
    (d � 2.04, 95% CI [1.35, 2.72]) than the magnitude of the
    difference between the single-masculine and gender-neutral con-
    ditions (d � 0.70, 95% CI [0.10, 1.30]), with neither effect size
    included within the confidence interval of the other. Moreover, a
    forward-selection stepwise regression analysis that included a lin-
    ear term corresponding to the number of perceivers who provided
    targets with a masculine sex-typed situational affordance (i.e., 0, 1,
    and 2), and a quadratic term that was the square of the linear term
    (i.e., 0, 1, and 4) indicated that the quadratic term explained a
    significant proportion of variance in the number of stereotypically
    masculine articles selected, t � 10.09, p � .001, whereas the linear
    term did not meet the entry criterion, t � 0.60, p � .953. This
    result helps to clarify the contrast results reported above for the
    stereotype of men; although differences between the means sug-
    gested the presence of concurrent accumulation, those differences
    were predominantly driven by a strong quadratic effect indicative
    of synergistic accumulation. Synergistic accumulation, therefore,
    best characterized the stereotype of men.

    Participant sex. Because there were not equal numbers of
    women and men in the conditions, target sex was added to the
    ANOVAs described above to test whether it could account for the
    results. The main effect of target sex was significant, with women
    selecting significantly more stereotypically feminine articles than
    men (Ms � 1.43 vs. 1.11) and fewer stereotypically masculine
    articles than men (Ms � 0.88 vs. 1.73), Fs � 14.05, ps � .001,

    both �p2 � .17. However, target sex never interacted with the
    experimental manipulation, Fs � 1.37, ps � .260, both �p2 � .04,
    nor did its inclusion meaningfully alter the manipulation’s effect.
    We also reanalyzed the data separately for women and men and
    found virtually identical results to those produced by the full
    sample.

    Discussion

    Experiment 3 showed that targets confirmed sex-stereotypes
    more strongly when two perceivers provided them with a sex-
    typed situational affordance than when only one had. Because the
    results were the product of a double-randomization design, they
    also showed that a main reason targets in Experiment 3 confirmed
    sex stereotypes was because they were encouraged to do so by the
    situational affordances that perceivers in Experiment 2 constructed
    for them. Indeed, a primary way that perceivers are hypothesized
    to elicit confirmatory behavior from targets is by constructing
    situations that afford them opportunities and constraints that en-
    courage an expectancy-consistent response. Our results were con-
    sistent with this hypothesized mechanism.

    Of course, if the situational affordances were so restrictive that
    they prevented targets from disconfirming the stereotypes, then the
    observed effects would not be particularly interesting. However,
    this was not the case. All targets in Experiment 3 could have
    predominantly disconfirmed sex stereotypes. For example, targets
    in the single-feminine and single-masculine conditions could have
    chosen all gender-neutral articles, thus avoiding the sex-typed
    articles entirely, and targets in the double-feminine and double-
    masculine conditions were not required to select more than a single
    sex-typed article. In no case, therefore, were targets compelled to
    confirm sex stereotypes. Nevertheless, they did confirm sex ste-
    reotypes and, critically, they did so more than the situational
    affordances required.

    Although the data cannot explain why the situational affor-
    dances had this effect, it may have to do with the way that targets
    construed the situation (Darley & Fazio, 1980; Snyder & Stukas,
    1999). Targets believed that their partners had chosen articles for
    them on the basis of their profiles, and expected to discuss with
    their partners how much they enjoyed reading a subset of them.
    Thus, in targets’ minds, the articles appeared to reflect their
    partners’ perceptions of them, and the articles they chose to read
    appeared to have interpersonal consequences. Given these param-
    eters, targets might have gone along with the situational affor-
    dances for two reasons.

    First, targets’ belief that their partners selected articles for them
    to read on the basis of their profiles may have led them to engage
    in a process akin to the confirmation bias; they may have read the
    article summaries with an eye toward seeing how their interests
    aligned with the articles, a strategy that may have caused them to
    see connections that they otherwise would have missed. In turn,
    these connections may have increased their willingness to read
    sex-typed articles that they might have rejected under different
    circumstances. Second, targets may have wanted to affiliate with
    their partners in the anticipated meeting and perceived this goal to
    be more achievable if they chose the most prevalent type of
    article in the article sets. For example, targets in the double-
    feminine condition may have selected to read more stereotypi-
    cally feminine articles than required to make the anticipated meet-

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    836 MADON ET AL.

    ing go more smoothly. This interpretation is consistent with the
    established finding that targets are more susceptible to self-
    fulfilling prophecies when they are motivated to get along with
    perceivers (Snyder & Stukas, 1999).

    Experiment 4

    Experiments 1, 2, and 3 supported the hypothesis that the
    self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes can accumulate across perceiv-
    ers. Perceivers treated targets in stereotypic ways, and the greater
    the number of perceivers who did so, the more strongly targets
    confirmed a stereotype. We performed Experiment 4 to examine a
    potential downstream consequence of this effect: Once the cumu-
    lative self-fulfilling effect of a stereotype has been set in motion,
    it may influence the impressions that new perceivers form about a
    target, thereby bringing the self-fulfilling prophecy process full
    circle. One process through which this could occur is the funda-
    mental attribution error (Jones & Davis, 1965; Ross, 1977).

    Perceivers do not always take situational factors into account as
    much as they should when trying to understand the causes of a
    target’s behavior. Instead, they tend to assume that a target’s
    behavior corresponds to an underlying disposition. Perceivers’
    tendency to discount situational factors in favor of correspondent
    inferences may perpetuate a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling
    effect in the following way: New perceivers who observe a target
    behave in a stereotypic way may misattribute the behavior to an
    underlying disposition when it was actually caused by a stereo-
    type’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect. Such a tendency might
    even occur among perceivers who are aware that prior perceivers
    encouraged the target’s stereotypic behavior. After all, some of the
    earliest attribution research established that perceivers make cor-
    respondent inferences about targets who they know lacked free
    choice (Jones & Harris, 1967). This finding raises the possibility
    that new perceivers will assume that targets’ behavior reflects their
    dispositions even when they know that the behavior was shaped by
    the actions of others. Consistent with this possibility, we tested two
    interrelated hypotheses in Experiment 4.

    One hypothesis was that a stereotype’s self-fulfilling effect can
    influence the way new perceivers judge targets. Although this is
    not the first time such an effect has been examined (Snyder &
    Swann, 1978), it does underscore a theoretically important point
    that has been overlooked; perceivers who judge targets solely on
    the basis of individuating information (a process referred to as
    individuation) may develop stereotypic impressions when targets’
    individuating information was caused by a stereotype’s self-
    fulfilling effect (Jussim, 2012). Thus, individuation (which, by
    definition, involves no stereotyping) can cause perceivers to form
    stereotypic impressions, thereby setting the stage for future restric-
    tions on targets’ opportunities.

    The other hypothesis was that perceivers do not appropriately
    adjust their impressions even when they know that targets’ stereo-
    typic behavior was shaped by the actions of others. This hypothesis
    represents a novel test of the fundamental attribution error because
    it conceptualizes it as a mechanism through which a stereotype’s
    cumulative self-fulfilling effect can transcend social interactions.
    We tested these hypotheses with respect to sex stereotypes, focus-
    ing on the common beliefs that women and men have different
    personality traits, academic competencies, career aptitudes, and
    suitability for careers (Haines, Deaux, & Lofaro, 2016).

    Method

    Participants. Undergraduates (N � 230) at Rutgers Univer-
    sity participated to fulfill a course requirement. There were 138
    women, 90 men, and 2 participants who omitted a response,
    including 16 African Americans, 65 Asians/Asian Americans, 112
    European Americans, 15 Latina/os, 19 participants who reported
    “other,” and 3 participants who omitted a response.

    Design. Participants were randomly assigned to a 5 (target
    behavior: strong-feminine vs. moderate-feminine vs. gender-
    neutral vs. moderate-masculine vs. strong-masculine) 2 (situa-
    tional awareness: aware vs. unaware) between-subjects experimen-
    tal design. Target behavior manipulated how strongly a target
    confirmed sex stereotypes by varying the number of stereotypi-
    cally feminine and masculine articles the target selected to read
    from a group of articles that afforded sex-typed behavior to vary-
    ing degrees. Situational awareness manipulated whether partici-
    pants were made aware of the situational affordances that gave rise
    to the target’s article selections. Although the target was fictitious,
    participants believed the target would be their partner during the
    experiment, and the target’s article selections, though experimen-
    tally manipulated, were based on the average article selections of
    participants in Experiment 3. Therefore, participants acted as per-
    ceivers and the fictitious partner was the target.

    Materials and measures.
    Article selection lists. Five article selection lists manipulated

    target behavior. Each list included the titles and brief summaries of
    three magazine and newspaper articles that the target had purport-
    edly selected to read. The number of stereotypically feminine,
    gender-neutral, and stereotypically masculine articles in the lists
    matched the average number of stereotypically feminine, gender
    neutral, and stereotypically masculine articles, rounded to the
    nearest integer, that had been selected by targets in the five
    conditions of Experiment 3. For example, Table 3 shows that
    targets in the double-feminine condition of Experiment 3 chose an
    average of 1.81 stereotypically feminine articles, 1.15 gender-
    neutral articles, and 0 stereotypically masculine articles. These
    values were rounded to the nearest integer to create the article
    selection list for the strong-feminine condition in the current
    experiment: two stereotypically feminine articles, one gender-
    neutral article, and zero stereotypically masculine articles.

    The same procedure created the remaining article selection lists:
    (a) strong-masculine condition: zero stereotypically feminine arti-
    cles, one gender-neutral article, and two stereotypically masculine
    articles; (b) moderate-feminine condition: one stereotypically fem-
    inine article, two gender-neutral articles, and zero stereotypically
    masculine articles; (c) moderate-masculine condition: zero stereo-
    typically feminine articles, two gender-neutral articles, and one
    stereotypically masculine article; (d) gender-neutral condition: one
    stereotypically feminine article, two gender-neutral articles, and
    one stereotypically masculine article. Note that the absence of
    counterstereotypic articles in the strong and moderate conditions
    was a direct consequence of perceivers’ behavior in Experiment 2.
    That is, because perceivers in Experiment 2 overwhelmingly chose
    not to select any counterstereotypic articles for the sex-typed
    targets to read, targets in Experiment 3 had none to select in any
    but the gender-neutral condition. The behavior of perceivers in
    Experiment 2, therefore, had a cascading effect that was repre-
    sented here in the target’s article selections.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    837CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    Articles. The experiment manipulated situational awareness
    by giving perceivers in the aware and unaware conditions different
    information about the group of articles from which the target
    selected three to read. Perceivers in the aware conditions received
    one of the five article sets from Experiment 3, all of which
    conveyed the target’s restricted choices: In the strong-feminine
    condition they received the article set from the double-feminine
    condition of Experiment 3 (four stereotypically feminine articles,
    two gender-neutral articles, and zero stereotypically masculine
    articles). In the strong-masculine condition they received the arti-
    cle set from the double-masculine condition of Experiment 3 (zero
    stereotypically feminine articles, two gender-neutral articles, and
    four stereotypically masculine articles). In the moderate-feminine
    and moderate-masculine conditions they received the article sets
    from the single-feminine (two stereotypically feminine articles,
    four gender-neutral articles, and zero stereotypically masculine)
    or single-masculine (zero stereotypically feminine articles, four
    gender-neutral articles, and two stereotypically masculine arti-
    cles) conditions of Experiment 3, respectively. In the gender-
    neutral condition they received the article set from the gender-
    neutral condition of Experiment 3 (one stereotypically feminine
    article, four gender-neutral articles, and one stereotypically mas-
    culine article). Perceivers in the unaware conditions received the
    18-article pool given to perceivers in Experiment 2 (six stereotypi-
    cally feminine, six gender-neutral, six stereotypically masculine
    articles). Because these articles reflected diverse interests, they
    gave the impression that the target had considerable choice when
    selecting which three articles to read.

    Measures.
    Perceiver judgments. Perceivers judged the target along four

    dimensions that are relevant to sex stereotypes, including sex-
    typed traits, general academic competency, aptitude in the tradi-
    tionally masculine fields of math and science, and career suitabil-
    ity.

    Sex-typed traits. Ten questions assessed perceivers’ judg-
    ments of the target’s sex-typed traits. Two questions used a 7-point
    bipolar scale to assess how gentle versus rough and emotional
    versus logical they judged the target with anchors 1 (very gentle or
    very emotional) and 7 (very rough or very logical). The rest used
    a 7-point response scale with anchors 1 (not at all) and 7 (very) to
    assess the degree to perceivers judged the target to possess four
    stereotypically feminine traits (attuned to others feelings, femi-
    nine, sensitive, and submissive), and four stereotypically mascu-
    line traits (confident, masculine, independent, and assertive). Re-
    sponses were reverse scored as necessary and then averaged to
    create one score per perceiver (� � .76). Higher scores reflected
    the perception that the target possessed more stereotypically fem-
    inine traits.

    General academic competency. Four questions assessed per-
    ceivers’ judgments of the target’s general academic competency:
    (a) How strong is your partner’s overall academic performance?
    (b) Would your partner be confident in her or his general academic
    abilities? (c) How intelligent is your partner?, and (d) How smart
    do you consider your partner? Perceivers responded on 7-point
    scales with anchors 1 (not at all) and 7 (very). Responses were
    averaged to create one score per perceiver (� � .88), with higher
    values reflecting the perception that the target had greater general
    academic competency.

    Aptitude in math and science. Four questions assessed per-
    ceivers’ judgments of the target’s aptitude in the domains of math
    and science: (a) Would your partner be confident in her or his math
    abilities? (b) Would your partner be confident in her or his scien-
    tific abilities? (c) Would your partner be good at math?, and (d)
    Would your partner be good at science? Perceivers responded on
    7-point scales with anchors 1 (not at all) and 7 (very). Responses
    were averaged to create one score per perceiver (� � .90). Higher
    values reflected the perception that the target had more aptitude for
    these traditionally masculine domains.

    Career suitability. To assess perceivers’ judgments of the
    target’s career-suitability, perceivers answered four questions rel-
    evant to careers with sex-typed subspecialties. For example, one
    question asked “ If your partner was a doctor, would she or he be
    better at surgery or dermatology?” Perceivers responded to this
    question on a 7-point scale with anchors 1 (surgery) and 7 (der-
    matology). The other careers and subspecialties were high school
    teacher (science teacher vs. English teacher), trades person (me-
    chanic vs. hair-stylist), and sales person (car sales person vs.
    cosmetic sales person). The subspecialties of dermatology, English
    teacher, hairstylist, and cosmetic sales person corresponded to
    feminine sex-typed subspecialties. Responses were averaged to
    create one score per perceiver (� � .86). Higher values indicated
    that perceivers judged the target as better-suited for stereotypically
    feminine careers.

    Manipulation checks. Seven questions assessed whether per-
    ceivers had adequately attended to the target’s article selections.
    One question asked perceivers to freely recall the titles of the
    target’s article selections. The other six questions used a multiple-
    choice format to assess perceivers’ memory for the article sum-
    maries, which described the three articles selected by the target.
    Each multiple-choice question had only one correct response.
    Responses to the multiple-choice questions were coded as 0 (in-
    correct) or 1 (correct) and then summed to create one variable per
    perceiver that equaled the total number of correct responses.

    Suspicion check. Perceivers reported what they believed the
    experiment was about.

    Procedures. The procedures matched those from Experiment
    3 with these exceptions. Perceivers expected to receive informa-
    tion about activities their partner had performed, and to judge their
    partner’s interests, characteristics, and abilities. Following this
    cover story, perceivers were told that their partner had already
    received the titles and brief summaries of a set of magazine and
    newspaper articles and, from this set, had chosen three articles to
    read. Perceivers then received information about the articles that
    had ostensibly been available for their partner to choose, and an
    article selection list that showed the three articles their partner
    ultimately selected to read. Perceivers next judged their partner on
    the four stereotype-relevant dimensions, after which they com-
    pleted the manipulation and suspicion checks, and were then
    debriefed.

    Results

    Preliminary analyses.
    Descriptive statistics. Supplemental Table 5 presents descrip-

    tive statistics.
    Manipulation checks. There were 211 perceivers (93%) who

    correctly listed all three article titles, 159 (69%) who correctly

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    838 MADON ET AL.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    answered all six multiple choice questions concerning these arti-
    cles, and 149 (65%) who scored perfectly on both measures.
    Although a few perceivers correctly listed either zero (n � 9, 4%)
    or one (n � 3, 1%) article title, these perceivers still performed
    reasonably well on the multiple choice questions, answering at
    least four of the six questions correctly. Thus, perceivers had
    adequately attended to the target’s article selections.

    Suspicion check. There were four suspicious perceivers. Re-
    moving their data did not meaningfully alter the results, and their
    data were retained in all of the analyses.

    Main analyses. The data were first analyzed with a 5 (target
    behavior) 2 (situational awareness) MANOVA in which the
    dependent variables were judgments of the target’s sex-typed
    traits, general academic competency, aptitude in math and science,
    and career suitability. There was a substantial main effect for target
    behavior, Wilks’
    � .374, F(16, 642.20) � 15.25, p � .001, �p2 �
    .22. Neither the main effect of situational awareness, Wilks’

    .992, F(4, 210) � 0.41, p � .800, �p2 � .01, nor the Target
    behavior Situational awareness interaction, Wilks’
    � .944,
    F(16, 642.20) � 0.76, p � .733, �p2 � .01, achieved significance.
    Four ANOVAs replicated these results (Supplemental Table 6),
    and yielded significant linear relations between target behavior and
    each dependent variable, ts � 7.30, ps � .001. Tukey’s HSD
    contrasts further supported these linear trends (Supplemental Table
    7). Table 4 presents the means for target behavior.

    These results reveal two clear patterns. First, perceivers judged
    the target as having more stereotypically feminine characteristics
    and abilities the greater the number of stereotypically feminine
    articles selected by the target. Second, this effect was even present
    among perceivers who were made aware of the target’s con-
    strained choices. These results support the hypotheses that a ste-
    reotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect can have ripple effects
    that influence the way new perceivers judge a target, and that these
    ripple effects are not mitigated by perceivers’ awareness that a
    target’s behavior was shaped by the actions of others.

    Because there were not equal numbers of women and men in the
    10 conditions, perceiver sex was added to the MANOVA de-
    scribed above to examine its effect. There was a significant main
    effect of perceiver sex, Wilks’
    � .951, F(4, 199) � 2.54, p �
    .041, �p2 � .05, and a marginally significant Perceiver sex
    Target behavior Situational awareness interaction, Wilks’

    .887, F(16, 608.59) � 1.52, p � .088, �p2 � .03. Follow-up
    ANOVAs yielded a significant main effect of perceiver sex:
    Women judged the target as having fewer stereotypically feminine

    sex-typed traits than did men (Ms � 3.94 vs. 4.18), F(1, 203) �
    8.64, p � .004, �p2 � .04. The ANOVAs also yielded two mar-
    ginally significant interactions: In the unaware condition, women
    judged the target as having somewhat more general academic
    competency than did men, (Ms � 5.02 vs. 4.70), F(1, 204) � 2.76,
    p � .098, �p2 � .01. In the strong-feminine condition, women
    judged the target as somewhat better-suited for stereotypically
    feminine careers than did men (Ms � 6.01 vs. 5.32), F(4, 206) �
    2.15, p � .076, �p2 � .04. In no case, however, did perceiver sex
    meaningfully alter the effects of target behavior or situational
    awareness. Additional ANOVAs that examined the data of women
    and men separately produced results that were nearly identical to
    those produced by the full sample. Supplemental Figure 1a–1d
    presents scatter plots of women’s and men’s judgments across the
    five experimental conditions of target behavior.

    Discussion

    The results of Experiment 4 broadly confirmed the hypothesis
    that a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect can influence
    how targets are viewed by others. Perceivers judged a target
    according to the target’s behavior, inferring that a target who
    confirmed the stereotype of women generally had stereotypically
    feminine traits and abilities, and that a target who confirmed the
    stereotype of men generally had stereotypically masculine traits
    and abilities. Importantly, perceivers made these correspondent
    inferences even when they were privy to the situational affor-
    dances that gave rise to the target’s behavior. Although one might
    wonder whether this tendency reflects a failure of perceivers to
    notice the situational affordances more than a failure to take them
    into account, the well-established tendency for people to make
    correspondent inferences about others whom they know lacked
    free choice suggests that perceivers fell prey to the fundamental
    attribution error when judging the target.

    We also want to emphasize that a failure of perceivers to take
    the situational affordances into account is not merely a replication
    of the fundamental attribution error. Because the experiment fo-
    cused exclusively on behaviors that had been elicited by a stereo-
    type’s prior cumulative self-fulfilling effect, the results highlight
    how the fundamental attribution error can serve as a mechanism
    whereby a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect can per-
    petuate from one social interaction to another. Indeed, the stereo-
    typic judgments that perceivers made were not caused by a reli-
    ance on sex stereotypes, but instead by a reliance on the target’s

    Table 4
    Experiment 4: Perceivers’ Judgments of the Target

    Target behavior

    Personality
    traits

    General
    academic

    competence

    Aptitude in
    math and
    science

    Career
    suitability

    M SD M SD M SD M SD

    Strong-feminine 4.60 0.67 4.43 0.81 3.82 0.62 5.81 0.84
    Moderate-feminine 4.16 0.71 4.61 0.70 4.14 0.69 4.92 1.20
    Gender-neutral 4.05 0.56 4.93 0.80 4.13 0.72 4.59 1.07
    Moderate-masculine 3.66 0.43 5.07 0.82 4.86 0.79 3.24 0.88
    Strong-masculine 3.70 0.54 5.59 0.92 5.52 1.00 2.87 1.07

    Note. M � mean; SD � standard deviation.
    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    839CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000142.supp

    behavior— behavior that confirmed sex stereotypes to varying
    degrees because of the stereotype’s prior cumulative self-fulfilling
    effect. These results are particularly troubling because they applied
    not only to judgments of sex-typed traits, but also to judgments of
    general academic competence, career aptitude, and career suitabil-
    ity; judgments that could encourage perceivers to exclude or
    wrongfully channel some targets into areas of study and work on
    the basis of the accumulating effects of stereotypic beliefs.

    General Discussion

    The idea that stereotypes can have cumulative self-fulfilling
    effects regularly appears in the psychological literature (Jussim,
    2012). Such assertions, though theoretically compelling, have been
    purely speculative, made in the absence of empirical support. The
    present research provided an initial test of the hypothesized effect,
    and strongly supported the accumulation hypothesis. Across the
    board, targets confirmed stereotypes about weight and sex to a
    greater extent when two perceivers held stereotypic expectations
    about them than when only one did. In addition, targets’ confir-
    matory behavior had downstream consequences. Naïve perceivers
    who had not caused targets to confirm the stereotypes nonetheless
    judged them according to the stereotypic behaviors they had been
    channeled to adopt, an effect that even occurred among perceivers
    who were aware that targets’ behavior was shaped by the actions
    of others.

    These findings advance theoretical and empirical understanding
    about the self-fulfilling nature of stereotypes in three critical
    respects. First, they help to clarify how stereotype-based self-
    fulfilling prophecies can create or exacerbate social problems
    despite that false expectations typically have only modest self-
    fulfilling effects on targets’ behavior within dyadic relations. Sec-
    ond, they show that situational affordances, which have long been
    thought to contribute to the self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes, can
    also operate as a mechanism of accumulation. Because this re-
    search demonstrated the effect of this mechanism among indepen-
    dent perceivers, it also explains how multiple perceivers who do
    not know one another can, through their collective actions, con-
    struct a situation that more strongly encourages targets to confirm
    a stereotype than can the actions of individual perceivers acting
    singly. Third, the findings suggest that the stereotypic behaviors
    that targets exhibit as a result of a stereotype’s cumulative self-
    fulfilling effect can become the foundation upon which new per-
    ceivers form impressions of them. Thus, the cumulative self-
    fulfilling effects of stereotypes may create seemingly defensible
    positions from which new perceivers may afford targets opportu-
    nities and constraints that could widen the gap between advantaged
    and disadvantaged groups.

    Self-Fulfilling Prophecies and Social Problems

    For most of the 20th century, the psychological literature char-
    acterized the self-fulfilling prophecy as a powerful phenomenon
    capable of producing large-scale social problems in areas such as
    hiring, education, wages, and health care (Merton, 1948; Ross et
    al., 2010; Snyder, 1984). Such claims were appealing because they
    offered a psychological and sociological explanation for unde-
    niable social injustice. Yet, as empirical evidence mounted, it
    seemed that claims of powerful self-fulfilling prophecies may have

    been overstated. Empirical research consistently showed that self-
    fulfilling prophecy effects were relatively modest in magnitude
    (Rosenthal, 1994, 2003). Even so, such findings did not preclude
    the possibility that powerful self-fulfilling prophecy effects could
    still arise through processes of accumulation.

    In support of this possibility, Madon and colleagues showed that
    the self-fulfilling effects of parents’ expectations on their adoles-
    cents’ alcohol use accumulate across mothers and fathers (Madon
    et al., 2004), and over time among particular adolescents (Madon,
    Willard, Guyll, Trudeau, & Spoth, 2006). However, because their
    findings pertained only to interpersonal expectations, which are
    idiosyncratic and, therefore, an unlikely cause of widespread social
    injustice, it was not clear how the accumulation processes they
    identified could produce the large-scale social problems proposed
    by the theoretical literature. Consideration of this issue naturally
    suggested the influence of stereotypes, which have always been at
    the heart of claims linking self-fulfilling prophecies to social
    problems. Thus, despite initial empirical support for general pro-
    cesses of accumulation, an important question remained unan-
    swered: Can the self-fulfilling effects of stereotypes accumulate?
    The results of the present research indicated that they can: Con-
    sistently, the stereotypic expectations of multiple perceivers
    caused targets to more strongly confirm a stereotype than did the
    stereotypic expectations of individual perceivers.

    Magnitude of Stereotype-Based Cumulative
    Self-Fulfilling Prophecy Effects

    Because the cumulative self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes is
    fundamentally an issue of magnitude, it is useful to compare the
    effects reported in this research to those reported in the broader
    literature. In this research, the average self-fulfilling prophecy
    effect associated with a single perceiver’s stereotypic expectation
    (d � 0.65) was similar to the meta-analytic effect size of interper-
    sonal self-fulfilling prophecy effects within dyadic relations (d �
    0.60; Rosenthal, 1994). The present research also indicated, how-
    ever, that a second perceiver’s stereotypic expectation had an
    additional self-fulfilling effect beyond that associated with the first
    perceiver’s self-fulfilling effect. Across the experiments, the self-
    fulfilling effect uniquely attributable to a second perceiver’s ste-
    reotypic expectation was d � 1.04, on average. Furthermore,
    because of this additional unique effect, the average total self-
    fulfilling effect attributable to both perceivers combined was d �
    1.69, (i.e., .65 � 1.04 � 1.69), which is more than twice the
    magnitude of the self-fulfilling effect elicited by one perceiver in
    this research and in the broader literature.

    The accumulation effect observed herein is important because it
    suggests that the typical self-fulfilling effects reported in the
    literature may underestimate the power of stereotypes. This is
    because the literature has focused exclusively on the self-fulfilling
    effect of stereotypes within dyadic relations even though stereo-
    types are often consensual. Thus, different perceivers may hold
    similar stereotypic expectations about a target. If these expecta-
    tions are inaccurate, then each perceiver may exert a self-fulfilling
    effect that could combine with the self-fulfilling effects of other
    perceivers to ultimately have a powerful influence on the target’s
    behavior. Such a process is troublesome because it may exacerbate
    social problems by virtue of creating unjust social trajectories.
    Moreover, if new perceivers fail to adjust for the fact that a

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    840 MADON ET AL.

    stereotype’s prior self-fulfilling effect caused a target’s stereotypic
    behavior, as was the case in this research, then these social trajec-
    tories could become self-sustaining, or at the very least difficult to
    change.

    Situational Affordances

    The present research tested for the accumulation of stereotype-
    based self-fulfilling prophecy effects in the context of situational
    affordances. When perceivers construct situational affordances in
    accord with their stereotypic expectations, they encourage targets
    to behaviorally confirm a stereotype by channeling their behavior
    in the direction of the stereotype. The potential for a stereotype’s
    self-fulfilling effect to accumulate across perceivers arises when
    multiple perceivers provide similar situational affordances to the
    same target. Consistent with this mechanism, the targets in this
    research more strongly confirmed stereotypes about weight and
    sex when multiple perceivers had independently provided them
    with a situational affordance that encouraged confirmation of the
    stereotypes than when only one perceiver had done so.

    Importantly, however, targets were not compelled to act in
    stereotypic ways. Although the situational affordances channeled
    their behavior in the direction of the stereotypes, all targets had the
    freedom to predominantly disconfirm the stereotypes. The fact that
    they did not exercise this freedom suggests that more than just the
    objective features of the situational affordances impinged on their
    behavior. The established propensity for people to actively con-
    struct social reality suggests that targets may have confirmed the
    stereotypes because of the way they interpreted the situational
    affordances. For example, we speculated that the targets in Exper-
    iment 1 may have perceived a large amount of candy as a form of
    cover that permitted them to take many pieces without detection,
    and that the targets in Experiment 3 may have considered the
    interpersonal consequences of their article selections for the antic-
    ipated meeting with their partners. One way to understand the
    results of this research, therefore, is that perceivers paved a be-
    havioral path for targets that targets followed because of the way
    they construed it.

    This explanation also suggests that different circumstances
    might have led targets to construe the same situational affordances
    in ways that discouraged them from confirming the stereotypes.
    For example, the literature on dyadic self-fulfilling prophecies
    indicates that targets are less susceptible to self-fulfilling prophe-
    cies when they are aware of perceivers’ expectations (Hilton &
    Darley, 1985). Applying this finding to the present research sug-
    gests that targets might have tended to disconfirm the stereotypes
    had they attributed the situational affordances to perceivers’ ste-
    reotypic expectations. For instance, if targets believed that they
    received a large amount of candy because of their perceived
    weight, or predominantly sex-typed articles because of their per-
    ceived sex, then they may have intentionally taken less candy or
    fewer sex-typed articles. Thus, the way that targets interpret a
    situation may have a bearing on a stereotype’s cumulative self-
    fulfilling effect.

    Other Potential Mechanisms of Accumulation

    The present focus on situational affordances as a mechanism of
    a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect is consistent with

    historical analyses of stereotype-based self-fulfilling prophecies.
    However, consideration of the broader social psychological liter-
    ature suggests other potential mechanisms of the accumulation
    process. We next discuss two that seem particularly likely in light
    of the empirical evidence: self-concept change and amplification.

    Self-concept change. Perceivers’ expectations can change tar-
    gets’ self-concepts through an iterative interaction sequence (Dar-
    ley & Fazio, 1980). Though typically observed within dyadic
    relations, such changes could be greater when multiple perceivers
    hold similar expectations about the same target due to the per-
    ceived credibility ascribed to consensual beliefs. Analogous to
    Kelley’s (1973) covariation model, when an expectation is idio-
    syncratic to one perceiver, targets may conclude that it says more
    about the perceiver’s characteristics than their own, thus weaken-
    ing the expectation’s apparent credibility and reducing self-
    concept change.

    By contrast, when an expectation is consensually shared by
    multiple perceivers, as in the case of some stereotypes, it may be
    given more credibility. Targets may wonder, quite reasonably, why
    multiple perceivers would expect them to possess or lack a par-
    ticular characteristic if it were not true? This could lead targets to
    conclude that the expectation says more about their own charac-
    teristics than those of the perceivers, thus strengthening the expec-
    tation’s apparent credibility and increasing self-concept change.
    Although the number of perceivers who must share an expectation
    for this to happen is an empirical question, it is possible that two
    perceivers might be enough, in which case the critical factor
    driving the effect may be a categorical shift from an idiosyncratic
    expectation to one that is consensual.

    However, because self-concept change is not behavioral, it does
    not satisfy the self-fulfilling prophecy criterion of behavioral con-
    firmation. Nevertheless, changes to targets’ self-concepts could
    initiate behavioral confirmation through self-verification strivings.
    Targets who internalize a perceiver’s false expectation may engage
    in self-verification processes that subsequently cause them to
    behaviorally confirm the false expectation (Madon et al., 2008;
    Scherr, Madon, Guyll, Willard, & Spoth, 2011; Snyder & Swann,
    1978). The credibility ascribed to consensually shared stereotypic
    expectations may exacerbate this effect. This is because consen-
    sually shared expectations may produce greater changes to targets’
    self-concepts than idiosyncratic expectations, and correspond-
    ingly, elicit more stereotype-confirming behavior from targets via
    self-verification processes. Moreover, to the extent that targets
    discount a single perceiver’s stereotypic expectation, a target’s
    susceptibility to a stereotype’s self-fulfilling effect may require an
    emergent consensus of expectations. That is to say, there may be
    circumstances under which accumulation is necessary for any
    self-fulfilling prophecy effect to occur, and in such cases syner-
    gistic accumulation may be especially likely, such as what hap-
    pened in Experiment 1.

    Amplification. In this research, the self-fulfilling effect of
    stereotypes accumulated across perceivers who never interacted
    with one another, an effect that occurred because multiple perceiv-
    ers independently provided a target with a similar situational
    affordance. This result shows that the accumulation process does
    not require social interaction among perceivers. However, when
    perceivers do interact, their expectations may become amplified.
    For example, Willard, Madon, Guyll, Scherr, and Buller (2012)
    showed that perceivers’ false expectations about a target’s hostility

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    841CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    became more extreme when perceivers interacted with another
    perceiver who also believed the target had a hostile personality.

    The tendency for social interaction to amplify perceivers’ ex-
    pectations has important implications for a stereotype’s cumulative
    self-fulfilling effect. First, the more amplified perceivers’ expec-
    tations, the more likely perceivers may be to construct situational
    affordances that encourage confirmation of a stereotype. Second,
    an amplified expectation may change targets’ self-concepts more
    than an expectation that has not been amplified, thus also evoking
    more stereotype-consistent behavior from targets through the self-
    verification processes described above. Examination of these am-
    plification processes awaits research paradigms that permit multi-
    ple perceivers to interact prior to treating targets in stereotypic
    ways.

    Limitations

    External validity. The methods of this research created an
    artificial situation in which perceivers independently made behav-
    ioral decisions (e.g., selecting candy for a target) on the basis of
    targets’ social group membership (e.g., heavy vs. thin) that were
    passed along to targets through experimental procedures. The
    experimental control created by this approach was useful for
    testing whether the accumulation of self-fulfilling prophecy effects
    across perceivers is possible, but it also simplified the accumula-
    tion process. Notably, it removed the potential influence of a host
    of factors that might typically be present in the naturalistic envi-
    ronment (e.g., certainty of expectations, perceiver and target rela-
    tions, self-verification strivings, social interaction, etc.), and which
    could influence a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling effect.

    Although this limitation may raise questions about the external
    validity of the findings, it also highlights a potential contribution of
    the present research inasmuch as it suggests that stereotype-based
    self-fulfilling prophecy effects may be especially likely to accu-
    mulate when targets’ opportunities and constraints are tightly
    controlled by perceivers; historical and contemporary examples
    include American segregation, South African apartheid, caste sys-
    tems, and cultures that oppress women. We are not suggesting that
    the accumulation of stereotype-based self-fulfilling prophecies is
    necessarily limited to these kinds of contexts, but rather that these
    contexts may be especially conducive to eliciting the effect. In-
    deed, Madon et al.’s (2004) finding that self-fulfilling prophecy
    effects accumulated across perceivers in the context of parents’
    expectations about their adolescents’ future alcohol use suggests
    that the accumulation of self-fulfilling prophecy effects may re-
    flect a general process of interpersonal influence that can occur in
    less restrictive situations and contexts than noted here.

    Patterns of accumulation. The present research provided
    clear and consistent evidence of accumulating self-fulfilling
    prophecy effects, but the pattern of these effects differed across the
    stereotypes examined. Whereas synergistic accumulation charac-
    terized the overweight stereotype and the stereotype of men, con-
    current accumulation characterized the stereotype of women. Sev-
    eral points regarding these differences deserve mention. First, we
    cannot easily explain why different accumulation patterns emerged
    for the stereotypes of women and men. Initially, we considered
    the possibility that the women in our sample might have felt more
    comfortable selecting stereotypically masculine articles than the
    men did selecting stereotypically feminine articles. However, there

    was no evidence to support this interpretation as women and men
    responded similarly to the article sets. We also cannot attribute the
    difference to method variance because the same method was used
    to test the cumulative effects of both stereotypes. The absence of
    any clear explanation points to the need for future research.

    Second, the tendency for synergistic accumulation to predomi-
    nate the findings may seem to suggest that it is the more prevalent
    of the two patterns, especially because it also characterized Madon
    et al.’s (2004) examination of interpersonal cumulative self-
    fulfilling prophecy effects. Although possible, an alternative inter-
    pretation is that synergistic accumulation is particularly likely to
    emerge with the kinds of behaviors that have been used to test the
    accumulation hypothesis. Both this research and Madon et al.,
    (2004) examined behaviors that were not bound by a target’s
    ability. The amount of candy a target takes, the type of articles a
    target selects, and the amount of alcohol an adolescent drinks do
    not require any special talent, training, or aptitude that could
    restrict targets from confirming a false expectation. As a result,
    such behaviors may be particularly susceptible to the accelerating
    pattern of synergistic accumulation. Other behaviors, such intelli-
    gence or athleticism, may be less susceptible to this accelerating
    pattern because targets’ true abilities may set boundaries that are
    difficult to surpass. In such cases, concurrent accumulation may be
    more typical. Further, when accumulation does occur with ability-
    based behaviors, accumulation may be maximized with only a few
    perceivers because targets may quickly reach the limit of their true
    abilities.

    Finally, ample research in social psychology indicates that mo-
    tivational, cognitive, and dispositional factors can moderate the
    strength of self-fulfilling prophecy effects (Snyder & Stukas,
    1999). An important next step in theory building is to investigate
    whether these factors also affect the presence of concurrent and
    synergistic accumulation. It is possible, for example, that syner-
    gistic accumulation is rendered more likely by conditions that
    intensify self-fulfilling prophecy effects, such as when perceivers
    are motivated to control targets’ behavior, targets have lower status
    or power than perceivers, and perceivers’ expectations are clear
    but targets’ self-views are unclear. Conversely, concurrent accu-
    mulation may be rendered more likely by conditions that mitigate
    self-fulfilling prophecy effects, such as when perceivers are mo-
    tivated to get along with targets, targets have higher status or
    power than perceivers, and perceivers’ expectations are unclear but
    targets’ self-views are clear. Discovering the motivational, cogni-
    tive, and dispositional factors that tend to elicit one or the other
    pattern of accumulation will provide greater theoretical under-
    standing about the accumulation process.

    Demand characteristics. A key finding of this research was
    that perceivers constructed situational affordances that proximally
    caused targets to confirm the stereotypes. We speculated that this
    effect occurred because of the way targets construed the situation.
    However, one might wonder whether demand characteristics offers
    a more parsimonious explanation (e.g., “they gave me a lot of
    candy, they must want me to take many pieces.”). Two consider-
    ations argue against this interpretation. First, the suspicion rates
    were very low and, second, in the one instance in which removing
    the data of suspicious participants affected the results (Experiment
    1), it led to a stronger, not weaker, accumulation effect, suggesting
    the influence of psychological reactance (Brehm, 1966) rather than
    demand characteristics.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    842 MADON ET AL.

    Number of perceivers. Because the present research exam-
    ined accumulation in the context of two perceivers, it cannot
    address what happens when more than two perceivers are in-
    volved. The theorized link between self-fulfilling prophecies and
    social problems implies that a stereotype’s cumulative self-
    fulfilling effect strengthens with additional perceivers. However, it
    is also possible that two is special, meaning that a stereotype’s
    cumulative self-fulfilling effect may level-off after the minimum
    requirement of two perceivers has been reached. The more general
    issue is that there may sometimes be a curvilinear, asymptotic
    relationship between the number of perceivers who hold a stereo-
    typic expectation about a target and the target’s tendency to
    behaviorally confirm the stereotype. Whether this is the case will
    require future research to test the accumulation hypothesis across
    more than two perceivers. If future research supports the “two is
    special” hypothesis, then long-standing assumptions about the way
    that self-fulfilling prophecies contribute to social problems may
    need to be reconsidered.

    Protected status. Finally, even though our results showed that
    stereotypes can have cumulative self-fulfilling effects, we demon-
    strated this effect with social groups about whom people feel
    relatively comfortable expressing negative attitudes (e.g., Haines
    et al., 2016; Leskinen, Rabelo, & Cortina, 2015; Puhl & Heuer,
    2009). As such, our procedures might have increased the chances
    that participants would treat targets in line with their stereotypes,
    a necessary step in the self-fulfilling prophecy process. This raises
    the possibility that perceivers may be less inclined to apply their
    stereotypes to groups with greater protected status (Madon, Smith,
    & Guyll, 2005), such as those associated with race or military
    service, in which case a stereotype’s cumulative self-fulfilling
    effect may be mitigated or even preempted.

    Conclusion

    Self-fulfilling prophecies are hypothesized to contribute to so-
    cial problems by generating and perpetuating group inequalities.
    The cumulative self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes is one means
    through which such inequalities can arise. Consistent with this
    process, the present research provided the first empirical evidence
    that the self-fulfilling effect of stereotypes can accumulate across
    perceivers. Targets more strongly confirmed stereotypes about
    weight and sex when two perceivers treated them in stereotypic
    ways than when only one did. Moreover, targets’ confirmatory
    behavior biased new perceivers’ judgments of them, thereby show-
    ing how the cumulative self-fulfilling effects of stereotypes can
    transcended beyond the original interactions that produced them.
    These findings are consistent with a long line of research within
    social psychology emphasizing the power of beliefs to create
    reality and demonstrate how even small self-fulfilling prophecy
    effects can contribute to social problems via an accumulation
    process.

    References

    Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. Oxford, Eng-
    land: Academic Press.

    Crandall, C. S. (1994). Prejudice against fat people: Ideology and self-
    interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 882– 894.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.882

    Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification-suppression model
    of the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin,
    129, 414 – 446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.414

    Darley, J. M., & Fazio, R. H. (1980). Expectancy confirmation processes
    arising in the social interaction sequence. American Psychologist, 35,
    867– 881. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.10.867

    Fazio, R. H. (1990). Advances in experimental social psychology: Vol. 23.
    Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model
    as an integrative framework (pp. 75–109). San Diego, CA: Academic
    Press.

    Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social
    psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston,
    MA: Houghton Mifflin.

    Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are
    a-changing . . . or are they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes,
    1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 9, 1–11.

    Hilton, J. L., & Darley, J. M. (1985). Constructing other persons: A limit
    on the effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 1–18.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(85)90002-2

    Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). A theory of correspondent inferences:
    From acts to dispositions. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
    mental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219 –266). New York, NY: Aca-
    demic Press.

    Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal
    of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    0022-1031(67)90034-0

    Jussim, L. (2012). Social perception and social reality: Why accuracy
    dominates bias and self-fulfilling prophecy. New York, NY: Oxford
    University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195366600
    .001.0001

    Jussim, L., Eccles, J. S., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception, social
    stereotypes, and teacher expectations: Accuracy and the quest for the
    powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
    experimental social psychology (Vol. 28, pp. 281–388). San Diego, CA:
    Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60240-3

    Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psy-
    chologist, 28, 107–128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034225

    Klein, O., & Snyder, M. (2003). Advances in experimental social psychol-
    ogy: Vol. 35. Stereotypes and behavioral confirmation: From interper-
    sonal to intergroup perspectives (pp. 153–234). San Diego, CA: Elsevier
    Academic Press.

    Koffka, K. (1935). The principles of gestalt psychology. New York, NY:
    Harcourt, Brace and Company.

    Kunda, Z., & Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions from stereotypes,
    traits, and behaviors: A parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psycho-
    logical Review, 103, 284 –308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X
    .103.2.284

    Leskinen, E. A., Rabelo, V. C., & Cortina, L. M. (2015). Gender stereo-
    typing and harassment: A “catch-22” for women in the workplace.
    Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 21, 192–204. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1037/law0000040

    MacKinnon, D. P., Cheong, J., & Pirlott, A. G. (2012). Statistical media-
    tion analysis. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D.
    Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in
    psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 313–331). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
    logical Association.

    MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation
    analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593– 614. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

    Madon, S., Guyll, M., Aboufadel, K., Montiel, E., Smith, A., Palumbo, P.,
    & Jussim, L. (2001). Ethnic and national stereotypes: The Princeton
    trilogy revisited and revised. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
    tin, 27, 996 –1010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167201278007

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    843CUMULATIVE SELF-FULFILLING EFFECTS OF STEREOTYPES

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.882

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.414

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.10.867

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2885%2990002-2

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2867%2990034-0

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2867%2990034-0

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195366600.001.0001

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195366600.001.0001

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601%2808%2960240-3

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034225

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.284

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.284

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/law0000040

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/law0000040

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167201278007

    Madon, S., Guyll, M., Buller, A. A., Scherr, K. C., Willard, J., & Spoth, R.
    (2008). The mediation of mothers’ self-fulfilling effects on their chil-
    dren’s alcohol use: Self-verification, informational conformity, and
    modeling processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
    369 –384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.369

    Madon, S., Guyll, M., Hilbert, S. J., Kyriakatos, E., & Vogel, D. L. (2006).
    Stereotyping the stereotypic: When individuals match social stereotypes.
    Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 178 –205. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00057.x

    Madon, S., Guyll, M., Spoth, R., & Willard, J. (2004). Self-fulfilling
    prophecies: The synergistic accumulative effect of parents’ beliefs on
    children’s drinking behavior. Psychological Science, 15, 837– 845.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00764.x

    Madon, S., Smith, A. E., & Guyll, M. (2005). Social norms regarding
    protected status and threat reactions to the stigmatized. Journal of
    Applied Social Psychology, 35, 572– 602. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
    .1559-1816.2005.tb02136.x

    Madon, S., Willard, J., Guyll, M., Trudeau, L., & Spoth, R. (2006).
    Self-fulfilling prophecy effects of mothers’ beliefs on children’s alcohol
    use: Accumulation, dissipation, and stability over time. Journal of Per-
    sonality and Social Psychology, 90, 911–926. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
    0022-3514.90.6.911

    Merton, R. K. (1948). The self-fulfilling prophecy. The Antioch Review, 8,
    193–210. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/4609267

    Puhl, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity.
    Obesity Research, 9, 788 – 805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2001.108

    Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: A review and
    update. Obesity, 17, 941–964. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.636

    Rawlings, J. O., Pantula, S. G., & Dickey, D. A. (2001). Applied regression
    analysis: A research tool (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag
    New York, Inc.

    Rist, R. C. (2000). Student social class and teacher expectations: The
    self-fulfilling prophecy in ghetto education. Harvard Educational Re-
    view, 70, 257–301. http://dx.doi.org/10.17763/haer.70.3.1k0624l610
    2u2725

    Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspec-
    tive. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 176 –179. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770698

    Rosenthal, R. (2003). Covert communication in laboratories, classrooms,
    and the truly real world. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
    12, 151–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.t01-1-01250

    Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Teacher expectations for the disad-
    vantaged. Scientific American, 218, 19 –23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
    scientificamerican0468-19

    Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distor-
    tions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
    experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). New York, NY:
    Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3

    Ross, L., Lepper, M., & Ward, A. (2010). History of social psychology:
    Insights, challenges, and contributions to theory and application. Hand-
    book of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3–50). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Scherr, K. C., Madon, S., Guyll, M., Willard, J., & Spoth, R. (2011).
    Self-verification as a mediator of mothers’ self-fulfilling effects on
    adolescents’ educational attainment. Personality and Social Psychology
    Bulletin, 37, 587– 600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399777

    Snyder, M. (1984). When belief creates reality. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
    Advances in experimental psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 247–305). Orlando,
    FL: Academic Press.

    Snyder, M., & Stukas, A. A., Jr. (1999). Interpersonal processes: The
    interplay of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activities in social
    interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 273–303. http://dx.doi
    .org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.273

    Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1978). Behavioral confirmation in social
    interaction: From social perception to social reality. Journal of Experi-
    mental Social Psychology, 14, 148 –162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-
    1031(78)90021-5

    Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and
    interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes.
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656 – 666. http://dx
    .doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656

    Steiger, J. H. (2004). Beyond the F test: Effect size confidence intervals
    and tests of close fit in the analysis of variance and contrast analysis.
    Psychological Methods, 9, 164 –182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-
    989X.9.2.164

    Willard, J., Madon, S., Guyll, M., Scherr, K. C., & Buller, A. A. (2012).
    The accumulating effects of shared expectations. European Journal of
    Social Psychology, 42, 497–508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.874

    Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal mediation
    of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of Exper-
    imental Social Psychology, 10, 109 –120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
    0022-1031(74)90059-6

    Wuensch, K. L. (2012). Using SPSS to obtain a confidence interval for
    Cohen’s d. Retrieved from http://core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/SPSS/CI-
    d-SPSS

    Received May 25, 2016
    Revision received March 23, 2018

    Accepted April 17, 2018 �

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    844 MADON ET AL.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.2.369

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00057.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00057.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00764.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02136.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02136.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.911

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.911

    http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/4609267

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2001.108

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.636

    http://dx.doi.org/10.17763/haer.70.3.1k0624l6102u2725

    http://dx.doi.org/10.17763/haer.70.3.1k0624l6102u2725

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770698

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770698

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.t01-1-01250

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0468-19

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0468-19

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601%2808%2960357-3

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399777

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.273

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.273

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2878%2990021-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2878%2990021-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.2.164

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.874

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2874%2990059-6

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2874%2990059-6

    http://core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/SPSS/CI-d-SPSS

    http://core.ecu.edu/psyc/wuenschk/SPSS/CI-d-SPSS

  • Correction to Madon et al. (2018)
  • In the article “The Accumulation of Stereotype-Based Self-Fulfilling Prophecies” by Stephanie
    Madon, Lee Jussim, Max Guyll, Heather Nofziger, Elizabeth R. Salib, Jennifer Willard, and Kyle
    C. Scherr (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2018, Vol. 115, No. 5, pp. 825– 844. doi:
    10.1037/pspi0000142), three errors occurred due to printer errors. The last sentence in the Type I
    Error section should read as follows: Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to
    examine the effect of experimental factors on multiple dependent variables of the same underlying
    construct. The second sentence of the Profile subsection of Experiment 1 should read as follows:
    The profile always described the target as female, European American, 21 years old, “between 5 feet
    4 inches and 5 feet 6 inches tall,” and as having a constellation of personality traits that was held
    constant. The third sentence in the Accumulation subsection of Experiment 1 should read as
    follows: In the current data, for example, it was possible that the signal communicated by the
    situational affordances was too weak to elicit a self-fulfilling effect when only the expectations of
    individual perceivers were considered, in which case targets might have ignored or discounted the
    signal.

    The online version of this article has been corrected.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000173

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000173

      The Accumulation of Stereotype-Based Self-Fulfilling Prophecies
      Self-Fulfilling Prophecies and Their Cumulative Effects
      Situational Affordances
      Research Overview
      Analytic Strategy
      Type I Error
      Power and Effect Sizes
      Experiment 1
      Method
      Participants
      Experimental design
      Phase 1: Materials and measures
      Profile
      Photograph
      Perceiver behavior
      Manipulation checks
      Suspicion check for perceivers
      Phase 2: Materials and measures
      Candy
      Target behavior
      Filler questions and props
      Suspicion check for targets
      Procedures
      Phase 1
      Phase 2

      Results
      Preliminary analyses
      Descriptive statistics
      Manipulation checks
      Suspicion checks
      Main analyses
      Situational affordances
      Target behavior
      Self-fulfilling prophecy effect
      Accumulation
      Pattern of accumulation

      Discussion
      Overview of Experiments 2 and 3
      Experiment 2
      Method
      Participants
      Experimental design
      Materials and measures
      Profile
      Perceiver behavior
      Manipulation check
      Suspicion check
      Procedures
      Results
      Preliminary analyses
      Descriptive statistics
      Manipulation check
      Suspicion check
      Article pools
      Main analyses
      Discussion
      Experiment 3
      Method
      Participants
      Design
      Materials and measures
      Article sets
      Target behavior
      Profile
      Suspicion check
      Procedures
      Results
      Preliminary analyses
      Descriptive statistics
      Suspicion check
      Main analyses
      Participant behavior
      Self-fulfilling prophecy effect
      Accumulation
      Pattern of accumulation
      Participant sex

      Discussion
      Experiment 4
      Method
      Participants
      Design
      Materials and measures
      Article selection lists
      Articles
      Measures
      Perceiver judgments
      Sex-typed traits
      General academic competency
      Aptitude in math and science
      Career suitability
      Manipulation checks
      Suspicion check
      Procedures
      Results
      Preliminary analyses
      Descriptive statistics
      Manipulation checks
      Suspicion check
      Main analyses
      Discussion
      General Discussion
      Self-Fulfilling Prophecies and Social Problems
      Magnitude of Stereotype-Based Cumulative Self-Fulfilling Prophecy Effects
      Situational Affordances
      Other Potential Mechanisms of Accumulation
      Self-concept change
      Amplification
      Limitations
      External validity
      Patterns of accumulation
      Demand characteristics
      Number of perceivers
      Protected status
      Conclusion
      References
      Correction to Madon et al. (2018)

    What Will You Get?

    We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

    Premium Quality

    Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

    Experienced Writers

    Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

    On-Time Delivery

    Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

    24/7 Customer Support

    Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

    Complete Confidentiality

    Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

    Authentic Sources

    We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

    Moneyback Guarantee

    Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

    Order Tracking

    You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

    image

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    image

    Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

    From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

    Preferred Writer

    Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

    Grammar Check Report

    Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

    One Page Summary

    You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

    Plagiarism Report

    You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

    Free Features $66FREE

    • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
    • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
    • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
    • Paper Formatting $05FREE
    • Cover Page $05FREE
    • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
    • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
    • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
    • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
    image

    Our Services

    Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

    • On-time Delivery
    • 24/7 Order Tracking
    • Access to Authentic Sources
    Academic Writing

    We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

    Professional Editing

    We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

    Thorough Proofreading

    We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

    image

    Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

    Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

    Check Out Our Sample Work

    Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

    Categories
    All samples
    Essay (any type)
    Essay (any type)
    The Value of a Nursing Degree
    Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
    Nursing
    2
    View this sample

    It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

    Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

    0+

    Happy Clients

    0+

    Words Written This Week

    0+

    Ongoing Orders

    0%

    Customer Satisfaction Rate
    image

    Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

    We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

    See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

    image

    We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

    We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

    • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
    • Customized writing as per your needs.

    We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

    We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

    • Proactive analysis of your writing.
    • Active communication to understand requirements.
    image
    image

    We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

    We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

    • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
    • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
    Place an Order Start Chat Now
    image

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy