ASIGNMENT3#1

Rubric Detail

Select

Don't use plagiarized sources. Get Your Custom Essay on
ASIGNMENT3#1
Just from $13/Page
Order Essay

Grid View

or

List View

to change the rubric’s layout.

Content

Name: NURS_6640_Week3_Assignment1_Rubric

Grid View

List View

 

Excellent

Good

Fair

Poor

Quality of Work Submitted:
The extent of which work meets the assigned criteria and work reflects graduate level critical and analytic thinking.

Points:

Points Range: 27 (27%) – 30 (30%)

Assignment exceeds expectations. All topics are addressed with a minimum of 75% containing exceptional breadth and depth about each of the assignment topics.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 24 (24%) – 26 (26%)

Assignment meets expectations. All topics are addressed with a minimum of 50% containing good breadth and depth about each of the assignment topics.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 21 (21%) – 23 (23%)

Assignment meets most of the expectations. One required topic is either not addressed or inadequately addressed.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 0 (0%) – 20 (20%)

Assignment superficially meets some of the expectations. Two or more required topics are either not addressed or inadequately addressed.

Feedback:

Quality of Work Submitted:
The purpose of the paper is clear.

Points:

Points Range: 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)

A clear and comprehensive purpose statement is provided which delineates all required criteria.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)

Purpose of the assignment is stated, yet is brief and not descriptive.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)

Purpose of the assignment is vague or off topic.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)

No purpose statement was provided.

Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:
Understand and interpret the assignment’s key concepts.

Points:

Points Range: 9 (9%) – 10 (10%)

Demonstrates the ability to critically appraise and intellectually explore key concepts.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 8 (8%) – 8 (8%)

Demonstrates a clear understanding of key concepts.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 7 (7%) – 7 (7%)

Shows some degree of understanding of key concepts.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 0 (0%) – 6 (6%)

Shows a lack of understanding of key concepts, deviates from topics.

Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:

Apply and integrate material in course resources (i.e. video, required readings, and textbook) and credible outside resources.

Points:

Points Range: 18 (18%) – 20 (20%)

Demonstrates and applies exceptional support of major points and integrates 2 or more credible outside sources, in addition to 2-3 course resources to suppport point of view.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 16 (16%) – 17 (17%)

Integrates specific information from 1 credible outside resource and 2-3 course resources to support major points and point of view.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 14 (14%) – 15 (15%)

Minimally includes and integrates specific information from 2-3 resources to support major points and point of view.

Feedback:

Points:

Points Range: 0 (0%) – 13 (13%)

Includes and integrates specific information from 0 to 1 resoruce to support major points and point of view.

Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:
Synthesize (combines various components or different ideas into a new whole) material in course resources (i.e. video, required readings, textbook) and outside, credible resources by comparing different points of view and highlighting similarities, differences, and connections.

Points:
Points Range: 18 (18%) – 20 (20%)

Synthesizes and justifies (defends, explains, validates, confirms) information gleaned from sources to support major points presented. Applies meaning to the field of advanced nursing practice.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 16 (16%) – 17 (17%)

Summarizes information gleaned from sources to support major points, but does not synthesize.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 14 (14%) – 15 (15%)

Identifies but does not interpret or apply concepts, and/or strategies correctly; ideas unclear and/or underdeveloped.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 0 (0%) – 13 (13%)

Rarely or does not interpret, apply, and synthesize concepts, and/or strategies.

Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
Paragraph and Sentence Structure: Paragraphs make clear points that support well developed ideas, flow logically, and demonstrate continuity of ideas. Sentences are clearly structured and carefully focused–neither long and rambling nor short and lacking substance.

Points:
Points Range: 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)

Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)

Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity 80% of the time.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)

Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity 60%- 79% of the time.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)

Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity < 60% of the time.

Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
English writing standards: Correct grammar, mechanics, and proper punctuation

Points:
Points Range: 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)

Uses correct grammar, spelling, and punctuation with no errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)

Contains a few (1-2) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)

Contains several (3-4) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)

Contains many (≥ 5) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors that interfere with the reader’s understanding.

Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
The paper follows correct APA format for title page, headings, font, spacing, margins, indentations, page numbers, running head, parenthetical/in-text citations, and reference list.

Points:
Points Range: 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)

Uses correct APA format with no errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)

Contains a few (1-2) APA format errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)

Contains several (3-4) APA format errors.

Feedback:

Points:
Points Range: 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)

Contains many (≥ 5) APA format errors.

Feedback:

Show Descriptions Show Feedback

Quality of Work Submitted:
The extent of which work meets the assigned criteria and work reflects graduate level critical and analytic thinking.–

Levels of Achievement:

Excellent 27 (27%) – 30 (30%)

Assignment exceeds expectations. All topics are addressed with a minimum of 75% containing exceptional breadth and depth about each of the assignment topics.

Good 24 (24%) – 26 (26%)

Assignment meets expectations. All topics are addressed with a minimum of 50% containing good breadth and depth about each of the assignment topics.

Fair 21 (21%) – 23 (23%)

Assignment meets most of the expectations. One required topic is either not addressed or inadequately addressed.

Poor 0 (0%) – 20 (20%)

Assignment superficially meets some of the expectations. Two or more required topics are either not addressed or inadequately addressed.
Feedback:

Quality of Work Submitted:
The purpose of the paper is clear.–

Levels of Achievement:

Excellent 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)

A clear and comprehensive purpose statement is provided which delineates all required criteria.

Good 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)

Purpose of the assignment is stated, yet is brief and not descriptive.

Fair 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)

Purpose of the assignment is vague or off topic.

Poor 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)

No purpose statement was provided.
Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:
Understand and interpret the assignment’s key concepts.–

Levels of Achievement:

Excellent 9 (9%) – 10 (10%)

Demonstrates the ability to critically appraise and intellectually explore key concepts.

Good 8 (8%) – 8 (8%)

Demonstrates a clear understanding of key concepts.

Fair 7 (7%) – 7 (7%)

Shows some degree of understanding of key concepts.

Poor 0 (0%) – 6 (6%)

Shows a lack of understanding of key concepts, deviates from topics.
Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:

Apply and integrate material in course resources (i.e. video, required readings, and textbook) and credible outside resources.–

Levels of Achievement:

Excellent 18 (18%) – 20 (20%)

Demonstrates and applies exceptional support of major points and integrates 2 or more credible outside sources, in addition to 2-3 course resources to suppport point of view.

Good 16 (16%) – 17 (17%)

Integrates specific information from 1 credible outside resource and 2-3 course resources to support major points and point of view.

Fair 14 (14%) – 15 (15%)

Minimally includes and integrates specific information from 2-3 resources to support major points and point of view.

Poor 0 (0%) – 13 (13%)

Includes and integrates specific information from 0 to 1 resoruce to support major points and point of view.
Feedback:

Assimilation and Synthesis of Ideas:
The extent to which the work reflects the student’s ability to:
Synthesize (combines various components or different ideas into a new whole) material in course resources (i.e. video, required readings, textbook) and outside, credible resources by comparing different points of view and highlighting similarities, differences, and connections.–

Levels of Achievement:
Excellent 18 (18%) – 20 (20%)
Synthesizes and justifies (defends, explains, validates, confirms) information gleaned from sources to support major points presented. Applies meaning to the field of advanced nursing practice.
Good 16 (16%) – 17 (17%)
Summarizes information gleaned from sources to support major points, but does not synthesize.
Fair 14 (14%) – 15 (15%)
Identifies but does not interpret or apply concepts, and/or strategies correctly; ideas unclear and/or underdeveloped.
Poor 0 (0%) – 13 (13%)
Rarely or does not interpret, apply, and synthesize concepts, and/or strategies.
Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
Paragraph and Sentence Structure: Paragraphs make clear points that support well developed ideas, flow logically, and demonstrate continuity of ideas. Sentences are clearly structured and carefully focused–neither long and rambling nor short and lacking substance.–

Levels of Achievement:
Excellent 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)
Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity
Good 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)
Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity 80% of the time.
Fair 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)
Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity 60%- 79% of the time.
Poor 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)
Paragraphs and sentences follow writing standards for structure, flow, continuity and clarity < 60% of the time. Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
English writing standards: Correct grammar, mechanics, and proper punctuation–

Levels of Achievement:
Excellent 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)
Uses correct grammar, spelling, and punctuation with no errors.
Good 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)
Contains a few (1-2) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors.
Fair 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)
Contains several (3-4) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors.
Poor 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)
Contains many (≥ 5) grammar, spelling, and punctuation errors that interfere with the reader’s understanding.
Feedback:

Written Expression and Formatting
The paper follows correct APA format for title page, headings, font, spacing, margins, indentations, page numbers, running head, parenthetical/in-text citations, and reference list.–

Levels of Achievement:
Excellent 5 (5%) – 5 (5%)
Uses correct APA format with no errors.
Good 4 (4%) – 4 (4%)
Contains a few (1-2) APA format errors.
Fair 3.5 (3.5%) – 3.5 (3.5%)
Contains several (3-4) APA format errors.
Poor 0 (0%) – 3 (3%)
Contains many (≥ 5) APA format errors.
Feedback:

Total Points: 100

Name: NURS_6640_Week3_Assignment1_Rubric

%7B%220.00000

%7B%220.27000

%7B%220.24000

%7B%220.21000

%7B%220.24000
%7B%220.00000

%7B%220.05000

%7B%220.04000

%7B%220.03500

%7B%220.00000

%7B%220.09000

%7B%220.08000

%7B%220.21000

%7B%220.07000

%7B%220.00000

%7B%220.18000

%7B%220.16000

%7B%220.14000

%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.18000
%7B%220.16000
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.14000
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.09000
%7B%220.08000
%7B%220.07000

Exit

%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.18000
%7B%220.16000
%7B%220.14000
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.18000
%7B%220.16000
%7B%220.14000
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.27000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500
%7B%220.00000
%7B%220.05000
%7B%220.04000
%7B%220.03500

Required Readings

American Nurses Association. (2014). Psychiatric-mental health nursing: Scope and standards of practice (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

· Standard 2 “Diagnosis” (pages 46-47)

Wheeler, K. (Ed.). (2014). Psychotherapy for the advanced practice psychiatric nurse: A how-to guide for evidence-based practice (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

· Chapter 5, “Supportive and Psychodynamic Psychotherapy” (pp. 225–238 and pp. 245–258)

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Young, J. M., & Solomon, M. J. (2009). How to critically appraise an article. Nature Clinical Practice. Gastroenterology & Hepatology, 6(2), 82–91.

 

How to Critically Appraise an Article by Young, J.; Solomon, M., in Nature Reviews Gastroenterology & Hepatology, Vol. 6/Issue 2. Copyright 2009 by Nature Publishing Group. Reprinted by permission of Nature Publishing Group via the Copyright Clearance Center.

Select one of the following articles on psychodynamic therapy to evaluate in your Assignment:

Aznar-Martinez, B., Perez-Testor, C., Davins, M., & Aramburu, I. (2016). Couple psychoanalytic psychotherapy as the treatment of choice: Indications, challenges, and benefits. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 33(1), 1–20. doi:10.1037/a0038503

Karbelnig, A. M. (2016). “The analyst is present”: Viewing the psychoanalytic process as performance art. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 33(supplement 1), S153–S172. doi:10.1037/a0037332

LaMothe, R. (2015). A future project of psychoanalytic psychotherapy: Revisiting the debate between classical/commitment and analytic therapies. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 32(2), 334–351. doi:10.1037/a0035982 

Migone, P. (2013). Psychoanalysis on the Internet: A discussion of its theoretical implications for both online and offline therapeutic technique. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 30(2), 281–299. doi:10.1037/a0031507 

Tummala-Narra, P. (2013). Psychoanalytic applications in a diverse society. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 30(3), 471–487. doi:10.1037/a0031375

  • PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET
  • :
    A Discussion of its Theoretical Implications for
    Both Online and Offline Therapeutic Technique

    Paolo Migone, MD
    Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, Parma, Italy

    Psychoanalysis over the Internet is discussed as a means of reflecting on the way
    we think about theory of technique generally, and on what we mean by
    “communication” between patient and analyst. The way we think about online
    therapy has direct implications for the way we practice “offline” therapy. This
    problem is discussed from the point of view of the history of the theory of
    psychoanalytic technique, with reference to the classic 1953 paper by Kurt
    Eissler (K. R. Eissler, 1953, The effect of the structure of the ego on psycho-
    analytic technique, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Vol. 1,
    pp. 104 –143) on “parameters,” and also with reference to the redefinition of
    psychoanalysis itself in terms of the analysis of the transference by the late
    Merton Gill (e.g., M. M. Gill, 1984, Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy: A
    revision, International Review of Psychoanalysis, Vol. 11, pp. 161–179). Online
    therapy is simply a different therapy, in the same way as two therapies, both
    offline (or both online), may be different from each other. The fil rouge that runs
    through this paper is a reflection on the very identity of psychoanalysis.

    Keywords: Internet psychotherapy, Internet psychoanalysis, online psychother-
    apy, theory of psychoanalytic technique, parameters of psychoanalytic
    technique

    It is a commonplace to say that the Internet is changing the way we communicate, and also
    the way we live, with repercussions that are not easily foreseeable. The worldwide web
    (www) is penetrating into every corner of our life, gradually changing ourselves and itself
    as it becomes more and more sophisticated in order to meet the most diversified needs.
    The importance of the Internet has been compared to the revolutionary discovery of the
    printing press.

    Here I will take into consideration only one of the many possibilities the Internet can
    offer, namely as a vehicle for psychoanalytic therapy. But this paper will not deal with the

    The author thanks Morris N. Eagle and John Kerr for their help in revising this paper.
    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Paolo Migone, MD, Via

    Palestro 14, 43123 Parma, Italy. E-mail: migone@unipr.it

    T
    hi

    s
    do

    cu
    m

    en
    t

    is
    co

    py
    ri

    gh
    te

    d
    by

    th
    e

    A
    m

    er
    ic

    an
    P

    sy
    ch

    ol
    og

    ic
    al

    A
    ss

    oc
    ia

    ti
    on

    or
    on

    e
    of

    it
    s

    al
    li

    ed
    pu

    bl
    is

    he
    rs

    .
    T

    hi
    s

    ar
    ti

    cl
    e

    is
    in

    te
    nd

    ed
    so

    le
    ly

    fo
    r

    th
    e

    pe
    rs

    on
    al

    us
    e

    of
    th

    e
    in

    di
    vi

    du
    al

    us
    er

    an
    d

    is
    no

    t
    to

    be
    di

    ss
    em

    in
    at

    ed
    br

    oa
    dl

    y.

    Psychoanalytic Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association
    2013, Vol. 30, No. 2,

    281

    –299 0736-9735/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0031507

    281

    mailto:migone@unipr.it

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0031507

    clinical aspects of Internet therapy (a literature already exists in this regard). It will deal,
    instead, specifically with the theoretical implications of both online and offline therapy for
    therapeutic technique, and in order to do so it will necessarily discuss also the differences
    between the two therapeutic settings. It is argued that the way we think about online
    therapy has direct implications for the way we think and practice traditional, “offline”
    therapy. In other words, this paper will not deal with the question of therapeutic action or
    with the validity of online therapy. Internet therapy is only taken as a pretext—an excuse,
    so to speak—in order to reflect on theory of psychoanalytic technique in general, and also
    on the identity of psychoanalysis versus psychoanalytic psychotherapy. It should be clear
    that this paper is not a plea for the practice of psychoanalysis online. Rather, it aims at
    encouraging a reflection on theory of technique. Psychoanalysis on the Internet is not
    discussed as such in this paper, but serves as a reference point to consider theory of
    technique, in particular the way we conceive “communication” between patient and
    analyst.

    Psychotherapy on the Internet has been referred to in many ways; for example, as
    online psychotherapy, telepsychotherapy, e-psychotherapy, etc., and it is a phenomenon
    that is rapidly growing. There are more and more web sites for counseling or for online
    psychotherapy, studies on the efficacy of this practice have been carried out, and so forth.
    In recent years, several psychoanalysts involved with the China American Psychoanalytic
    Alliance (CAPA, http://www.capachina.org) are experimenting with teaching, supervi-
    sion, and therapy with Skype to Chinese colleagues with the aim of helping the growth of
    psychoanalytic practice and culture in that country. (For discussions on the psychological
    implications of the Internet and on the interface between the Internet and psychoanalysis,
    see, among others, Turkle, 1985, 1995; Wallace, 1999; Bird, 2003; Akhtar, 2004; Ormay,
    2006; Malater, 2007; Monder, Toronto, & Aislie, 2007; Dini, 2009; Cairo & Fischbein,
    2010; Scharff, 2012; see also “Special issue on the Internet,” 2007, Vol. 94, Issue 1, The
    Psychoanalytic Review).

    Technical Aspects of Internet Communication

    The Internet allows us to connect and communicate with people who may live in any
    corner of the world at a very low cost, virtually for free, or, at worst, at the price of a local
    phone call. One may object that this happenstance is not altogether new, since the
    telephone already made this possible. In fact, in the United States the issue of “telephone
    analysis” was discussed at least as early as the 1950s (e.g., Saul, 1951). Commentators
    have variously considered telephone analysis a useful way to overcome certain resistances
    or impasses in the analysis, to replace missed sessions, to save time and reduce travel
    expenses in the case of long distances or when a patient’s handicaps limit movement, and
    when either patient or analyst move to another city and the parties do not want to interrupt
    an ongoing analysis.

    What the Internet can offer, compared to the telephone, is the opportunity for a
    video-conference (e.g., with Skype). Thanks to so-called virtual reality, it is possible to
    simulate the session almost exactly. There are those who even simulate the waiting room.
    With audio and video synchronized in real time, it is possible also to duplicate the timing
    of interventions, silences, the length and times of scheduled “sessions,” and various other
    rituals as if both partners were in the office. Concerning privacy, sophisticated programs
    (such as those used by Internet banks) may encrypt communications (this is true especially

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    282 MIGONE

    http://www.capachina.org

    with Skype, considered to be secure in computer-to-computer communications1), and
    ethical codes for Internet have been suggested (e.g., see American Psychological Asso-
    ciation, 1997; Manhal-Baugus, 2001; Heinlen, Welfel, Richmond, & O’Donnell, 2003;
    Mora, Nevid, & Chaplin, 2008; Fitzgerald, Hunter, Hadjistavropoulos, & Koocher, 2010).
    Nonetheless, in many respects, Internet psychotherapy can be considered a variation of
    telepsychiatry or even telemedicine, both of which have been experimented with for a
    number of years in order to reduce costs in countries such as Australia, where there are
    often formidable distances intervening between doctors and patients (e.g., see Dongier,
    1986; Preston, Brown, & Hartley, 1992; Baer et al., 1995; Kaplan, 1997, 2000; Brown,
    1998; Gammon, Sorlie, Bergvik, & Hoifodt, 1998; Gelber & Alexander, 1999; Zaylor,
    1999; Simpson, 2001; Taylor & Luce, 2003; Hilty, Marks, Urness, Yellowlees, & Nesbitt,
    2004; Bauer, Wolf, Haug, & Kordy, 2011; Wolf, 2011).

    Video-conferencing (e.g., with Skype, which is widely used) is not the only way of
    Internet communication; there are other modalities that are quite different. These modal-
    ities are distributed along a continuum of types of human communications, and they
    should not be lumped together, because each has its own specific characteristics that shape
    the therapeutic interaction—in the same way as, for that matter, various “normal,” offline
    therapeutic situations have their own characteristics that shape the interaction. For
    example, another possibility for therapeutic interchange is constituted by the written
    communications of e-mail or chat (the latter is in real time). Actually, these forms of
    written communication seem to be more widely used as methods for Internet therapy or
    counseling, perhaps because they do not require any special technical arrangements
    beyond an ordinary personal computer (incidentally, we should not forget the widespread
    use of SMS [short message service] with cellular phones between patients and analysts).
    Other commonly used modalities are discussion lists, forums, and blogs or self-help
    groups, where—in a way analogous to groups such as, for example, Alcoholic Anony-
    mous—more people can interact and talk about common themes, or else can simply listen
    (“lurk”) and profit from what others say (for an overview, see Houston, Cooper, & Ford,
    2002).

    It may be worthwhile to spend a few moments on the differences between written and
    oral communication before proceeding (Migone, 1998b). The enormous diffusion of
    communication by e-mail may represent a veritable return to the era of correspondence
    through letters, an era which had disappeared with the advent of telephone. But upon
    reflection, e-mail is similar to hand-written letters chiefly in one respect; namely, the fact
    that one has to write down what one wants to say, inducing, due to the slowness of the
    process, a different emotional and reflexive disposition. To be sure, this putative slowness
    is true especially of hand-written letters, as writing with a keyboard is invariably much
    faster. Moreover, thanks to word processing, the process has been speeded up further, for

    1 According to the Handbook of China American Psychoanalytic Alliance (CAPA), “Skype is
    encrypted using a proprietary code that has never been released by the developers, despite
    substantial pressure on the part of many powerful public entities. . . . It is nearly impossible to detect
    not only the contents of the conversations, but also their existence. . . . Apparently, Skype is so safe
    that criminals can speak using it, and the police cannot listen in—and this is Interpol, not just some
    local cops. Skype has been able to reject court orders to decrypt, because its central offices are in
    Luxembourg, and thus protected from the EU confidentiality laws (Skype was developed in Latvia).
    However, . . . all the comments on security discussed above apply to the computer-to-computer
    communications. The paid features of Skype and texting—are not secure» (Buckner, 2011, p. 13,
    italics in the original text).

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    283PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    it is now also possible to review the text and erase “errors” or simply to delete text with
    great rapidity. To be sure, it was always possible to recopy a letter with changes or even
    tear it into pieces and throw it into the wastebasket, with the option of retrying it on
    another day. Yet editorial changes on written letters generally take more time.

    An important difference between e-mail and surface mail (“snail mail”) is the trans-
    mission speed, which is close to real time with e-mails (they can reach their destination
    within seconds or minutes) and very slow for surface mail (days). This arguably creates
    a sense of immediacy in sending an e-mail that a regular letter— even if it is by special
    post—lacks. But this immediacy is offset by the fact that with e-mail it is difficult or
    impossible to communicate other meanings except the content itself, which is privileged
    at the expense of nonverbal or analogical communication. Not only it is impossible to see
    the facial expressions and to hear the tone of voice, as in face-to-face contact, but it is
    clearly impossible in e-mail to see personal calligraphy, except for the style allowed by
    word processing; namely, choice of font, capital letters (as in screaming), italics, bold (in
    programs that allow this), and “emoticons” (a well known term that means emotions
    symbolized by icons, e.g., using parenthesis for the mouth, colons for the eyes, etc.).
    Whether for these reasons, or due to the sense of immediacy of nearly real-time commu-
    nication, or because of a vague ancestral tie to that forerunner of all e-mails—the
    memo—people do not ordinarily put into their e-mails anything like the concentration,
    circumspection, or art they once put into their letters. That is not to say they couldn’t if
    they chose. And to this must be added the pertinent comparison that people have never put
    the same level of concentration or art into their conversation— except in diplomatic
    situations and in salons, where it is expected. Yet this lack of art in conversation obviously
    does not prevent psychotherapy from taking place, any more than letter writing necessarily
    either enhances it or prevents it. For the record, the first historical example of “psycho-
    analysis by letters” could be considered the correspondence between Freud and Fliess, as
    several historians of psychoanalysis have pointed out.

    Why Is Psychoanalysis on the Internet Interesting?

    In the introduction above, I have discussed the technical aspects of various ways of
    Internet communication, and it should be repeated that they are quite different from each
    other, since each one has its own peculiarities that shape the interaction. Let’s focus now
    specifically on psychoanalysis on the Internet, and let us ask why it might be interesting.
    In this regard, I want to make clear, as I emphasized before, that I am not fundamentally
    interested in online analysis per se, even though in some cases I have practiced it, as have
    others. What has always been of great interest to me, indeed has been fascinating to me,
    is the way some colleagues have faced and discussed the issue of online psychoanalysis,
    how they have addressed themselves to this “new” object, and especially their way of
    seeing similarities and differences with “traditional” (i.e., offline) psychoanalysis. I have
    been particularly intrigued by their claims to be either in favor or against online psycho-
    analysis and their reasoning. What has fascinated me was the logic behind this endorse-
    ment or disavowal as the case may be.

    In my view, online psychoanalysis is interesting because it forces us to reflect on what
    it is not; that is, traditional psychoanalysis. The way online psychoanalysis is discussed is
    revealing of the way psychoanalysis without Internet could be conceived and practiced,
    and especially of what we mean by “communication” between patient and analyst. In
    particular, I will try to show in detail the danger of relying on a stereotyped understanding

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    284 MIGONE

    of the technique of traditional therapy, at times virtually a ritualized one, in which the link
    between theory and technique could be lost. I want to make clear that I am not criticizing
    the actual practice of some colleagues; I am criticizing only a possible way of conceiving
    theory of technique. In fact, a stereotyped understanding of technique would seem to
    inevitably lead to errors in the conduct also of offline therapy.2

    This, then, is the source of my interest in Internet therapy. In particular, it was during
    an animated discussion I had in 1998 in a forum by e-mail with the editorial board of an
    online professional journal with which I was involved at the time that I was struck by the
    difficulty some colleagues had in fashioning a thoughtful approach to this issue. It was
    then that I realized that the problem lay not in developing a theory of online therapy per
    se, but in a failure to clarify the underlying understanding of the theory of technique itself.
    That is, the difficulty these colleagues experienced would have arisen in facing any
    situation that deviated from “normal” therapy; it was as if the technique of normal therapy
    had been learned as a ritualized procedure without consideration of the underlying
    implications.

    There is not a separate theoretical arena for Internet therapy any more than there is a
    separate arena for those therapies practiced in other kinds of “heterodox” situations. In
    recent decades, new frontiers and the “widening scope” (L. Stone, 1954, p. 567) of
    psychoanalytic intervention have tested the inner consistency of the so-called “basic
    model of psychoanalytic technique” (Eissler, 1953, p. 108), or “classical technique” as it
    historically developed. This testing of our technique is still encountered in daily, routine
    work, whenever the patient—labeled for example as “borderline”—succeeds in challeng-
    ing or confusing an analyst armed with the best intentions. Some mistaken ways of
    conceiving therapeutic work may never come to light if the analyst relies on traditional
    clinical practice and has an untaxing patient population. But they immediately become
    evident as soon as the analyst has to face a new situation with a different type of patient.
    I believe these problems derive from the way theory of technique was transmitted in some
    psychoanalytic circles in the past, and at times we may still see its remnants today.

    More than 60 years ago Leon Saul (1951), the first analyst I know of to ponder the use
    of the telephone in psychoanalysis, addressed the underlying point in The Psychoanalytic
    Quarterly:

    All thinking is restricted by inertia. We think as we were taught to think. New ideas, attitudes,
    and approaches always encounter resistance. This is especially true in psychoanalysis, where
    because the personal analysis mobilizes the unconscious submissiveness to the parents, and
    the narcissistic identification with them, the teacher’s authority tends to be unusually great,
    and carries with it special obligation to impart a truly academic and scientific outlook. This
    is an ironic quirk in a science born of one man’s devotion to reality despite the weight of all
    sorts of authority.
    In view of these considerations, one wonders if the idea of using modern technology in the
    form of the telephone, as an adjunct to psychoanalytic technique, will be met with horrified
    resistance, or whether most analysts are already far ahead of this in their thinking and
    anticipate experimenting with televisual communication if and when this becomes practicable
    (Saul, 1951, p. 287).

    2 In another paper (Migone, 2009), I have utilized the same approach to so called “brief
    therapy,” which provides the opportunity for reflecting on wider problems pertaining to both “brief”
    and “long-term” therapy and to the theory of technique of psychotherapy in general.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    285PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    These words sound prophetic. Prophetic, too, was Saul’s summary judgment: “Every
    technical procedure is only a means to an end, and its use must depend upon the basis of
    the rationale of all treatment: psychoanalytic accuracy in understanding the patient” (p.
    290). In the paper Saul discussed, among other things, the usefulness of using the phone
    with a patient who at the moment was unable to handle her emotions during sessions.
    However, he realized that over the phone—for reasons too complicated to discuss
    here—she was able to talk about specific transferential issues, making possible their
    working through and the subsequent resumption of regular sessions. The use of telephone,
    in this case, perfectly satisfied the criteria that 2 years later Kurt Eissler (1953) introduced
    as a way of systematizing in a coherent way the introduction of modifications of “basic
    model technique” which he called parameters.

    And it is with Eissler’s conceptualization that I want to anchor my reflections on
    Internet therapy. I am well aware that Eissler’s (1953) conceptualization of “parameters
    of technique” is generally considered outdated in contemporary psychoanalysis, but—as
    I will try to show in this paper—I am convinced that it is often misunderstood, and it
    should remain an unavoidable point of reference within the debate on theory of technique.
    It is generally recognized that today theory of technique is more a subject of debate rather
    than of consensus, but in order to have a debate we need to have positions to be confronted
    with each other, and my paper represents just one of these positions.

    Could Internet Be Considered a “Parameter” of Technique?

    I am obliged to begin by noting at the outset that Eissler himself was against the idea of
    an online psychoanalysis, at least as it could be conceived of 30 years ago. At a meeting
    held at Cornell Medical Center in New York on the occasion of the 30th anniversary of
    his classic 1953 paper on parameter, I heard him say that one might argue that maybe there
    was some truth in the criticisms some had leveled against his concept of parameter, to the
    extent that nobody ever succeeded in conducting an analysis with the computer or by
    passing over to the patient interpretations written on pieces of paper. The implication,
    perhaps, was that there were limits to how austere an analysis could be. What this implies
    for the current discussion I will take up shortly.

    First, we need to spend a few words on that classic paper itself, since, as I said, I am
    convinced that it is often misunderstood. I will try to show that Eissler’s concept of
    parameter cannot be easily dismissed, because it still has important implications for the
    identity of psychoanalysis. As is well known, Eissler’s 1953 paper was written in the mid
    20th century in an historical period marked by a great expansion of psychoanalysis within
    the U.S. as more and more patients sought psychoanalytic treatment, including some with
    diagnoses indicative of severe psychopathology. Very soon analysts realized that classic
    technique could not be applied to all such patients, and that modifications were necessary
    depending on the severity of the diagnostic condition. Classical technique, in fact, was
    based on a privileged use of verbal interpretation, with the virtual elimination of all other
    “spurious” factors such as reassurances, advice, variations of length and number of weekly
    sessions, and so forth. The analyst was supposed to stay as neutral as possible, to remain
    a blank screen, sitting behind the couch in order to minimize his influence on the patient,
    and limiting himself to verbal interpretations that were conceived as psychoanalysis’
    mutative factor par excellence. It is in this context that Eissler’s paper has to be
    understood.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    286 MIGONE

    In his paper, Eissler systematized the problem of pragmatically indispensable modi-
    fications of technique in the light of the theoretical implications of ego psychology (at the
    time the dominant theory in North American psychoanalysis); that is, of the felt need of
    taking into greater consideration the points of view of adaptation and defense. He called
    a “parameter” any change in basic model technique (which was defined with “zero”
    parameters), and suggested that a technique could be still called psychoanalysis if the
    introduction of a parameter was based on the following four criteria: 1) it should be
    introduced only when there is evidence that the standard technique is not sufficient (that
    is, when there is an ego deficit that does not allow the patient to tolerate basic model
    technique); 2) it should never go beyond an inevitable minimum; 3) it should be
    eliminated before the end of analysis; 4) its repercussions on the transference should never
    be so great that they cannot be worked through and eliminated with interpretation (see
    Migone, 2010, pp. 69 –70).

    Thus Eissler reasserted the value of basic model technique, which can never be
    reached in reality, as he was well aware, but remains useful as an heuristic goal, while
    accepting the use of parameters on the conditions that they could be reduced to a minimum
    and worked through with interpretation (which would be evidence of structural change
    and thus indicative of the repair of the ego deficit that had required the parameter’s
    introduction in the first place). In other words, this conception of psychoanalysis relies on
    the role of interpretation, which is strictly linked to a specific ego structure, while it is well
    known that contemporary psychoanalysis relies on a much wider conception of treatment
    seen as a wide ranging and diversified set of interventions. Behind this statement, of
    course, there are important and often debated issues concerning the identity of psycho-
    analysis itself.3 In fact, one might arguably say that some “psychoanalytic” theories or
    techniques— especially today—are identical to some “psychotherapeutic” theories or
    techniques. However, many discussions on the identity of psychoanalysis (and especially
    of the “psychoanalysts”) often confuse theoretical with political issues; that is, the need to
    clarify our position and to discuss them in depth should not be inhibited by the (often
    fantasized) threat of exclusion from the psychoanalytic movement (to this regard, see
    Migone, 2011).

    We all know that psychoanalysis today is practiced in a very different way from the
    classical, mid-20th-century technique, but curiously this “new” technique can be ex-
    plained in different ways according to which theory we use. For example, Eissler himself,
    who is still considered by many as the champion of old fashioned classical technique, was
    extremely flexible with his patients, to the point that his technique to some extent could
    resemble a form of “contemporary” psychoanalysis. A recent article by Emanuel Garcia
    (2009), Eissler’s literary executor, describes in detail his technique, and it is impressive to
    see how Eissler was flexible, free, and able to intervene according to his patients’ needs
    while simultaneously maintaining a psychoanalytic stance (one might argue that the same
    could be said of Freud’s technique, with the important difference that they had another
    rationale: Freud was trying to do everything he could in order to recover memories of
    traumatic events, while Eissler was trying to do everything he could also in order to
    respect the patients’ defenses). What I mean is that there is nothing wrong in having a

    3 As a recent example of such debates, see the four critical Letters to the Editor (by Lewis Aron,
    2010; Yoram Hazan, 2010; Emanuel Berman, 2010; and Steven Stern, 2010), stirred by Rachel
    Blass’ (2010a) paper titled “Affirming ‘That’s not psycho-analysis!’: On the value of the politically
    incorrect act of attempting to define the limits of our field” (see also Blass’ [2010b] reply and my
    comment [Migone, 2011]).

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    287PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    flexible technique and improving our therapeutic potential, but the theoretical problem
    remains: how do we differentiate psychoanalysis from psychoanalytic psychotherapy (this
    in fact was the main goal of Eissler’s paper) or from one of the many types of experiential
    therapies? (i.e., based on corrective experiences, without an attempt at uncovering un-
    conscious content or clarifying meanings). I think that Eissler tried to give a reply to this
    question; that is, he defined on the conceptual level what could be properly called
    psychoanalysis. As I said, this is often perceived as a hot issue because many colleagues
    fear that these theoretical discussions imply a threat of exclusion from the psychoanalytic
    movement. But this is a big misunderstanding of the relationship between theory and
    technique. In fact an implication of a correct understanding of this discussion is that—in
    Eissler’s terms—in order to perform a correct psychoanalytic technique one has to be
    flexible (i.e., perform what could be defined a “psychotherapy” from the descriptive point
    of view), otherwise he would not be a psychoanalyst (a strict adherence to a psychoan-
    alytic technique with a patient who cannot tolerate it would be a technical error). And even
    if, for the entire course of the treatment, it would be impossible to eliminate—in Eissler’s
    terminology—the parameters before the end of analysis (i.e., even if it would remain a
    “psychotherapy”), still the therapist would be employing psychoanalytic technique and
    theory (i.e., he would remain a “psychoanalyst”).

    It is becoming more clear now why I am pivoting these reflections on Internet
    psychoanalysis on Eissler’s concept of parameter. But there are other reasons why
    Eissler’s concept of parameter is interesting in this regard: Ironically, and contrary to the
    skepticism of many analysts toward therapy over the Internet, it would seem that an online
    psychoanalysis, based only on the “impersonal” communication between therapist and
    patient, could satisfy the comparatively austere criteria for classical psychoanalysis, seen
    by many as “deeper” and more therapeutic than other therapies! For on the Internet the
    mutative ingredient, verbal interpretation, remains in place. Nor are other aspects of the
    classical treatment set-up altered (neutrality, a blank screen, analyst out of sight, etc.).
    Actually, in all these respects, we could say that e-mail therapy mimics classical psycho-
    analysis.

    How are we to understand this surprising resemblance? A full discussion would entail
    a detailed evaluation of the history of theory of psychoanalytic technique over the last
    century. Here I can make only brief comments (for further discussion, see Migone, 1991,
    1994a, 1995, 1998a, 2000, 2001, 2003, 2010 chapters 1 and 4). To begin with, the current
    cautiousness about endorsing Internet therapy can perhaps partly be explained by refer-
    ence to the fact that recent decades have witnessed growing criticism within the psycho-
    analytic movement, amounting to almost a complete rejection, of the conceptions of the
    classical model insofar as they were based on the therapist’s anonymity. That is based on
    what I once called, borrowing a medical metaphor, the analyst’s “personectomy” (Migone,
    1994b, p. 130; 2004, p. 151). As Kernberg (2011), among others, has argued, this
    development is possibly due to the fact that often “anonymity” has been confused with
    “neutrality,” and it could also be “a product . . . of the authoritarian structure of psycho-
    analytic ego psychological and Kleinian institutions in the 1940s and 1950s” (p. 656). It
    is precisely that personectomized model that would seem to be exemplified in an extreme
    form, indeed to the point of caricature, by Internet therapy. Yet herein also lies an
    opportunity: The diffusion of Internet therapy, among other things, by reopening this
    problem within the debate on psychoanalytic technique, gives us the opportunity to
    reexamine these issues of theory of technique.

    Following Eissler’s argument, a therapy with parameters (i.e., modifications of the
    rules according to the patient’s needs, with interventions by the therapist who in a complex

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    288 MIGONE

    way modulates the classical model technique) is indicated for those patients who, due to
    their ego deficits, are unable to tolerate a model of technique based only on interpretations.
    Should we deduce, then, that Internet therapy, with its reliance on verbal interpretation, is
    indicated for those patients who have an intact ego (who, by the way, are extremely rare)
    or that have milder forms of psychopathology? I think this is not the way to set the
    problem, the issue being more complicated. But please note, however, the apparent
    paradox: On the face of things, Internet therapy would seem to be indicated for healthier
    patients, who could benefit from a restricted diet of verbal interpretations alone. Then
    again, for different reasons, it would also seem indicated for sicker patients; that is, those
    who are afraid of emotional closeness or overinvolvement with the therapist (such as
    Saul’s [1951] patient mentioned earlier who used the telephone for a period of time).

    Incidentally, in this context it is not compelling to argue that today, thanks to the
    possibilities offered by the multimedia communication, “virtual” therapy does not need
    separate justification given the extent to which it can simulate “real” therapy. I do not
    recall the further details of Eissler’s discussion in his talk as to the possibilities of
    psychoanalysis with the computer, but I think that if skepticism toward Internet therapy
    was once understandable, technological advances do not of themselves provide sufficient
    reason to be less skeptical and to think that Internet therapy can be indicated for either
    healthier or sicker patients. But in any event this is not the way, in my opinion, that we
    should be thinking about the problem. The issue is not whether it is possible or not to
    simulate with “virtual” reality (today possible over the Internet) the “real” reality of the
    patient�therapist interaction, with the latter being held up as a model or an ideal which
    we should try to approximate as closely as we can. The problem needs to be stated
    differently; namely, by reflecting on the theoretical premises inherent in Eissler’s con-
    ceptualization of “classic” technique, the very premises that have themselves lately been
    viewed critically in many psychoanalytic circles.

    Eissler’s reasoning had a high degree of inner coherence, and his 1953 paper is still
    valid regarding the role of the analytic frame in the logical structure of psychoanalytic
    interpretation (in this regard, see Codignola’s [1977] important contribution on—I quote
    his book’s subtitle—the “logical structure of psychoanalytic interpretation”). In his paper
    Eissler wanted to deal with the important issue of the relationship between words and
    action in analysis, and the role of mentalization, as we might now call it, as a guarantee
    of the patient’s autonomy from the environment; that is, from the parameter that was
    introduced in order to help him reestablish his psychological equilibrium. On such an
    opportunity hangs the possibility of psychoanalytic change, that is, structural change. In
    fact, a parameter (such as a reassurance, an action, or a corrective experience) is
    introduced just because interpretation does not work, and it takes the place of interpre-
    tation, so to speak. In Eissler’s words, it is necessary because of an “ego deficit” (e.g., the
    inability to understand the meaning of a symptom), so that only a behavior (the parameter)
    and not words (interpretation) can help the patient. When the patient is able to use
    interpretation (i.e., to mentalize), the parameter can be eliminated and it is possible to go
    back to the “basic model technique.” Incidentally, it must be clear that, according to this
    line of argument, if Internet can be a “parameter” of technique, the reverse could also be
    true; namely that non-Internet therapy can be a parameter of Internet therapy if the latter
    is the treatment set for a given patient. For example, an Internet therapy patient might
    temporarily need to be reassured with one or more vis-à-vis sessions if he is going through
    specific difficulties that, for various reasons, cannot be worked through within the online
    setting, so that the analyst decides to introduce a parameter that in this case consists of

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    289PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    traditional, offline therapy.4 This possibility may appear paradoxical, but is totally coher-
    ent with the concept of parameter if one is able to understand what Eissler was trying to
    say regarding theory of technique and the logical structure of interpretation (see, to this
    regard, Codignola, 1977).

    The aspect of Eissler’s conceptualization that more directly concerns us here, how-
    ever, and that has come to be seen in a very different light in recent years, involves what
    he called “basic model technique.” Embedded in his description of basic model technique
    was the idea that only one kind of technique, the classic one, in which the analyst sat out
    of sight and was careful not to intrude his own personality in any way, is good for evoking
    the transference. Tightly linked to this premise was the further notion that this kind of
    model, with the analyst endeavoring to provide only a “blank screen,” guarantees a “pure
    and uncontaminated” transference, and a truly free association; that is, uncontaminated by
    the analyst’s influence. As argued in later years by several authors (in primis Gill [1982,
    1983, 1984, 1994], who in his own way reevaluated and reinstituted the intuitions of
    Sullivan and other interpersonalist authors dating back to the 1920s and 1930s), this faith
    in the analyst’s effective nonbeing could be highly naïve. Indeed, this faith can lead to the
    analyst’s having an increased influence on the patient precisely because it is not analyzed
    (in fact it is not considered to exist; see Migone, 2000; Green, Kernberg, & Migone, 2009,
    pp. 219 –221).

    Here let us examine for a moment—and only as one of the many examples of a way
    of reasoning around the meaning of a given psychoanalytic frame—Gill’s critique of
    Macalpine’s (1950) earlier conception of psychoanalysis as an “infantile setting.” Ma-
    calpine had argued that, as an “infantile setting,” classical analysis would be apt to evoke
    just that kind of transference which we want to analyze. She defined the “infantile setting”
    in terms of the regular frequency of sessions, the use of the couch, a stable, unchanging
    environment, and so forth; that is, the very model of technique that Eissler would
    subsequently talk about in his paper. But Gill (1984, pp. 169 –170) pointed out a
    contradiction in this regard: If transference is conceived as spontaneous and “uncontam-
    inated” by the present situation, why then would we need specific measures (such as the
    couch, a stable, unchanging environment, etc.) in order to provoke it? In other words, why
    would we need to “manipulate” it by providing an “infantile setting”? The transference
    that now appears would not be a repetition of the past enacted in front of an analyst who
    remained a blank screen, but simply a more or less expectable reaction to that “infantile
    setting.” It would be an “infantile transference,” or, alternatively, a “classic transference”
    evoked by a “classic frame,” only in the sense of an iatrogenic reaction conceptually
    similar to hypnosis, which is the contrary of psychoanalysis. Very beautiful are the pages
    in Gill’s critique in which—pace the orthodox analytic view— he shows how a classical
    analysis could in effect be a “manipulatory psychotherapy,” while a once-a-week and
    vis-à-vis therapy in which the transference is carefully analyzed could be a “psychoanal-
    ysis” in every respect.

    Obviously, or perhaps not so obviously, Gill’s demolition is not meant to be a critique
    of the ground rules of classical psychoanalysis—which are rules like any other, neither
    better nor worse— but a critique of the idea that any given frame, any given set of rules,

    4 In a similar vein, elsewhere (Migone, 2000) I described the clinical case of a patient who,
    during a vis-à-vis therapy, had to lay on the couch for a few sessions in order to overcome specific
    transferential difficulties originated by the vis-à-vis setting. That intervention was conceptualized
    and worked through as a typical parameter in Eissler’s (1953) sense; the seemingly paradoxical title
    of that paper was “A psychoanalysis on the chair and a psychotherapy on the couch.”

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    290 MIGONE

    guarantees the analyst’s lack of influence on the transference, and that only those rules
    should be used for every patient in all cultures and historical periods. In point of fact, the
    latter stipulation is indeed the underlying implication of how psychoanalysis exists
    institutionally, otherwise we would not have the International Psychoanalytic Association
    setting standards such as the use of the couch and a schedule of four times a week as the
    desired frequency (even if we all know that these rules are followed more in theory than
    in practice, and that changes in these rules are under way).

    The inability of the “classic” rules to guarantee an uncontaminated transference across
    the board necessitates an alteration in how we understand the analysis of transference.
    This is why—according to Gill—the classical rules are not justified any more, so that he
    proceeds to do away altogether with “extrinsic criteria” (couch, frequency, etc.), while
    redefining “intrinsic criteria” (he centers them only on the analysis of transference) and
    espousing a radical and enlarged definition of psychoanalysis which is applicable in the
    most diverse settings, including once-a-week therapy or therapy with variable frequency,
    groups, emergency consultations, brief therapies, treatment of patients with more severe
    diagnoses and/or on medication. What is important is that the analyst always does his best
    to analyze the transference. This is the only intrinsic criterion Gill saved, and it could
    perhaps better be defined as “analysis of the relationship,” since Gill conceived it in a
    “relativistic” or “perspectival” way, that is as an analysis of the patient�therapist
    interaction, which is always influenced by the setting, whatever it is.

    Thus, it would be a mistake to argue that the classic model is inferior to another model;
    for example, without a couch, or with a lower frequency per week, or, for that matter, even
    over the Internet. The classic model is a model like any other; it will only evoke its own
    type of “contamination” of the transference. Every patient, in fact, will react to a given
    setting not according to some ideal model we think is valid for every patient, but in his
    or her own idiosyncratic way since it is the transference itself, based on the patient’s past
    experiences, that will determine how the setting will be perceived by the patient. As a very
    simplistic example, if a patient had quite reserved and silent parents perhaps he will be at
    ease with an “orthodox” analyst, while a different patient with parents who were very
    warm and exuberant might perceive this same analyst as cold or detached. It is obvious
    that it would be wrong to see as transference only the latter’s reaction and consider as
    “normal” (i.e., nontransferential) the nonconflictual state the first patient is in when he is
    with a silent and reserved analyst. Indeed his apparently “normal” reaction with an
    “orthodox” analyst would not allow us to shed light on this important area of the patient’s
    functioning, which, however, would likely become more visible if he were to be exposed
    to a different setting with the result that it could then be analyzed (for a discussion of
    Gill’s ideas, see Migone, 1991, pp. 71, 2000, 2005, 2010, pp. 71–79; Green, Kernberg, &
    Migone, 2009, pp. 216 –221). The allusion to Internet therapy here is obvious, because
    transference configurations might appear that would never appear in other settings (and
    this is true, of course, for any setting).

    It may be clear at this point why I wanted to pivot my reflections on Internet
    psychotherapy on a review of Eissler’s concept of parameter together with Gill’s position
    on intrinsic and extrinsic criteria for defining psychoanalysis. If we accept that there is no
    longer any “gold standard” for psychoanalysis in terms of extrinsic criteria, it logically
    follows that we can also conduct an analysis on the Internet (however, as I stated earlier,
    the aim if my paper is not to show the legitimacy of psychoanalysis on the Internet, which
    is only an implication of a more general discussion on theory of technique and on the
    identity of psychoanalysis; in other words, it would be a misunderstanding to conclude,
    simplistically, that this paper is “in favor of psychoanalysis on the Internet”). What is

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    291PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    important is the careful analysis of the transference manifestations according to the
    context in which the patient�therapist interaction occurs, in this instance on the Internet
    in all its variations. We must simply proceed with the awareness that this context, like any
    other context, will always have an important influence on the transference and on the
    countertransference as well.

    Further Considerations

    In order to avoid possible misunderstandings, some further clarifications are necessary.
    Specifically, it is important to analyze some of the implications underlying the argument
    that has been made to the effect we should not exclude Internet therapy a priori. Many
    colleagues have a summarily critical and even dismissive attitude toward Internet therapy.
    Such a reaction is understandable if we think of its possible abuses in terms of fostering
    “wild analysis” or its indiscriminate use as a substitute for traditional therapy. Actually,
    the logic of why Internet therapy should selectively be abused is not clear, since many
    therapists might find it more difficult and the format itself is less remunerative. Perhaps
    it may be abused in this first, pioneering phase as some exploit this new modality to search
    for patients that cannot so readily be reached and enrolled in treatment in other ways. But
    soon this territory will be overpopulated and the “first come, first served” rule will no
    longer apply. In which case qualified practitioners will crowd out less able ones to more
    or less the same extent they do now. I do think that we should be critical of Internet
    therapy but only to the extent that we are equally critical of traditional therapy, which is
    equally prone to being abused and practiced in a “wild” fashion. What I think should be
    emphasized is that a critical attitude toward Internet therapy on a priori grounds can hide
    a permissive attitude toward traditional therapy. Moreover, such an a priori rejection may
    involve the incorrect assumption that the external criteria of psychoanalysis are the
    determining factors (this issue, of course, has to do with the identity of psychoanalysis).
    To the contrary, it is the meaning of the whole experience that characterizes therapy
    (including the interplay between extrinsic and intrinsic criteria).

    This gives us the opportunity to reflect on what we mean by “communication”
    between patient and analyst. Communication refers to the meanings that are transmitted,
    and this transmission can occur in many different ways, not only in person or during live
    interactions such as in the traditional psychoanalytic situation. As we well know, at times
    a half word by telephone (or, for that matter, over the Internet, even with an e-mail or an
    sms) may have more powerful effects than a detailed communication during a session. The
    common idea that communication between patient and analyst ideally should occur during
    a traditional psychoanalytic session might hide a misunderstanding of the relationship
    between extrinsic and intrinsic criteria of psychoanalysis. In fact, the privileging of
    extrinsic criteria cannot but lead to technical errors also within traditional, non-Internet
    psychoanalysis. In this regard, examples are innumerable. Let us think only of the couch:
    Those who are against the therapeutic use of Internet may be the same (actually, often are
    the same) who believe that the couch (or any other element of the setting, for that matter)
    is essential for psychoanalysis, when in itself the couch is irrelevant (what is relevant is
    the patient’s reaction to the couch, as well as to the chair or to any other aspect of the
    ground rules; see Migone, 1998a, 2000). This stereotyped reasoning, as Galli (1988, 1990,
    2002, 2006) has argued convincingly, brings with it a reification of technique, as if, so to
    speak, technique could itself become “theory.”

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    292 MIGONE

    Internet psychotherapy would appear to be particularly useful not only when there is
    a great geographical distance between patient and therapist, but also when it is used in
    ways that are consonant with Eissler’s conceptualization of a parameter. That is, it may be
    indicated in those cases (typical examples are schizoid personality, agoraphobia, or social
    phobia) when the patient is unable to face the personal contact with the therapist while he
    is able to open up at a certain emotional distance that is being symbolized by physical
    distance (there thus being an “ego deficit” in Eissler’s terms). The patient might well be
    engaged in an initial phase of treatment in this way if, for example, he initially asks for
    help on the Internet or by e-mail; then after his resistances are worked through he might
    be able to continue with traditional psychotherapy if this is the modality deemed appro-
    priate. Of course, also the opposite could occur; a patient might begin with traditional
    psychotherapy and later, after all resistances are worked through (both on the part of the
    patient and of the analyst), he could continue with Internet therapy if this modality is
    considered appropriate. In any case, a deep and prolonged analysis of all transference and
    countertransference dynamics is indispensable before such decisions—just like for any
    other decision, for that matter; for example, regarding the complex problem of the
    termination of an analysis (or of many other problems that involve complex technical
    decision and that are typical receptacles of defenses on the part both of the patient and the
    therapist and of transferential and countertransferential issues, e.g., to name only one,
    deciding whether to accept the patient’s request of inviting the spouse at a session in order
    to discuss a specific problem). Especially countertransference should be carefully ana-
    lyzed, not only because the choice of Internet therapy could stem from defensive reasons
    on the part of the analyst (e.g., in order to avoid an emotional closeness with the patient
    that the analyst sees— erroneously, as we have seen in our discussion—as related to
    vis-à-vis therapy), but also because, conversely, the choice itself of preferring a tradi-
    tional, “normal,” non-Internet therapy rather than an Internet therapy could be a counter-
    transferential issue (e.g., in order to avoid specific emotions that the analyst sees—again,
    erroneously—as related to Internet therapy, or due to his prejudice toward Internet, or to
    his fantasies about Internet or the new technologies, and so on, in other words to his
    “transference,” i.e., countertranference in the narrow sense).

    Internet therapy can have its own dignity as a technique, exactly in the same way as
    other techniques have their own dignity, techniques such as group therapy, family therapy
    and so on. In fact, in these latter cases the question of which technique to choose and why
    should always remain open (e.g., with a given patient should we recommend individual or
    couple therapy?). What is important—in a truly psychoanalytic way of thinking—is not so
    much our final decision, but the fact that the question remains open and that the
    transference and countertransference dynamics of these preferences are continuously
    analyzed. In a way, the choice made is less important than our continuous questioning as
    to why we made this choice and not another one.

    The theoretical and technical problems of Internet psychoanalysis are in many ways
    identical to those of telephone analysis. This technique has now been practiced for decades
    by many analysts, though for many years it was not so frequently discussed, or at least not
    so frequently as it should have been, given the fact that it has been so widely used. (Often,
    telephone calls to patients were described as “incidents” during a “normal” analysis). In
    recent years, however, discussions of telephone analysis have become more open (see,
    e.g., Hymer, 1984; Lindon, 1988; Sleek, 1997; Zalusky, 1998; Aronson, 2000; Lipton,
    2001; Richards, 2001; Zarem, 2001; Leffert, 2003; Bassen, 2007; etc.), and a panel on
    telephone analysis was held on August 1, 2009, at the 26th IPA Congress in Chicago,

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    293PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    chaired by Charles Hanly and Horacio Etchegoyen, with papers by Jill Scharff, David
    Scharff, Geoff Anderson, and Neville Symington.

    Personally, I am convinced that the issue of telephone analysis was previously not so
    widely discussed as it deserved because its theoretical examination could not have helped
    but have important repercussions on the whole relationship between theory and technique
    within “traditional” psychoanalysis. The tacit, unvoiced risk here was that a discussion of
    telephone analysis would have amplified the momentum of a shift in the field in the
    direction of the position taken, for example, by the late Gill (1984). The myth of technique
    characterized by extrinsic criteria remained very reassuring for many analysts, especially
    at a time when past theoretical certainties seemed to have led to a crisis and any number
    of alternative theoretical models were appearing in the psychoanalytic marketplace. The
    theoretical pole having been revealed as fragile, clinging to the technical pole was an
    understandable inclination for analysts reacting to the threat of what they perceived as a
    loss of identity (see Migone, 2000, pp. 229 –231). (I will not comment here on the wider
    political and economic implications of the need to have a fixed procedure and setting for
    the sake of a worldwide professionalization of psychoanalysis; these are easily under-
    standable and not the subject of this paper.)

    Internet psychotherapy could be considered on its own terms as a new frontier, in the
    same way as in the history of psychoanalysis a “widening scope” (L. Stone, 1954) led
    many analysts to face new technical problems that had the beneficial effect of forcing a
    reformulation of theory. Think, for example, of the therapy of psychosis (Sullivan), of
    children (Melanie Klein), of narcissism (Kohut), of borderline and severe personality
    disorders (Kernberg and others), and also of adolescents, small and large groups, families,
    addictions, delinquencies, and so forth. As we know, the exploration of these territories
    produced a healthy rethinking of the classic theory of technique, one that in turn produced
    generalized innovations that have enriched our understanding of curative factors.

    What is important is not the fact that therapy is conducted with or without the Internet,
    but the theory we use to justify it, including most especially our ability and commitment
    to analyze the transferential and countertransferential reasons that lie behind the choice.
    Has perhaps the patient, or the analyst, in their preference for Internet psychotherapy,
    enacted a defense against some aspect of traditional therapy? And if this is the case, why?
    Contrariwise, has the choice for traditional therapy been made by one of the two parties
    or both as the expression of a resistance to an aspect of Internet psychotherapy that would
    have been possible or useful to investigate? And so on. These considerations are not
    specific to Internet therapy; they are the same choices as the ones we make for any
    intervention or therapeutic modality whether individual, group, family, or couples therapy.
    These choices, as well as their opposite, can be used as defensive receptacles, and it is the
    careful analysis of the dynamics involved that constitutes the fulcrum of our work. There
    is no place where we can rest psychoanalytically (for a more detailed discussion of these
    problems, see Migone, 1994a, 1995, 2000, 2005, 2009, 2010, chap. 4). What I want to
    emphasize is that we are not talking only of Internet therapy but of therapy tout court; that
    is, of investigating the rationale behind any technical choice. It is only by clarifying the
    theory of technique that is behind our choices that we can avoid being stuck in the blind
    alleys of Technique with a capital “T” (i.e., a technique without a theory, or that has taken
    the place of theory), and face, among other things, the technical issues of therapy with or
    without the Internet.

    I would like to make one final comment on the notion that a classic technique as
    embodied in a particular set-up is apt to evoke the specific transferential or “regressive”
    reactions that we want to analyze, because here once again there is an interesting parallel

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    294 MIGONE

    with Internet psychoanalysis. We often hear that the Internet represents a particularly
    charged setting for many subjects, more specifically that it evokes intense emotions and
    regressive or “perverse” states. Think of erotic chat lines, or of the phenomenon of a
    sudden and intense falling in love on the Internet, or of pedophilia, and so on. It seems that
    the Internet is able to stir deep or repressed emotions, emotions that may, at times, be
    paradoxically more intense than those evoked by normal, offline situations. Actually, in
    my opinion, this may well be true for particular individuals but not for all, and it would
    be a mistake to generalize these phenomena when they might only apply to a given society
    or subculture. But let us assume that it is a more or less general phenomenon for the
    purpose of argument. In that case, an argument could be made that this is precisely what
    we want to have happen in the analysis, so we could analyze it. What I would like to point
    out here is that this kind of logic is the same that is utilized in the case of classic
    psychoanalytic technique, insofar as it is asserted in that connection that a particular set-up
    (couch, rituals, the analyst as blank screen, Macalpine’s [1950] “infantile setting”) aims
    at provoking the given behavior—transference—that we want to analyze. According to
    this reasoning, classical psychoanalysis and Internet psychoanalysis would be to that
    extent homologous. Indeed, once again, Internet therapy would seem to be a veritable
    caricature of psychoanalysis with infantile transference being evoked in the latter and
    perverse activity—with its characteristic transferences—in the former. These reactions
    would equally be forms of presumed analytic “regression.”

    As should be clear at this point, I disagree with this logic. In fact, in both cases the
    mistake is to generalize to all subjects the effect that a given stimulus has on a particular
    group of people. Moreover, even if this kind of reaction might in fact be generalizable
    across different subjects, it is not clear why we would want to evoke this type of
    transference and not another (see, in this regard, Gill’s [1984, pp. 168 –171] lucid critique
    of the concept of regression in analysis). In other words, why shouldn’t we be just as
    interested in evoking a transference that is different from the one evoked by classical
    psychoanalysis— or for that matter by the Internet?

    One final point in closing. This paper, as it has been emphasized, does not deal with
    the differences between online and offline therapy per se, but on the theoretical implica-
    tions of the way we think about a given therapeutic setting; in other words, it is a paper
    on theory of psychoanalytic technique. However, a de-emphasis on the differences
    between online and traditional therapy might overlook the fact that indeed there is a
    difference, and this difference should be clearly spelled out, but again with a privileged
    emphasis on the theoretical implication of this difference (and—as we’ll see— on its
    philosophical implication, in a way): one aspect is undoubtedly absent in Internet therapy
    but present in traditional therapy—the “physical” body of the patient. This absence can be
    a fundamental problem for so-called body or movement therapies; that is, therapies that
    use the patient’s body as such within therapy, and not just the patient’s fantasies and
    emotions about his or her body. From this point of view, Internet therapy is surely
    “inferior” to traditional therapy. But, if the considerations I have advanced in this paper
    are cogent, then we cannot deny that traditional therapy, logically speaking, is itself
    “inferior” to Internet psychotherapy— or to any other type of nontraditional therapy, for
    that matter— because it is deprived of important data that can only be evoked with the sole
    presence of a “virtual” body. “Virtual” reality and “real” reality (assuming that the latter
    can be understood as such, a philosophically tricky proposition that cannot be discussed
    here) are not superior one to the other, but simply two different kinds of experience. Each
    one deserves to be investigated and respected; each one is able to give us precious
    information on human nature.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    295PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    References

    Akhtar, S. (2004). Editor’s introduction: New technologies and psychoanalysis. International
    Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic Studies, 1, 200 –201. doi:10.1002/aps.67

    American Psychological Association. (1997). Services by telephone, teleconferencing, and Internet.
    A statement by the ethics committee of the American Psychological Association. Retrieved from
    http://www.apa.org/ethics/stmnt01.html

    Aron, L. (2010). On: Responding to Rachel Blass’ article ‘Affirming “That’s not psycho-analysis!”
    On the value of the politically incorrect act of attempting to define the limits of our field’. The
    International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91, 1279 –1280. doi:10.1111/j.1745-
    8315.2010.00348.x

    Aronson, J. K. (2000). Use of the telephone in psychotherapy. Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
    Baer, L., Cukor, P., Jenike, M., Leahy, B., O’Laughlen, J., & Coyle, J. (1995). Pilot studies of

    telemedicine for patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychi-
    atry, 152, 1383–1385.

    Bassen, C. R. (2007). Telephone analysis (Panel report). Journal of the American Psychoanalytic
    Association, 55, 1033–1041. doi:10.1177/00030651070550030101

    Bauer, S., Wolf, M., Haug, S., & Kordy, H. (2011). The effectiveness of internet chat groups in
    relapse prevention after inpatient psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 219 –226. doi:
    10.1080/10503307.2010.547530

    Berman, E. (2010). On ‘Affirming “That’s not psycho-analysis!”’. The International Journal of
    Psychoanalysis, 91, 1281–1282. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00349.x

    Bird, J. (2003). “I wish to speak to the despisers of the body”: The Internet, physicality, and
    psychoanalysis. Journal for the Psychoanalysis of Culture & Society, 8, 121–126. doi:10.1353/
    psy.2003.0003

    Blass, R. B. (2010a). Affirming “That’s not psycho-analysis!” On the value of the politically
    incorrect act of attempting to define the limits of our field. The International Journal of
    Psychoanalysis, 91, 81–89. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2009.00211.x

    Blass, R. B. (2010b). On: The comments of Emanuel Berman, Lewis Aron, Yoram Hazan and
    Steven Stern. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91, 1285–1287. doi:10.1111/j.1745-
    8315.2010.00352.x

    Brown, F. W. (1998). Rural telepsychiatry. Psychiatric Services, 49, 963–964.
    Buckner, L., (Ed.). (2011). Handbook of China American psychoanalytic alliance (CAPA). New

    York, NY: CAPA.
    Cairo, I. (Moderator), & Fischbein, S. V. (Reporter). (2010). Panel report: Psychoanalysis and

    virtual reality [2009] (Irene Cairo, Moderator). The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91,
    4, 985–988. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00300.x

    Codignola, E. (1977). Il vero e il falso. Saggio sulla struttura logica dell’interpretazione psicoa-
    naitica [The truth and the false: Essay on the logical structure of psychoanalytic interpretation].
    In R. Langs (Ed.), The Yearbook of Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy (vol. 2, pp. 326 –377).
    New York: Gardner Press. Turin, Italy: Boringhieri.

    Dini, K. (2009). Internet interaction: The effects on patients’ lives and analytic process. Panel report.
    Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 57, 979 –988. doi:10.1177/
    0003065109337874

    Dongier, M. (1986). Telepsychiatry: Psychiatric consultation through two-way television. A con-
    trolled study. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie, 31, 32–34.

    Eissler, K. R. (1953). The effect of the structure of the ego on psychoanalytic technique. Journal of
    the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 104 –143. Retrieved from http://www.psychomedia.it/
    pm/modther/probpsiter/eiss53-2.htm

    Fitzgerald, T. D., Hunter, P. V., Hadjistavropoulos, T., & Koocher, G. P. (2010). Ethical and legal
    considerations for internet-based psychotherapy. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 39, 173–187.
    doi:10.1080/16506071003636046

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    296 MIGONE

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aps.67

    http://www.apa.org/ethics/stmnt01.html

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00348.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00348.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00030651070550030101

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2010.547530

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2010.547530

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00349.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/psy.2003.0003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/psy.2003.0003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2009.00211.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00352.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00352.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00300.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0003065109337874

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0003065109337874

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/eiss53-2.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/eiss53-2.htm

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/16506071003636046

    Galli, P. F. (1988). Le ragioni della clinica. [The reasons of critique] Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane,
    3, 3–8.

    Galli, P. F. (1990, June). Psychoanalysis as the story of a crisis. Paper presented at the annual
    meeting of the Rapaport-Klein Study Group, Stockbridge, MA. Retrieved from http://
    www.psychomedia.it/rapaport-klein/galli90.htm

    Galli, P. F. (2002). La persona e la tecnica. Appunti sulla pratica clinica e la costruzione della
    teoria psicoanalitica [The person and the technique. Notes on clinical practice and on the
    construction of psychoanalytic theory], (2nd ed.). Milan, Italy: FrancoAngeli.

    Galli, P. F. (2006). Tecnica e teoria della tecnica in psicoanalisi tra arcaico e postmoderno
    [Psychoanalytic technique and theory of technique between archaic and postmodern]. Psicot-
    erapia e Scienze Umane, 40, 153–164.

    Gammon, D., Sorlie, T., Bergvik, S., & Hoifodt, T. S. (1998). Psychotherapy supervision conducted
    by videoconferencing: A qualitative study of users’ experiences. Journal of Telemedicine and
    Telecare, 4(Suppl. 1), 33–35. doi:10.1258/1357633981931353

    Garcia, E. E. (2009). Enduring relevance: An introduction to the clinical contributions of K. R.
    Eissler. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 78, 1109 –1126.

    Gelber, H., & Alexander, M. (1999). An evaluation of an Australian videoconferencing project for
    child and adolescent telepsychiatry. Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare, 5(Suppl.1), 21–23.
    doi:10.1258/1357633991933297

    Gill, M. M. (1982). The analysis of transference. Vol. 1: Theory and technique. New York, NY:
    International Universities Press.

    Gill, M. M. (1983). The interpersonal paradigm and the degree of the therapist’s involvement.
    Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 19, 200 –237.

    Gill, M. M. (1984). Psychoanalysis and psychotherapy: A revision. International Review of
    Psychoanalysis, 11, 161–179. Retrieved from http://www.psychiatryonline.it/ital/10Gil-aI.htm

    Gill, M. M. (1994). Psychoanalysis in transition: A personal view. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
    Green, A., Kernberg, O. F., & Migone, P. (2009). Un dialogo sulla differenza tra psicoanalisi e

    psicoterapia psicoanalitica. [A dialogue on the difference between psychoanalysis and psycho-
    therapy]. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 43, 215–234.

    Hazan, Y. (2010). On: Response to Rachel B. Blass’ paper about the value of attempting to define
    the limits of psychoanalysis. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91, 1280 –1281.
    doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00350.x

    Heinlen, K. T., Welfel, E. R., Richmond, E. N., & O’Donnell, M. S. (2003). The nature, scope, and
    ethics of psychologists’ e-therapy web sites: What consumers find when surfing the web.
    Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 40, 112–124. doi:10.1037/0033-
    3204.40.1-2.112

    Hilty, D. M., Marks, S. L., Urness, D., Yellowlees, P. M., & Nesbitt, T. S. (2004). Clinical and
    educational telepsychiatry applications: A review. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/La Revue
    Canadienne de Psychiatrie, 49, 1, 12–23.

    Houston, T. K., Cooper, L. A., & Ford, D. E. (2002). Internet support groups for depression: A
    1-year prospective cohort study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 2062–2068. doi:
    10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2062

    Hymer, S. M. (1984). The telephone session and the telephone between sessions. Psychotherapy in
    Private Practice, 2, 51–65. doi:10.1300/J294v02n03_08

    Kaplan, E. H. (1997). Telepsychotherapy. Psychotherapy by telephone, videotelephone, and com-
    puter videoconferencing. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice & Research, 6, 227–237.

    Kaplan, E. H. (2000). Rejection of the visual in telepsychotherapy. CyberPsychology & Behavior,
    3, 981–987. doi:10.1089/109493100452246

    Kernberg, O. F. (2011). Divergent contemporary trends in psychoanalytic theory. Psychoanalytic
    Review, 98, 633–664. doi:10.1521/prev.2011.98.6.633

    Leffert, M. (2003). Analysis and psychotherapy by telephone: Twenty years of clinical experience.
    Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 51, 101–130. doi:10.1177/
    00030651030510011301

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    297PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    http://www.psychomedia.it/rapaport-klein/galli90.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/rapaport-klein/galli90.htm

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/1357633981931353

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/1357633991933297

    http://www.psychiatryonline.it/ital/10Gil-aI.htm

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00350.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.40.1-2.112

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.40.1-2.112

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2062

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2062

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J294v02n03_08

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/109493100452246

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/prev.2011.98.6.633

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00030651030510011301

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00030651030510011301

    Lindon, J. A. (1988). Psychoanalysis by telephone. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 52, 521–528.
    Lipton, L. (2001). Long distance psychoanalysis. Clinical Social Work Journal, 29, 35–52. doi:

    10.1023/A:1005206527225
    Macalpine, I. (1950). The development of transference. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 19, 501–539.
    Malater, E. (2007). Caught in the Web: Patient, therapist, e-mail, and the Internet. Psychoanalytic

    Review, 94, 151–168. doi:10.1521/prev.2007.94.1.151
    Manhal-Baugus, M. (2001). E-therapy: Practical, ethical, and legal issues. CyberPsychology &

    Behavior, 4, 551–563. doi:10.1089/109493101753235142
    Migone, P. (1991). La differenza tra psicoanalisi e psicoterapia: Panorama storico del dibattito e

    recente posizione di Merton M. Gill [The difference between psychoanalysis and psychotherapy:
    Historical review of the debate and recent position by Merton G. Gill]. Psicoterapia e Scienze
    Umane, 4, 35–65.

    Migone, P. (1994a). The problem of “real” trauma and the future of psychoanalysis. International
    Forum of Psychoanalysis, 3, 89 –96.

    Migone, P. (1994b). Intervento sul caso di disturbo ossessivo-compulsivo [Intervention on the
    clinical case of obsessive-compulsive disorder]. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 3, 129 –131.

    Migone, P. (1995). Terapia o ricerca della verità? Ancora sulla differenza tra psicoanalisi e
    psicoterapia [Therapy or search for truth? Again on the difference between psychoanalysis and
    psychotherapy]. Il Ruolo Terapeutico, 69, 28 –33. Retrieved from http://www.psychomedia.it/
    pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt69-95.htm

    Migone, P. (1998a). Chi ritiene indissolubile il binomio divano/psicoanalisi si pone fuori dalla
    logica psicoanalitica [Those who believe in the indissolubility of the link between psychoanal-
    ysis and couch are outside of the psychoanalytic approach]. Il Ruolo Terapeutico, 78, 16 –21.
    Retrieved from http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt78le98.htm

    Migone, P. (1998b, February). Intervention at the panel “Mailing lists” at the International Congress
    of Internet & Mental Health, Genoa, Italy. Retrieved from http://www.psychiatryonline.it/ital/
    congress/migo.htm

    Migone, P. (2000). A psychoanalysis on the chair and a psychotherapy on the couch. Implications
    of Gill’s redefinition of the differences between psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. In D. K.
    Silverman & D. L. Wolitzky (Eds.), Changing conceptions of psychoanalysis: The legacy of
    Merton M. Gill (pp. 219 –235). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.

    Migone, P. (2001). La differenza tra psicoanalisi e psicoterapia psicoanalitica è solo una questione
    politica [The difference between psychoanalysis and psychoanalytic psychotherapy is only a
    political issue]. Il Ruolo Terapeutico, 86, 17–20. Retrieved from http://www.psychomedia.it/
    pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt86pp01.htm

    Migone, P. (2003). La psicoterapia con Internet [Psychoanalysis with Internet]. Psicoterapia e
    scienze umane, 4, 57–73.

    Migone, P. (2004). Editoriale [Editorial]. Psicoterapia e Scienze Umane, 38, 149 –152. Retrieved
    from http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt97-04.htm

    Migone, P. (2005). Terapeuti “brevi” o terapeuti “bravi”? Una critica al concetto di terapia breve
    [“Brief” therapists or “good” therapists? A critique to the concept of brief therapy]. Psicoterapia
    e Scienze Umane, 39, 347–370.

    Migone, P. (2009, June). What does “brief” mean? A theoretical critique of the concept of
    short-term psychotherapy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Rapaport-Klein Study
    Group, Stockbridge, MA. Submitted for publication.

    Migone, P. (2010). Terapia psicoanalitica [Psychoanalytic therapy], 2nd ed. Milan, Italy:
    FrancoAngeli.

    Migone, P. (2011). On the definition of psychoanalysis. The International Journal of Psychoanal-
    ysis, 92, 1315–1317. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2011.00431.x

    Monder, B., Toronto, E., & Ainslie, G. (2007). Time out of mind. Psychoanalytic explorations of
    patient’s excessive engagement with virtual reality. Psychoanalyst-Psychologist, Summer, 27,
    34 –35.

    Mora, L., Nevid, J., & Chaplin, W. (2008). Psychologist treatment recommendations for Internet-

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    298 MIGONE

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1005206527225

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1005206527225

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/prev.2007.94.1.151

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/109493101753235142

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt69-95.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt69-95.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt78le98.htm

    http://www.psychiatryonline.it/ital/congress/migo.htm

    http://www.psychiatryonline.it/ital/congress/migo.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt86pp01.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt86pp01.htm

    http://www.psychomedia.it/pm/modther/probpsiter/ruoloter/rt97-04.htm

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2011.00431.x

    based therapeutic interventions. Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 3052–3062. doi:10.1016/
    j.chb.2008.05.011

    Ormay, T. (2006). Cybertherapy: Psychotherapy on the Internet. International Journal of Psycho-
    therapy, 10, 51–60.

    Preston, J., Brown, F. W., & Hartley, M. (1992). Using telemedicine to improve health care in
    distant areas. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 43, 25–32.

    Richards, A. K. (2001). Talking cure in the 21st century: Telephone psychoanalysis (Panel report).
    Psychoanalytic Psychology, 18, 388 –391. doi:10.1037/0736-9735.18.2.388

    Saul, L. J. (1951). A note on the telephone as a technical aid. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 20,
    287–290.

    Scharff, J. S. (2012). Clinical issues in analyses over the telephone and the Internet. The Interna-
    tional Journal of Psychoanalysis, 93, 81–95. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2011.00548.x

    Simpson, S. (2001). The provision of a telepsychology service to Shetland: Client and therapist
    satisfaction and the ability to develop a therapeutic alliance. Journal of Telemedicine and
    Telecare, 7 (Suppl. 1), 34 –36. doi:10.1258/1357633011936633

    Sleek, S. (1997). Providing therapy from a distance. APA Monitor, 38(8), 1.
    Stern, S. (2010). On: ‘Affirming: “That’s not psychoanalysis!”’ by R. Blass. The International

    Journal of Psychoanalysis, 91, 1283–1284. doi:10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00351.x
    Stone, L. (1954). The widening scope of indications for psychoanalysis. Journal of the American

    Psychoanalytic Association, 2, 567–594. doi:10.1177/000306515400200402
    Taylor, C. B., & Luce, K. H. (2003). Computer- and Internet-based psychotherapy interventions.

    Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 18 –22. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01214
    Turkle, S. (1985). The second self: Computers and the human spirit. New York, NY: Simon &

    Schuster.
    Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the Internet. New York, NY: Simon &

    Schuster.
    Wallace, P. (1999). The psychology of the Internet. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    doi:10.1017/CBO9780511581670
    Wolf, A. W. (2011). Internet and video technology in psychotherapy supervision and training.

    Psychotherapy, 48, 179 –181. doi:10.1037/a0023532
    Zalusky, S. (1998). Telephone analysis: Out of sight, but not out of mind. Journal of the American

    Psychoanalytic Association, 46, 1221–1242. doi:10.1177/00030651980460041601
    Zarem, S. (2001). Choosing the telephone: A clinical exploration of potential advantage. Issues in

    Psychoanalytic Psychology, 23, 49 –57.
    Zaylor, C. (1999). Clinical outcomes in telepsychiatry. Journal of Telemedicine and Telecare,

    5(Suppl. 1), 59 –60.

    E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!

    Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be
    available online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and
    you will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    299PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.05.011

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.05.011

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0736-9735.18.2.388

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2011.00548.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1258/1357633011936633

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-8315.2010.00351.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000306515400200402

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.01214

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511581670

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023532

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00030651980460041601

      PSYCHOANALYSIS ON THE INTERNET
      Technical Aspects of Internet Communication
      Why Is Psychoanalysis on the Internet Interesting?
      Could Internet Be Considered a “Parameter” of Technique?
      Further Considerations
      References

    Literature in psychotherapy differs from other areas of clinical practice. Generally, there are no clinical trials in psychotherapy because it is often neither appropriate nor ethical to have controls in psychotherapy research. This sometimes makes it more difficult to translate research findings into practice. In your role, however, you must be able to synthesize current literature and apply it to your own clients. For this Assignment, you begin practicing this skill by examining current literature on psychodynamic therapy and considering how it might translate into your own clinical practice.

    Learning Objectives

    Students will:

    · Evaluate the application of current literature to clinical practice

    To prepare:

    · Review this week’s Learning Resources and reflect on the insights they provide.

    ·

    Select ONE of the
    Psychodynamic Therapy Articles
    from the Learning Resources to evaluate for this Assignment.

    ·

    Note: In nursing practice, it is not uncommon to review current literature and share findings with your colleagues. Approach this Assignment as though you were presenting the information to your colleagues.

    The Assignment

    In a 5- to 10-slide PowerPoint Presentation, address the following:

    1-Provide an
    Overview
    of the article you selected.

    2-What Population is under consideration?

    3-What WAS the Specific Intervention that WAS used?

    4-Is this a NEW Intervention or one that WAS already used?

    5-What WERE the author’s claims?

    6-Explain the Findings/Outcomes of the study in the article.

    7- Include
    Whether
    this will translate into practice with your own clients. If so, How? If not, Why?

    · 8-Explain Whether the limitations of the study might impact your ability to use the
    Findings/Outcomes
    presented in the article. Support your position with evidence-based literature.

    Note:

    The presentation should be 5–10 slides,
    not including the title and reference slides. Include presenter notes (no more than ½ page per slide) and use tables and/or diagrams where appropriate. Be sure to support your work with specific citations from the article you selected. Support your approach with evidence-based literature.

    What Will You Get?

    We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.

    Premium Quality

    Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.

    Experienced Writers

    Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.

    On-Time Delivery

    Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.

    24/7 Customer Support

    Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.

    Complete Confidentiality

    Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.

    Authentic Sources

    We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.

    Moneyback Guarantee

    Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.

    Order Tracking

    You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.

    image

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    Areas of Expertise

    Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.

    image

    Trusted Partner of 9650+ Students for Writing

    From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.

    Preferred Writer

    Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.

    Grammar Check Report

    Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.

    One Page Summary

    You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.

    Plagiarism Report

    You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.

    Free Features $66FREE

    • Most Qualified Writer $10FREE
    • Plagiarism Scan Report $10FREE
    • Unlimited Revisions $08FREE
    • Paper Formatting $05FREE
    • Cover Page $05FREE
    • Referencing & Bibliography $10FREE
    • Dedicated User Area $08FREE
    • 24/7 Order Tracking $05FREE
    • Periodic Email Alerts $05FREE
    image

    Our Services

    Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.

    • On-time Delivery
    • 24/7 Order Tracking
    • Access to Authentic Sources
    Academic Writing

    We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.

    Professional Editing

    We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.

    Thorough Proofreading

    We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.

    image

    Delegate Your Challenging Writing Tasks to Experienced Professionals

    Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!

    Check Out Our Sample Work

    Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality

    Categories
    All samples
    Essay (any type)
    Essay (any type)
    The Value of a Nursing Degree
    Undergrad. (yrs 3-4)
    Nursing
    2
    View this sample

    It May Not Be Much, but It’s Honest Work!

    Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.

    0+

    Happy Clients

    0+

    Words Written This Week

    0+

    Ongoing Orders

    0%

    Customer Satisfaction Rate
    image

    Process as Fine as Brewed Coffee

    We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.

    See How We Helped 9000+ Students Achieve Success

    image

    We Analyze Your Problem and Offer Customized Writing

    We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.

    • Clear elicitation of your requirements.
    • Customized writing as per your needs.

    We Mirror Your Guidelines to Deliver Quality Services

    We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.

    • Proactive analysis of your writing.
    • Active communication to understand requirements.
    image
    image

    We Handle Your Writing Tasks to Ensure Excellent Grades

    We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.

    • Thorough research and analysis for every order.
    • Deliverance of reliable writing service to improve your grades.
    Place an Order Start Chat Now
    image

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code Happy