write a 2-3-page-long essay overviewing each paper, and one paragraph of personal thoughts
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1998. 49:
43
–64
Copyright © 1998 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND
BEHAVIOR
Bryan Kolb and Ian Q. Whishaw
Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, T1K 3M4
Canada; e-mail: kolb@HG.ULETH.CA
KEY WORDS: dendrite arborization, environmental enrichment, cortex, neuropsychology,
experience
ABSTRACT
Brain plasticity refers to the brain’s ability to change structure and function.
Experience is a major stimulant of brain plasticity in animal species as di-
verse as insects and humans. It is now clear that experience produces multi-
ple, dissociable changes in the brain including increases in dendritic length,
increases (or decreases) in spine density, synapse formation, increased glial
activity, and altered metabolic activity. These anatomical changes are corre-
lated with behavioral differences between subjects with and without the
changes. Experience-dependent changes in neurons are affected by various
factors including aging, gonadal hormones, trophic factors, stress, and brain
pathology. We discuss the important role that changes in dendritic arboriza-
tion play in brain plasticity and behavior, and we consider these changes in
the context of changing intrinsic circuitry of the cortex in processes such as
learning.
CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
Assumptions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
METHODS OF STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Analysis of Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Analysis of the Brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46
EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Environmental Enrichment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Training in Specific Tasks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
Olfactory Experience in Rodents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51
Plasticity in the Avian Brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
0066-4308/98/0201-0043$08.00
43
The Invertebrate Nervous System. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53
Dendrites and Behavior in Humans . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54
MODULATION OF EXPERIENCE-DEPENDENT CHANGE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56
Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56
Sex Hormones . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57
Neurotrophins. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57
Stress. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58
Injury . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58
CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59
INTRODUCTION
One of the key principles of behavioral neuroscience is that experience can
modify brain structure long after brain development is complete. Indeed, it is
generally assumed that structural changes in the brain accompany memory
storage (Bailey & Kandel 1993). Although the idea that experience can modify
brain structure can probably be traced back to the 1890s (Ramon y Cajal 1928,
Tanzi 1893), it was Hebb who made this a central feature of his neuropsycho-
logical theory (Hebb 1949). Hebb did the first experiments on the conse-
quences of enriched rearing on the behavior of the rat (Hebb 1947). Later, the
group at Berkeley began to demonstrate changes in brain weight, cortical
thickness, acetylcholine levels, and dendritic structure that accompanied the
behavioral changes related to experience (e.g. Diamond et al 1967, 1981;
Globus et al 1973; Rosenzweig & Bennett 1978; Rosenzweig et al 1962). In
the 1970s, and continuing still, William Greenough and his colleagues initi-
ated a multidisciplinary investigation of the cellular effects of rearing animals
in visually or motorically enriched environments (e.g. Greenough & Chang
1989). Perhaps the fundamental point that this group has made over the past
decades is that synapses can form and dendrites can grow well beyond the peri-
od of brain development. Although this point is certainly not unique to Gree-
nough, he and his colleagues have shown most forcefully that the adult mam-
malian brain (and presumably other vertebrate brains as well) can add not only
dendrites and synapses in response to behavioral demands but also supportive
tissue elements such as astrocytes and blood vessels.
There is now an extensive literature correlating neural and glial changes
with behavioral change in species ranging from insects to humans. Because
many other chapters review synaptic changes during development, our focus is
on studies in which some sort of experimental manipulation has been shown to
change behavior and neural structure, especially in mammals.
Assumptions
The study of brain and behavioral correlations necessarily involves assump-
tions about methodology, and this review chapter is no exception. In particu-
44 KOLB & WHISHAW
lar, we assume that changes in the structural properties of the brain will reflect
changes in brain function. Furthermore, we assume that the most likely place
to identify neural changes associated with behavior is at the synapse. In order
to relate synaptic change to behavioral change, it also is assumed in the current
review that synaptic changes can be measured by analysis of the postsynaptic
structure of cells, either by light microscope or electron microscope tech-
niques. We do not consider neurochemical or neurophysiological changes in
the current review.
METHODS OF STUDY
Analysis of Behavior
Three important sets of behavioral distinctions are relevant to plastic changes
in the brain. The first relates to the difference between exercise and skill acqui-
sition. Running in a treadmill is wonderful exercise as it no doubt improves
cardiovascular function, reduces body fat, and improves health in old age, all
of which may contribute to brain plasticity. But running may not require much
in the way of plastic changes to support it. Learning a new skill, such as play-
ing a musical instrument, learning to type, or reading Braille requires exten-
sive practice, and this practice is likely instrumental in changing the neuropil
in relevant brain regions. This distinction is methodologically important be-
cause a group receiving exercise can be used as a control for nonspecific ef-
fects in a group receiving skill training. In most of the experiments reviewed
below some form of exercise regime is administered to a control group that
will subsequently be compared with a group receiving skill training.
A more subtle, and often unrecognized, distinction is that between volun-
tary movements and supporting reactions. Most voluntary movements, such as
advancing a limb to grasp food, require concomitant supporting reactions. For
example, when a quadruped, such as a rat, lifts and advances a limb to reach for
food in an experimental test, it must support its weight with its remaining
limbs. To do so, it usually supports and shifts its weight with the diagonal
couplet of the contralateral forelimb and the ipsilateral hind limb, and it some-
times assists in balancing itself and moving by using its tail (Whishaw & Mik-
lyaeva 1996). Whereas it is widely accepted that acquiring the act of reaching
is a skill that is accompanied by morphological changes in the forelimb area of
motor cortex (Greenough et al 1985), it is unclear whether balancing, weight
shifting, and tail use are also skills in the same sense. This could be tested em-
pirically, but the more important point relates to how much of the brain under-
goes plastic change during the acquisition of the skill of reaching. Perhaps only
the neurons controlling the reaching arm undergo change, but it is more likely
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 45
that the entire motor system will change to varying degrees as the animal ac-
quires the reaching skill. Thus, experiments must be quite complex and in-
clude measures from the brain area of interest, adjacent brain regions, and the
contralateral hemisphere as well as from appropriate control groups.
The third distinction is between recovery and compensation following
brain injury. There is a great deal of interest in how surviving brain tissue
changes to contribute to recovery following brain injury (e.g. Forgie et al 1996,
Jones & Schallert 1994, Prusky & Whishaw 1996). Careful analysis of the be-
havior of rats recovering from injury suggests that much of what appears on
cursory examination to be recovery is actually a compensatory substitution of
new movements for lost movements (Whishaw & Miklyaeva 1996). Thus, the
recovery of behavior following injury may not be due to spared neurons as-
suming the functions of lost neurons but may be due to spared neurons chang-
ing their morphology to support compensatory skills. Because both recovery
and compensation are potentially important avenues for therapeutics follow-
ing brain injury, this distinction, though subtle, is not unimportant.
Analysis of the Brain
The analysis of neuronal change and behavior rests upon the assumption that
changes will be found at the synapse when behavior changes. There are, how-
ever, two problems to solve. First, the visualization of morphology must pro-
vide similar results from animal to animal and study to study. Injections of
neuronal tracers to identify axons will not satisfy this concern because visuali-
zation of axon terminals depends critically upon how many cells are actually
affected by the injection. Furthermore, it is not always possible, a priori, to
know where in the brain to look for experience-dependent change, so injec-
tions into focal areas are often impractical. Second, because there are so many
neurons in the brain, it is essential that only a subset of cells is stained, at best
randomly, throughout the brain. Golgi-type stains solve these concerns. Some-
where between 1%–4% of neurons are stained, and there is good evidence that
the staining is random (e.g. Pasternak & Woolsey 1987). Furthermore, modern
Golgi techniques (e.g. Golgi-Cox; see Gibb & Kolb 1997) provide reliable and
extensive staining. Once the cells are stained with a Golgi procedure, the den-
dritic length can be measured with the aid of a light microscope. Cells are
traced using either some sort of semiautomated imaging system or a camera lu-
cida procedure in which cells are drawn with pen and ink. The length of den-
dritic arborization and the density of synaptic spines then can be estimated us-
ing various methods (e.g. Capowski 1989, Kolb et al 1997a). The rationale for
selecting dendrites is based upon their special attributes. In particular, den-
drites represent up to 95% of the receptor surface with which neurons form
46 KOLB & WHISHAW
connections (Schade & Baxter 1960). The dendrites grow and retract in re-
sponse to various events including neuronal activity, various chemicals, and
injury to adjacent neurons. This makes dendrites one of the more sensitive in-
dicators of change within the CNS. Finally, because dendrites form a large
fraction of the neuropil, they are an important indicator of the functional ca-
pacity of neural networks. Furthermore, because the majority of excitatory
synapses are found on synaptic spines, measurements of spine characteristics
including their density, shape, and ultrastructural characteristics also can be
made to supplement the dendritic results. (Of course, dendrites do not allow
quantification of the actual number of synapses; this can only be done using
electron microscopic procedures.)
EXPERIENCE AND THE CHANGING BRAIN
We can now identify a large range of neural changes associated with experi-
ence. These include increases in brain size, cortical thickness, neuron size,
dendritic branching, spine density, synapses per neuron, and glial numbers.
The magnitude of these changes should not be underestimated. For example, in
our own studies of the effects of housing rats in enriched environments, we
consistently see changes in young animals in overall brain weight on the order
of 7%–10% after 60 days (e.g. Kolb 1995). This increase in brain weight repre-
sents increases in glia, blood vessels, neuron soma size, dendritic elements,
and synapses. It would be difficult to estimate the total number of increased
synapses, but it is probably on the order of 20% in the cortex, which is an ex-
traordinary change!
Environmental Enrichment
The logic of enrichment studies is that one group of animals is placed in labo-
ratory cages while a second group is housed in a more stimulating environ-
ment, the extreme case being Hebb’s home (Hebb 1947). Hebb’s enrichment
procedure was extreme because Hebb’s rats roamed fully around his home,
whereas most studies of this sort have placed the experimental animals in large
enclosures that contain visually stimulating objects and an opportunity to in-
teract haptically with the environment, including the objects. In many studies,
the objects are changed routinely and in some studies the social housing condi-
tions may also be manipulated. As mentioned above, it was the group of Ben-
nett, Krech, Rosenzweig, Diamond, and their colleagues at the University of
California, Berkeley that first showed large changes in various measures of
cortical morphology. As important and seminal as the Berkeley experiments
were, they had the weakness that they did not demonstrate changes in brain or-
ganization so much as in brain size. It was not until the early 1970s that several
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 47
groups, including the Berkeley group, began to look at dendritic fields (e.g.
Globus et al 1973, Uylings et al 1978). The most thorough studies of this sort
were done, however, by Greenough. Typical experiments showed that the den-
dritic fields of these neurons increased by about 20% relative to cage-reared
animals (e.g. Greenough & Volkmar 1973; Volkmar & Greenough 1972).
These effects were not restricted to the visual cortex, although other regions
tended to show lesser effects and some cell types were relatively unaffected
(e.g. Greenough et al 1973). A parallel set of studies has examined changes in
the cerebellum of animals trained in complex motor tasks and, as might be an-
ticipated, there are parallel changes in the Purkinje cells, which are the major
output cell of the cerebellum. Furthermore, as in the studies of neocortical re-
gions, there is evidence that neuronal changes are not inevitable consequences
of experience because cerebellar granule cells do not show the same changes
(e.g. Floeter & Greenough 1979).
Although most studies of environment-dependent changes in the cortex
have been done with rodents, several studies have used monkeys or cats. In
general, these studies have found similar results (e.g. Beaulieu & Colonnier
1987, Floeter & Greenough 1979, Stell & Riesen 1987). One curious differ-
ence between the rodent and primate studies appears to be the effects upon the
visual system. Because monkeys are highly visual compared to rats, one might
predict greater effects upon the visual cortex of monkeys; yet the opposite ap-
pears to be true. In fact, it appears that the effects upon the primary visual cor-
tex of monkeys reared in enriched environments are negligible (e.g. Riesen et
al 1977, Struble & Riesen 1978). One explanation is that much of the explora-
tion of the visual world of monkeys is done without movement, and because
monkeys in relatively impoverished housing can still visually explore their en-
vironment, this stimulation may be sufficient to ensure the development of vis-
ual cortical synapses. In contrast, the visual system of the rat has relatively
poor acuity and is not designed for pattern vision so much as for spatial naviga-
tion. The gathering of spatial information likely requires movement in space.
An alternate explanation is that because the visual areas of the primate have
expanded dramatically, and because the primary visual cortex is multifunc-
tional, then it is “higher” visual areas that show greater experience-dependent
changes. In this case, one might predict that visual experiences that empha-
sized object exploration and recognition would lead to growth in the ventral
visual pathway, whereas visual experiences that emphasized visuomotor guid-
ance, such as in climbing or object manipulation, would lead to growth in the
dorsal visual pathway (for a discussion of the two pathways, see Milner &
Goodale 1995).
Most studies of dendritic change have used a Golgi-type technique to meas-
ure dendritic space, and from this there is an assumption that dendritic space is
48 KOLB & WHISHAW
correlated tightly with synaptic numbers. Turner & Greenough (1983, 1985)
examined this hypothesis directly by calculating the number of synapses per
neuron in the cortex of animals housed in enriched environments. They found
an increase of about 20% in the number of synapses per neuron in the brains of
enriched versus cage-reared animals. Thus, although the density of synapses in
a section of cortical tissue is relatively constant in enriched and cage-reared
animals, there is more dendritic space in the enriched animals and, conse-
quently, there are more synapses per neuron. Similarly, Beaulieu & Colonnier
(1988) analyzed the number and type of synapses in cats reared in laboratory
cages or in enriched housing. Like Turner & Greenough, they found that syn-
aptic changes correlated with experience. One additional finding, however,
was that experience increased the number of excitatory synapses per neuron
and decreased the number of inhibitory ones in the visual cortex. Thus, enrich-
ment had modified the excitatory-inhibitory equilibrium of the visual cortex.
One prediction from this observation is that neurons in the cortex of enriched
animals would be more reactive to visual stimulation than those in impover-
ished animals.
It is reasonable to expect that if there are increases in the size of the den-
dritic fields of neurons, and correspondingly in the number of synapses per
neuron, then these neurons will require more support both from glial cells, es-
pecially astrocytes, and from blood vessels. In one heroic series of studies,
Sirevaag & Greenough (e.g. 1987, 1988, 1991) used light and electron micro-
scope techniques to analyze 36 different aspects of cortical synaptic, cellular,
and vascular morphology in rats raised in complex or in caged-housing envi-
ronments. The simple conclusion was that there is a coordinated change not
only in neuronal morphology but also in glial, vascular, and metabolic pro-
cesses in response to differential experiences. Thus, not only are there more
synapses per neuron in animals with enriched experience, there is also more
astrocytic material, more blood capillaries, and a higher mitochondria volume.
(Mitochondrial volume is used as a measure of metabolic activity.) It is there-
fore clear that when the brain changes in response to experience there are the
expected neural changes but there are also adjustments in metabolic require-
ments of the larger neurons. One interesting implication of this conclusion is
that things that influence the maintenance and adjustment of the metabolic
components of the aging brain can be expected to influence the brain’s capac-
ity for neural change as well (e.g. Black et al 1987, 1989). This speaks to the
importance of examining the effects of exercise and nutrition on the brain’s ca-
pacity for change, especially in senescence. It is important to note in this con-
text, however, that merely having exercise is not sufficient to induce neuronal
changes. Black et al (1990) trained animals to negotiate a complex obstacle
course (“acrobat rats”) or placed rats in running wheels where they obtained
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 49
forced exercise. The animals in the wheels showed increased capillary forma-
tion but no change in cerebellar Purkinje cell synapses, whereas the acrobat
rats showed a 30% increase in Purkinje synapses. Thus, merely increasing neu-
ronal support does not change the neurons. The critical feature for neuronal
change is presumably increased neuronal processing, which would be facili-
tated by a complementary increase in metabolic support.
Training in Specific Tasks
Although it is tempting to conclude that the synaptic changes observed in ani-
mals housed in complex environments reflect changes in learning about the en-
vironment, there is little direct evidence of this. One way to approach this issue
is to train animals in specific tasks and then to demonstrate specific changes in
dendritic fields of neurons in regions suspected of being involved in the per-
formance of such tasks. Perhaps the most convincing studies of this sort were
done by Chang & Greenough (1982). These studies took advantage of the fact
that the visual pathways of the laboratory rat are about 90% crossed. That is,
about 90% of the cortical projections from the left eye project via the right lat-
eral geniculate nucleus to the right hemisphere. Chang & Greenough placed
occluders on one eye of rats and then trained the animals in a visual maze.
Comparison of the neurons in the two hemispheres revealed that those in the
trained hemisphere had larger dendritic fields. This experiment is compelling
because the rest of the two hemispheres (e.g. auditory, somatosensory, or ol-
factory regions) would still have interacted with the maze, and both hemi-
spheres would be required for the motor demands. It was only the visual cortex
contralateral to the open eye that could process and/or store the task-specific
visual information, however; and this was reflected by the specific dendritic
changes in that hemisphere.
Another experiment is relevant here. Although they did not train animals in
a visual learning task, Tieman & Hirsch (1982) raised cats with lenses that re-
stricted visual exposure to lines oriented vertically or horizontally. Many pre-
vious studies had shown that cells in the visual cortex of cats with such re-
stricted experience show a marked change in their tuning characteristics.
Hence, neurons in cats with selective exposure to lines of vertical orientation
are most excitable when presented with lines of the same orientation. The new
wrinkle in the Tieman & Hirsch study was that they examined the morphology
of visual cortical neurons from cats with selective horizontal or vertical visual
experience. Cats raised in a normal environment showed a random distribution
of orientation of dendritic fields, but cats raised with selective experiences
showed a change in the orientation of the dendritic fields. These changes were
specific, too, because they occurred in pyramidal cells in visual cortex and not
in the adjacent stellate cells.
50 KOLB & WHISHAW
A second set of experiments has taken advantage of the fact that rats are
very talented at using their forepaws to retrieve food from tubes, through bars,
and so on. Because the cortical control of the forelimbs is largely crossed, it is
possible to train one limb to reach for food and to compare the layer V neurons
in the forelimb region of motor cortex, many of which form the cortical spinal
tract, in the trained and untrained hemispheres. Several studies have shown
dendritic changes in the expected neurons (Greenough et al 1985, Kolb et al
1997a, Withers & Greenough 1989).
The changes in dendritic fields seen in the studies of visual and motor learn-
ing are strikingly reminiscent of the changes seen in studies of enriched rear-
ing, which have been taken as evidence that the observed changes in synaptic
connectivity in animals in enriched environments are somehow involved in
memory and learning (Greenough & Chang 1989). While this is a reasonable
conclusion, there may be important differences in details of dendritic change
in the enrichment and learning studies. It is generally found that enrichment
not only increases dendritic length but also increases the density of synaptic
spines on the dendrites. In contrast, animals trained in specific tasks show
changes in dendritic length but not in spine density (Kolb et al 1996). Thus, it
appears that although there are marked similarities between the effects of en-
riched rearing and specific training on dendritic fields, there may be differ-
ences in other measures of dendritic morphology, especially spine density.
Olfactory Experience in Rodents
Rodents have a keen sense of smell so it is reasonable to suppose that experi-
ence would have significant effects upon the structure of the olfactory system.
In fact, the general finding is that olfactory deprivation leads to restricted mor-
phological development of the olfactory system, whereas olfactory training or
olfactory “enrichment” leads to enhanced development (e.g. Doving & Pinch-
ing 1973, Pinching & Doving 1974, Rehn & Breipohl 1986, Rehn et al 1986)
(see also the extensive studies by Leon and colleagues, e.g. Leon 1992a,b).
One surprising finding is that olfactory experience not only changes the mor-
phology of existing neurons, but it also alters the number of neurons. For ex-
ample, odor deprivation results in reductions in neuronal number (e.g. Brunjes
& Frazier 1986, Meisami & Safari 1981, Skeen et al 1986), whereas enriched
odor exposure leads to increased neuron numbers (Rosselli-Austin & Williams
1990). This neuronal increase is not trivial, being in the order of 35-40%! Evi-
dence of increased neuron numbers in the olfactory system is especially in-
triguing because it has not been seen in analyses of neocortical or cerebellar
cortex. One important difference between the olfactory system and neocortical
and cerebellar regions is that olfactory neurons are generated throughout the
lifetime of rodents (e.g. Lois & Alvarez-Buylla 1994). Thus, it is likely that en-
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 51
hanced olfactory experience influences neuronal growth in the olfactory bulb
throughout life. One possible reason for this could be that the addition and de-
letion of olfactory neurons throughout life allow a mechanism for the nervous
system to form new olfactory memories and to modify existing ones. It is note-
worthy that the other forebrain structure that generates neurons throughout
adulthood is the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus, and this area has been im-
plicated in certain types of learning and memory.
Plasticity in the Avian Brain
Three general types of studies look at brain plasticity and behavior in birds.
These include studies of bird song, imprinting, and one-trial learning. Studies
of bird song have been reviewed extensively elsewhere (e.g. Bottjer & Arnold
1997) and largely have focused on the development of neurons and their con-
nectivity. Our emphasis here therefore is on imprinting and one-trial learning.
Imprinting is a process whereby an organism learns, during a sensitive peri-
od in development, to restrict its social preferences to a specific class of ob-
jects (e.g. Bateson 1966). Imprinting is especially common in precocial birds
such as chickens or geese. Within hours of hatching, a young bird will ap-
proach and follow a moving object, which may or may not resemble an adult
female of the same species. Horn and his colleagues (for reviews, see Dudai
1989, Horn 1985) have identified a specific neural region (region IMHV) in
the chick brain that changes morphologically during imprinting. For example,
there is increased metabolic activity and genetic (RNA) activity in IMHV dur-
ing imprinting. Morphological studies have emphasized ultrastructure where it
has been shown that imprinting is correlated with an increase in the length of
the postsynaptic density (PSD) of spine synapses in the IMHV, but only in the
left hemisphere (Horn et al 1985). The PSD is the active receptor-dense region
of the postsynaptic cell. Thus, as the PSD lengthens, the number of receptors
for neurotransmitters increases (Matus et al 1981). Horn (1985) noted that
small changes in the length of the postsynaptic density provide an effective
way for presynaptic cells to control the firing of postsynaptic cells, or to con-
trol local synaptic interactions. Significantly, there does not seem to be an in-
crease in synapse number in the IMHV during imprinting.
Several studies have taken advantage of the observation that one-day-old
chicks peck spontaneously at a small bright chrome bead. For instance, Patel &
Stewart (1988) coated the bead with either a substance with an aversive taste or
nonaversive water. Chicks presented with the aversive taste learn in one trial to
avoid the bead, whereas those presented with the nonaversive bead continue to
peck (for a review, see Rose 1985). Various regions of the chick brain, such as
the hyperstriatum, show enhanced activity following training as revealed both
by electrophysiological investigations and studies of glucose accumulation.
52 KOLB & WHISHAW
Patel & Stewart used a Golgi technique to impregnate chick brains 25 h after
training and found a twofold increase in spine density in the neurons in a re-
gion of the hyperstriatum (intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale) in the
“trained” chicks. They concluded that this represented an increase in synapses
that reflected the learning. This interpretation was supported by a second study
in which Patel et al (1988) trained chicks on the passive avoidance task de-
scribed above but, in their experiment, half of the trained chicks were given a
subconvulsive transcranial electroshock 5 min after training. This procedure
rendered about half of the trained animals amnesic for the experience. The
spine density was found to be higher in the chicks that remembered the aver-
sive nature of the training stimulus than in the chicks rendered amnesic. This
finding argues strongly in favor of a specific role for dendritic spines in
experience-dependent memory formation in the chick.
There is one additional study that suggests that their conclusion may not be
quite correct, however. Wallhausser & Scheich (1987) presented newly
hatched chicks with either a hen or an acoustic stimulus, with the goal of im-
printing the chicks to the visual or auditory stimulus. The neurons in different
regions of the hyperstriatum of the imprinted chicks were compared with those
of isolated chicks. There was a decrease in spine density. Thus, in the first
study, there was an increase 25 h after training, whereas in the latter study there
was a decrease 7 days after training. The simplest conclusion from the chick
studies is that the novel stimulation may cause an initial rapid increase in spine
density, followed by a pruning. The critical experiment here would be to exam-
ine the neurons in the brains of animals killed at different times in the training.
The Invertebrate Nervous System
There is a burgeoning literature on the effects of experience on the morphol-
ogy of neurons of invertebrates, both during metamorphosis (e.g. Jacobs &
Weeks 1990, Kent & Levine 1993) and in response to experience (e.g. Hoy et
al 1985). Two studies on Drosophila are especially intriguing. In one, Technau
(1984) showed that the complexity of neurons in Drosophila melanogaster de-
pends upon the flies’ living conditions. Flies were housed for 3 weeks either
singly in small plastic vials or in groups of 200 in larger enclosures with col-
ored visual patterns on the walls, various odor sources, and plants. Analysis of
the Kenyon cell fibers in the mushroom bodies (cells in the “brain” of the fly)
showed about 15% more fibers in the enriched versus impoverished flies. A
subsequent study by Heisenberg et al (1995) showed that most regions of the
Drosophila brain were continuously reorganized throughout life in response to
specific living conditions. In particular, social and sexual activity was associ-
ated with increased brain size, as was the volume of space available. These
experience-dependent changes in Drosophila are remarkable and leave little
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 53
doubt that experience is a major force in shaping the nervous system of all ani-
mals. Furthermore, changes in insect brains are not only seen in artificial lab
experiments but can also be seen in ecologically valid settings. For example,
Withers et al (1993) examined the changes in the brain of the honey bee in rela-
tion to the division of labor in adult worker bees. Adult worker bees spend
about the first 3 weeks of their 4–7-week life performing a variety of tasks
within the hive, including caring for the queen and brood (“nursing”). They
then make a dramatic transition in behavior and begin to forage outside for
food. Food foraging is a complex behavior that requires that the animal learn
the location of both the hive and the food and the nature of different foods, as
well as learn to recognize and use species-typical signals about food sources
from other bees. Withers et al (1993) found not only that the behavioral change
is associated with striking changes in brain structure, but that these changes are
dependent not on the age of the animal but on its foraging experience. This
honey bee model offers a new entry into the cellular mechanisms of neural and
behavioral plasticity.
Dendrites and Behavior in Humans
On the basis of studies in laboratory animals it is reasonable to expect correla-
tions between neuronal structure and behavior in humans. One way to ap-
proach this would be to look for a relationship between cell structure and edu-
cation. Jacobs et al (1993) did, in fact, consider this question and found a rela-
tionship between extent of dendritic arborization in a cortical language area
(Wernicke’s area) and amount of education. Hence, the cortical neurons from
the brains of deceased people with university education had more dendritic ar-
bor than those from people with high school education who, in turn, had more
dendritic material than those with less than high school education. Of course, it
may have been that people with larger dendritic fields were more likely to go to
university, but that is not easy to test.
Another way to look at the relationship between human brain structure and
behavior is to consider the functional abilities of people and to correlate them
with neuronal structure. For example, one might expect to find differences in
language-related areas between people with high and low verbal abilities. This
experiment is difficult to do, however, because it presupposes behavioral
measures before death, and this is not normally done. However, Jacobs et al
(1993) considered this possibility by taking advantage of the now well-
documented observation that females have verbal abilities that are superior to
those of males (for a review, see Kolb & Whishaw 1996). Thus, when they ex-
amined the structure of neurons in Wernicke’s area, they found that females
have more extensive dendritic arbors than males. Furthermore, in a subsequent
study, Jacobs et al (1993) found that this sex difference was present as early as
54 KOLB & WHISHAW
age 9, suggesting that such sex differences emerge within the first decade.
These sex differences in cortical architecture in humans are parallel to those
reported in other studies showing sex differences in cerebral blood flow and
glucose metabolism, with females having a level about 15% higher than that of
males (e.g. Baxter et al 1987).
Scheibel et al (1990) approached the matter in a slightly different way.
They began with two hypotheses. First, they suggested that there is a relation-
ship between the complexity of dendritic arbor and the nature of the computa-
tional tasks performed by a brain area. To test this hypothesis, they examined
the dendritic structure of neurons in different cortical regions that they pro-
posed to have functions that varied in computational complexity. For example,
when they compared the structure of neurons corresponding to the somatosen-
sory representation of the trunk versus those for the fingers, they found the lat-
ter to have more complex cells. They reasoned that the somesthetic inputs from
receptive fields on the chest wall would constitute less of an integrative and in-
terpretive challenge to cortical neurons than those from the fingers and thus
that neurons representing the chest were less complex. Similarly, when they
compared the cells in the finger area to those in the supramarginal gyrus
(SMG), a region that is associated with higher cognitive processes, they found
the SMG neurons to be more complex. The second hypothesis was that den-
dritic trees in all regions are subject to experience-dependent change. As a re-
sult, they hypothesized that predominant life experiences (e.g. occupation)
should be reflected in the structure of dendritic trees. Although they did not
test this hypothesis directly, they did have an interesting observation. In their
study comparing cells in the trunk area, finger area, and the SMG, they found
curious individual differences. For example, especially large differences in
trunk and finger neurons were found in the brains of people who were typists,
machine operators, and appliance repairmen. In each of these, a high level of
finger dexterity maintained over long periods of time may be assumed. In con-
trast, one case with no trunk-finger difference was a salesman in whom one
would not expect a good deal of specialized finger use. These results are sug-
gestive although we would agree with Scheibel et al’s caution that “a larger
sample size and far more detailed life, occupation, leisure, and retirement his-
tories are necessary” (p. 101). The preliminary findings in this study do sug-
gest that such an investigation would be fruitful.
Finally, one can look at pathological development and see if there is a neu-
ral correlate of abnormal behavior. In one such study, Purpura (1974) exam-
ined the dendritic structure of neurons from the brains of retarded versus aver-
age intelligence children. He did not quantify the dendritic length, but he did
find marked differences in dendritic structure. The retarded children had spin-
dly dendrites that had a very much reduced spine density. This abnormal spine
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 55
density is intriguing because it is reminiscent of the low spine density that we
have consistently observed in rats with cortical injury in what would be
equivalent to the third trimester of human development. Like retarded chil-
dren, these rats have severe behavioral deficits that render them unable to learn
cognitive tasks that are solved easily by animals with similar brain injuries
later in life (e.g. Kolb & Gibb 1991a). More recently, there have been several
studies of children with trisomic chromosomal states, such as Down’s syn-
drome and trisomy 13 (e.g. Becker et al 1986, Jay et al 1990, Marin-Padilla
1974), the general observation being that there is anomalous spine morphol-
ogy, decreased spine density, and small dendritic fields in many types of retar-
dation.
MODULATION OF EXPERIENCE-DEPENDENT CHANGE
The demonstration that dendritic and/or synaptic change is related to experi-
ence is intriguing, but it is not proof of a relationship. The critical experiments
are those in which an experimental manipulation alters the behavior and the
morphology changes in a meaningful manner. Various manipulations fit this
requirement, including especially age, sex hormones, neurotrophins, stress,
and injury.
Aging
Despite nearly a century of effort by scores of investigators, many of the basic
questions concerning changes in the aging brain are swirling in controversy
(Coleman & Flood 1987). Buell & Coleman (1979) first noted that the aged
brain showed an increase in dendritic growth that was hypothesized to com-
pensate for the loss of neurons with age. That is, their general idea is that the
number of synapses in the cortex is maintained by adding synapses to the den-
drites of the adjacent neurons in the cortex. This growth would seem to be in
accord with the general observation that most middle-aged people can be pre-
sumed to have suffered neuronal loss but do not appear demented. Further-
more, Buell & Coleman (1985) have shown that there is a failure of dendritic
growth in the demented brain. As intriguing as the relationship between aging,
dementia, and dendritic growth appears, we must caution that there nonethe-
less remains some controversy because not all brain regions appear to suffer
cell death with age (e.g. Coleman & Flood 1987).
Sex Hormones
There is accumulating evidence that the male brain and the female brain differ
in their structure and respond differently to experience. Specifically, Juraska
and her colleagues (e.g. Juraska 1984, 1986, 1990; Juraska et al 1985, 1989)
56 KOLB & WHISHAW
were the first to report that the visual cortex is more sensitive to experience in
males than it is in females. This is not a general increased sensitivity of males,
however, because they have also reported that the hippocampus is more sensi-
tive to experience in females than in males. These differences are related to the
circulating gonadal hormone and therefore can be manipulated with hormone
injections. Evidence of sex differences in cortical structure has now been
shown to occur in the prefrontal cortical regions of lab-reared rats (e.g. Kolb &
Stewart 1991, 1995), and recently it has been shown that injury to these mor-
phologically dimorphic areas produces sexually dimorphic differences in
functional recovery (Kolb & Cioe 1996).
The importance of sex hormones is not restricted to development. Stewart
& Kolb (1994) have shown that ovariectomized or gonadectomized adult rats
show significant change in cortical structure, especially in the females. Thus,
the brains of ovariectomized rats not only grew heavier, but the cortical neu-
rons showed a 25% increase in dendritic arbor and a 10% increase in spine
density. This result implies that cortical morphology is hormone-dependent
throughout the life of the animal. Because experience has sexually dimorphic
effects, it seems reasonable to suppose that changes in hormonal state, espe-
cially in postmenopausal women, will alter the brain’s response to experience.
Neurotrophins
Neurotrophins, which are chemicals known to have growth-enhancing proper-
ties in the nervous system, influence dendritic structure and also interact with
experience. It is known, for instance, that administration of nerve growth fac-
tor during adulthood increases both dendritic branching and spine density
throughout the cortex (e.g. Kolb et al 1997b). Furthermore, experience differ-
entially modulates the levels of different neurotrophins such as nerve growth
factor which, in turn, stimulates growth (e.g. Schoups et al 1995). Thus, it is
possible that one route of action of experience on the brain is to stimulate (or
inhibit?) the production of neurotrophins, and these, in turn, alter neuronal
structure. This promises to be an area of intense study in the near future.
Stress
Stress has effects on the neuroendocrine system and this, in turn, has been
shown to affect cell morphology (e.g. Sapolsky 1987, Sirevaag et al 1991,
Stewart & Kolb 1988). Most studies to date have focused on the hippocampal
formation, but there is reason to suspect that cortical neurons are also vulner-
able to the effects of stress (Stewart & Kolb 1988). There is no specific evi-
dence about whether stress interacts with experience-dependent changes in the
brain, but it seems likely.
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 57
Injury
When the cortex is damaged there are changes in the remaining cortex that are
correlated with functional outcome. For example, when Kolb & Gibb (1991b)
removed the frontal cortex in adult rats there was an initial drop in dendritic ar-
borization near the injury. This atrophy slowly resolved and four months later
there was a significant increase in dendritic morphology, which was correlated
with partial restitution of function. In contrast, large sensorimotor cortex le-
sions lead only to neuronal atrophy and no evidence of functional recovery
(Kolb et al 1997b).
These results are reminiscent of those seen in the aging and demented brain:
When there is evidence of dendritic growth it leads to functional recovery,
whereas when there is no dendritic growth there is no recovery. This principle
can be seen even more clearly in the developing brain. One of our consistent
findings over the past decade has been that when the cortex of the developing
rat is damaged in the first few days of life, which corresponds to a time just af-
ter neural proliferation is complete but neural migration and differentiation is
still ongoing, there is a marked generalized atrophy of dendritic arborization
and a decrease in spine density in neurons throughout the cortical mantle (for a
review, see Kolb 1995). This result is correlated with a miserable functional
outcome and is reminiscent of the marked abnormalities in the brains of re-
tarded children (Purpura 1974). In contrast, when the cortical mantle of rats is
damaged in the second week of life, which corresponds to the period of rapid
dendritic growth and synaptic formation, there is a generalized enhancement
of dendritic arborization and/or spine density throughout the remaining cortex
(Kolb & Gibb 1991a). This enhanced dendritic response is correlated with dra-
matic functional recovery. Thus, we see that if the injury in the cortex leads to
increased dendritic space, there is a good functional outcome, whereas if the
injury leads to a retarded development of dendritic material, there is a poor
functional outcome. Furthermore, we have shown that with treatments that re-
verse the dendritic atrophy, such as administration of neurotrophins or housing
in enriched environments, there is functional improvement.
CONCLUSIONS
One of the most intriguing questions in behavioral neuroscience concerns the
manner in which the brain, and especially the neocortex, can modify its func-
tion throughout one’s lifetime. Taken all together, the evidence discussed
above makes a strong case for a relationship between brain plasticity and be-
havioral change. Indeed, it is now clear that experience alters the synaptic or-
ganization of the brain in species as diverse as fruit flies and humans. Evidence
that these changes are functionally meaningful is more difficult to collect, but
58 KOLB & WHISHAW
there is little doubt that changes in synaptic organization are correlated with
changes in behavior. Thus, animals with extensive dendritic growth, relative to
untreated animals, show facilitated performance on many types of behavioral
measures. In contrast, animals with atrophy in dendritic arborization show a
decline in behavioral capacity. Similarly, factors that enhance dendritic
growth (e.g. nerve growth factor) facilitate behavioral outcome, whereas fac-
tors that block dendritic growth (e.g. brain injury at birth in rats) retard func-
tional outcomes. We should emphasize that although we have stressed changes
in dendritic morphology, there are multiple, and likely dissociable, changes in
the neuron morphology that correlate with behavioral change. These include
increases in dendritic length, dendritic branching pattern, spine density, syn-
apse number, synapse size, glial size and number, and metabolic activity.
The critical question that remains is how dendritic and synaptic change is
related to behavioral change. The current evidence clearly shows that den-
drites in the cortex may show a net proliferation, regression, or stability de-
pending upon various factors that affect behavior. It seems likely that a net pro-
liferation of dendrites is a response to an increased availability of afferent sup-
ply. In contrast, the net reduction in dendrites, which is seen in response to in-
jury, for example, is likely to reflect a decline in the afferent supply to a cell. In
this view, dendrites are hypothesized to be in a state in which they are con-
stantly ready to expand or retract their territory, limited largely by the avail-
ability of afferent nourishment and by the metabolic capacities of the cell. Be-
cause changes in the dendritic length are presumed to reflect changes in synap-
tic connectivity, it follows that increased dendritic arbor reflects increased
synapse formation, whereas decreased dendritic arbor reflects decreased syn-
apse formation.
The putative increase (or decrease) in afferent supply to a neuron leads to
the question of where these afferents arise. There is scant evidence that the
adult or even the infant brain is capable of growing new projections over long
distances. Thus, it seems most likely that changes in afferent supply reflect
changes in axonal arborizations of relatively nearby neighbors. A recent analy-
sis by Nicoll & Blakemore (1993) is instructive. They examined the patterns of
connections of pyramidal cells, which are the almost exclusive outputs of the
the neocortex. The axons of pyramidal cells make long-range connections to
other cortical regions or subcortical structures, but they also have axon collat-
erals that form extensive arborizations with nearby cells. In fact, the most com-
mon target of pyramidal cells is other cortical pyramidal cells. Nicoll & Blake-
more estimated that roughly 70% of the excitatory synapses on any layer II/III
pyramidal cell are derived from pyramidal cells in the near vicinity. One way
for the functioning of an intrinsic circuit to change is for the field of influence
of a neuron to change. For example, the diameter of a cell’s dendritic field
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 59
could expand, allowing the cell to interact with a larger number of neurons. Al-
ternatively, the axon terminal could redistribute to enlarge the field of influ-
ence, too. The fact that neurons can expand their field of influence means that
if neurons die, remaining ones could enlarge their field to make up for some of
the lost processing power.
It is assumed in our model that increasing the connectivity of pyramidal
cells will increase their functional capacity. But why should this be? Hebb
(1949) proposed the idea of cell assemblies in which networks of cortical neu-
rons were seen as being responsible for mental activity. A key component of
this model is that individual neurons have little role in behavioral control, but
rather behavior is dependent upon networks of neurons. Furthermore, Hebb
noted that a given neuron could participate in multiple networks, each with a
different function. Calvin (1996) likened this to the person who is on multiple
committees, each with a different function. Thus, if neurons have more con-
nections, they are hypothesized to have more influence on the observed behav-
ior.
A critical feature of this view is that afferent supply influences neuronal
function. We are now left with the question of what controls afferent supply.
Various factors, such as neurotrophins, can influence this supply, but they
must do it through some clear mechanism. One likely candidate is gene expres-
sion. There is ample evidence that the expression of genes in the mature brain
is influenced by environmental and behavioral events (e.g. Dudai 1989). Gene
expression thus provides a mechanism whereby cells can synthesize new pro-
teins needed to form more synapses. Studies in various species, especially
Aplysia, have shown that blockade of protein synthesis blocks long-lasting
changes in both synapses and behavior (Bailey & Kandel 1993). The most
likely mechanism for increased gene activity is neuronal activity, which is
stimulated by behavior and experience. Activity initiated by experience or be-
havior could therefore increase the activity of genetic mechanisms responsible
for dendritic and synaptic growth and, ultimately, behavioral change.
Visit the Annual Reviews home page at
http://www.AnnualReviews.org.
60 KOLB & WHISHAW
Literature Cited
Bailey CH, Kandel ER. 1993. Structural
changes accompanying memory storage.
Annu. Rev. Physiol. 55:397–426
Bateson PPG. 1966. The characteristics and
context of imprinting. Biol. Rev. 41:
177–220
Baxter LR Jr, Mazziotta JC, Phelps ME, Selin
CE, Guze GH, Fairbanks L. 1987. Cerebral
glucose metabolic rates in normal human
females versus normal males. Psychol.
Res. 21:237–45
Beaulieu C, Colonnier M. 1987. Effect of the
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 61
richness of the environment on the cat vis-
ual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 266:478–94
Beaulieu C, Colonnier M. 1988. Richness of
environment affects the number of con-
tacts formed by boutons containing flat
vesicles but does not alter the number of
these boutons per neuron. J. Comp. Neu-
rol. 274:347–56
Becker LE, Armstrong DL, Chan F. 1986.
Dendritic atrophy in children with Down’s
syndrome. Ann. Neurol. 20:520–26
Black JE, Greenough WT, Anderson BJ,
Isaacs KR. 1987. Environment and the ag-
ing brain. Can. J. Psychol. 41:111–30
Black JE, Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara
AA, Greenough WT. 1990. Learning
causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor ac-
tivity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar
cortex of adult rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 87:5568–72
Black JE, Polinsky M, Greenough WT. 1989.
Progressive failure of cerebral angiogene-
sis supporting neural plasticity in aging
rats. Neurobiol. Aging 10:353–58
Bottjer SW, Arnold AP. 1997. Developmental
plasticity in neural circuits for a learned
behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 20:459–81
Brunjes PC, Frazier LL. 1986. Maturation and
plasticity in the olfactory system of verte-
brates. Brain Res. Rev. 11:1–45
Buell SJ, Coleman PD. 1979. Dendritic
growth in the aged human brain and failure
of growth in senile dementia. Science 206:
854–56
Buell SJ, Coleman PD. 1985. Regulation of
dendritic extent in developing and aging
brain. In Synaptic Plasticity, ed. CW Cot-
man, pp. 311–33. New York: Raven
Calvin WH. 1996. The Cerebral Code. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press
Capowski J. 1989. Computer Techniques in
Neuroanatomy. New York: Plenum
Chang F-LF, Greenough WT. 1982. Lateral-
ized effects of monocular training on den-
dritic branching in adult split-brain rats.
Brain Res. 232:283–92
Coleman PD, Flood DG. 1987. Neuron num-
bers and dendritic extent in normal aging
and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Ag-
ing 8:521–45
Diamond MC, Dowling GA, Johnson RE.
1981. Morphologic cerebral cortical asym-
metry in male and female rats. Exp. Neu-
rol. 71:261–68
Diamond MC, Lindner B, Raymond A. 1967.
Extensive cortical depth measurements
and neuron size increases in the cortex of
environmentally enriched rats. J. Comp.
Neurol. 131:357–64
Doving KB, Pinching AJ. 1973. Selective de-
generation of neurones in the olfactory
bulb following prolonged odour exposure.
Brain Res. 52:115–29
Dudai Y. 1989. The Neurobiology of Memory:
Concepts, Findings, Trends. Oxford: Ox-
ford Univ. Press
Floeter MK, Greenough WT. 1979. Cerebellar
plasticity: modification of Purkinje cell
structure by differential rearing in mon-
keys. Science 206:227–29
Forgie ML, Gibb R, Kolb B. 1996. Unilateral
lesions of the forelimb area of rat motor
cortex: lack of evidence for use-dependent
neural growth in the undamaged hemi-
sphere. Brain Res. 710:249–59
Gibb R, Kolb B. 1997. A method of vibratome
sectioning of Golgi-Cox stained whole rat
brain. J. Neurosci. Methods. In press
Globus A, Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL, Dia-
mond MC. 1973. Effects of differential ex-
perience on dendritic spine counts in rat
cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
82:175–81
Greenough WT, Chang FF. 1989. Plasticity of
synapse structure and pattern in the cere-
bral cortex. In Cerebral Cortex, ed. A Pe-
ters, EG Jones, 7:391–440. New York: Ple-
num
Greenough WT, Larson JR, Withers GS.
1985. Effects of unilateral and bilateral
training in a reaching task on dendritic
branching of neurons in the rat motor-
sensory forelimb cortex. Behav. Neur.
Biol. 44:301–14
Greenough WT, Volkmar FR. 1973. Pattern of
dendritic branching in occipital cortex of
rats reared in complex environments. Exp.
Neurol. 40:491–504
Greenough WT, Volkmar FR, Juraska JM.
1973. Effects of rearing complexity on
dendritic branching in frontolateral and
temporal cortex of the rat. Exp. Neurol.
41:371–78
Hebb DO. 1947. The effects of early experi-
ence on problem solving at maturity. Am.
Psychol. 2:737–45
Hebb DO. 1949. The Organization of Behav-
ior. New York: Wiley
Heisenberg M, Heusipp M, Wanke C. 1995.
Structural plasticity in the Drosophila
brain. J. Neurosci. 15:1951–60
Horn G. 1985. Memory, Imprinting, and the
Brain: An Inquiry Into Mechanisms. Ox-
ford: Clarendon
Horn G, Bradley P, McCabe BJ. 1985.
Changes in the structure of synapses asso-
ciated with learning. J. Neurosci. 5:
3161–68
Hoy RR, Nolen TG, Casaday GC. 1985. Den-
dritic sprouting and compensatory synap-
62 KOLB & WHISHAW
togenesis in an identified interneuron fol-
low auditory deprivation in a cricket. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 82:7772–76
Jacobs B, Schall M, Scheibel AB. 1993. A
quantitative dendritic analysis of Wer-
nicke’s area. II. Gender, Hemispheric, and
environmental factors. J. Comp. Neurol.
237:97–111
Jacobs GA, Weeks JC. 1990. Postsynaptic
changes at a sensory to motoneuron syn-
apse contribute to the developmental loss
of a reflex behavior during insect meta-
morphosis. J. Neurosci. 10:1341–56
Jay V, Chan F-W, Becker LE. 1990. Dendritic
arborization in the human fetus and infant
with trisomy 18 syndrome. Dev. Brain
Res. 54:291–94
Jones TA, Schallert T. 1994. Use-dependent
growth of pyramidal neurons after neocor-
tical damage. J. Neurosci. 14:2140–52
Juraska JM. 1984. Sex differences in den-
dritic responses to differential experience
in the rat visual cortex. Brain Res. 295:
27–34
Juraska JM. 1986. Sex differences in develop-
mental plasticity of behavior and the brain.
In Developmental Neuropsychobiology,
ed. WT Greenough, JM Juraska, pp.
409–22. New York: Academic
Juraska JM. 1990. The structure of the cerebral
cortex: Effects of gender and the environ-
ment. In The Cerebral Cortex of the Rat,
ed. B Kolb, R Tees, pp. 483–506. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press
Juraska JM, Fitch J, Henderson C, Rivers N.
1985. Sex differences in the dendritic
branching of dentate granule cells follow-
ing differential experience. Brain Res.
333:73–80
Juraska JM, Fitch JM, Washburne DL. 1989.
The dendritic morphology of pyramidal
neurons in the rat hippocampal CA3 area.
II. Effects of gender and experience. Brain
Res. 479:115–21
Kent KS, Levine RG. 1993. Dendritic reor-
ganization of an identified neuron during
metamorphosis of the moth Manduca
sexta: the influence of interactions with the
periphery. J. Neurobiol. 24:1–22
Kolb B. 1995. Brain Plasticity and Behavior.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Kolb B, Cioe J. 1996. Sex-related differences
in cortical function after medial frontal le-
sions in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 110:
1271–81
Kolb B, Cote S, Ribeiro-da-Silva A, Cuello
AC. 1996. NGF stimulates recovery of
function and dendritic growth after unilat-
eral motor cortex lesions in rats. Neurosci-
ence 76: 1139–51
Kolb B, Forgie M, Gibb R, Gorny G, Rowntree
S. 1997a. Age, experience and the chang-
ing brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. In
press
Kolb B, Gibb R. 1991a. Sparing of function af-
ter neonatal frontal lesions correlates with
increased cortical dendritic branching: a
possible mechanism for the Kennard ef-
fect. Behav. Brain Res. 43:51–56
Kolb B, Gibb R. 1991b. Environmental en-
richment and cortical injury: behavioral
and anatomical consequences of frontal
cortex lesions in rats. Cereb. Cortex 1:
189–98
Kolb B, Gibb R, Gorny G, Ouellette A. 1996.
Experience dependent changes in cortical
morphology are age dependent. Soc. Neu-
rosci. Abstr. 22:1133
Kolb B, Gorny G, Cote S, Ribeiro-da-Silva A,
Cuello AC. 1997b. Nerve growth factor
stimulates growth of cortical pyramidal
neurons in young adult rats. Brain Res.
751:289–94
Kolb B, Stewart J. 1991. Sex-related differ-
ences in dendritic branching of cells in the
prefrontal cortex of rats. J. Neuroendocri-
nol. 3:95–99
Kolb B, Stewart J. 1995. Changes in neonatal
gonadal hormonal environment prevent
behavioral sparing and alter cortical mor-
phogenesis after early frontal cortex le-
sions in male and female rats. Behav. Neu-
rosci. 109:285–94
Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. 1996. Fundamentals of
Human Neuropsychology. New York:
Freeman. 4th ed.
Leon M. 1992a. Neuroethology of olfactory
preference development. J. Neurobiol. 23:
1557–73
Leon M. 1992b. The neurobiology of filial
learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 43:377–98
Lois C, Alvarez-Buylla A. 1994. Long-
distance neuronal migration in the adult
mammalian brain. Science 264:1145–48
Marin-Padilla M. 1974. Structural organiza-
tion of the cerebral cortex (motor area) in
human chromosomal aberrations: a Golgi
study. I. D½1 (13–15) trisomy, Patau syn-
drome. Brain Res. 66:375–91
Matus A, Pehling G, Wilkinson D. 1981.
Gamma-aminobutyric acid receptors in
brain postsynaptic densities. J. Neurobiol.
12:67–73
Meisami E, Safari L. 1981. A quantitative
study of the effects of early unilateral ol-
factory deprivation on the number and dis-
tribution of mitral and tufted cells and of
glomeruli in the rat olfactory bulb. Brain
Res. 222:81–107
Milner D, Goodale MA. 1995. The Visual
BRAIN PLASTICITY AND BEHAVIOR 63
Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford Univ.
Press
Nicoll A, Blakemore C. 1993. Patterns of local
connectivity in the neocortex. Neural
Comput. 5:665–80
Pasternak JF, Woolsey TA. 1987. On the
“selectivityá” of the Golgi-Cox method. J.
Comp. Neurol. 160:307–12
Patel SN, Rose SPR, Stewart MG. 1988.
Training induced dendritic spine density
changes are specifically related to memory
formation processes in the chick, Gallus
domesticus. Brain Res. 463:168–73
Patel SN, Stewart MG. 1988. Changes in the
number and structure of dendritic spines
25 hours after passive avoidance training
in the domestic chick, Gallus domesticus.
Brain Res. 449:34–46
Pinching AJ, Doving KB. 1974. Selective de-
generation in the rat olfactory bulb follow-
ing exposure to different odours. Brain
Res. 82:195–204
Prusky G, Whishaw IQ. 1996. Morphology
of identified corticospinal cells in the rat
following motor cortex injury: absence
of use-dependent change. Brain Res. 714:
1–8
Purpura DP. 1974. Dendritic spine “dysgene-
sis” and mental retardation. Science 186:
1126–28
Ramon y Cajal S. 1928. Degeneration and Re-
generation of the Nervous System. Lon-
don: Oxford Univ. Press
Rehn B, Breipohl W. 1986. Transient postna-
tal impacts on the mouse olfactory epithe-
lium proprium affect the granule cell de-
velopment in the mouse olfactory bulb. In
Ontogeny of Olfaction in Vertebrates, ed.
W Breipohl, pp. 143–56. Berlin: Springer-
Verlag
Rehn B, Breipohl W, Mendoza AS, Apfelbach
A. 1986. Changes in granule cells of the
ferret olfactory bulb associated with im-
printing on prey odours. Brain Res. 373:
114–25
Riesen AH, Dickerson GP, Struble RG. 1977.
Somatosensory restriction and behavioral
development in stumptail monkeys. Ann.
NY Acad. Sci. 290:285–94
Rose SPR. 1985. The cell biological conse-
quences of passive avoidance training in
the chick. Adv. Behav. Biol. 28:39–49
Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. 1978. Experien-
tial influences on brain anatomy and brain
chemistry in rodents. In Studies on the De-
velopment of Behavior and the Nervous
System, ed. G Gottlieb, pp. 289–387. New
York: Academic
Rosenzweig MR, Krech D, Bennett EL, Dia-
mond M. 1962. Effects of environmental
complexity and training on brain chemis-
try and anatomy: a replication and exten-
sion. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55:
429–37
Rosselli-Austin L, Williams J. 1990. Enriched
neonatal odor exposure leads to increased
numbers of olfactory bulb mitral and gran-
ule cells. Dev. Brain Res. 51:135–37
Sapolsky RM. 1987. Glucocorticoids and hip-
pocampal damage. Trends Neurosci. 10:
346–49
Schade JB, Baxter CF. 1960. Changes during
growth in volume and surface area of corti-
cal neurons in the rabbit. Exp. Neurol. 2:
158–78
Scheibel AB, Conrad T, Perdue S, Tomiyasu
U, Wechsler A. 1990. A quantitative study
of dendrite complexity in selected areas of
the human cerebral cortex. Brain Cogn.
12:85–101
Schoups AA, Elliott RC, Friedman WJ, Black
IB. 1995. NGF and BDNF are differen-
tially modulated by visual experience in
the developing geniculocortical pathway.
Dev. Brain Res. 86:326–34
Sirevaag AM, Black JE, Greenough WT.
1991. Astrocyte hypertrophy in the dentate
gyrus of young male rats reflects variation
of individual stress rather than group envi-
ronmental complexity manipulations. Exp.
Neurol. 111:74–79
Sirevaag AM, Greenough WT. 1987. Differ-
ential rearing effects on rat visual cortex
synapses. III. Neuronal and glial nuclei,
boutons, dendrites, and capillaries. Brain
Res. 424:320–32
Sirevaag AM, Greenough WT. 1988. A multi-
variate statistical summary of synaptic
plasticity measures in rats exposed to com-
plex, social and individual environments.
Brain Res. 441:386–92
Sirevaag AM, Greenough WT. 1991. Plastic-
ity of GFA-immunoreactive astrocyte size
and number in visual cortex of rats reared
in complex environments. Brain Res. 540:
273–78
Skeen LC, Due BR, Douglas FE. 1986. Neona-
tal sensory deprivation reduces tufted cell
number in mouse olfactory bulbs. Neuro-
sci. Lett. 63:5–10
Stell M, Riesen A. 1987. Effects of early envi-
ronments on monkey cortex neuroana-
tomical changes following somatomotor
experience: effects on layer III pyramidal
cells in monkey cortex. Behav. Neurosci.
101:341–46
Stewart J, Kolb B. 1988. The effects of neona-
tal gonadectomy and prenatal stress on
cortical thickness and asymmetry in rats.
Behav. Neural Biol. 49:344–60
64 KOLB & WHISHAW
Stewart J, Kolb B. 1994. Dendritic branching
in cortical pyramidal cells in response to
ovariectomy in adult female rats: suppres-
sion by neonatal exposure to testosterone.
Brain Res. 654:149–54
Struble RG, Riesen AH. 1978. Changes in cor-
tical dendritic branching subsequent to
partial social isolation in stumptail mon-
keys. Dev. Psychobiol. 11:479–86
Tanzi E. 1893. I fatti e le induzioni nell’ od-
ierna istologia del sistema nervoso. Riv.
Sper. Freniatr. Med. Leg. Alienazioni
Ment. 19:419–72
Technau G. 1984. Fiber number in the mush-
room bodies of adult Drosophila mela-
nogaster depends on age, sex and experi-
ence. J. Neurogenet. 1:13–26
Tieman SB, Hirsch HVB. 1982. Exposure to
lines of only one orientation modifies den-
dritic morphology of cells in the visual
cortex of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 211:
353–62
Turner AM, Greenough WT. 1983. Synapses
per neuron and synaptic dimensions in oc-
cipital cortex of rats reared in complex, so-
cial, or isolation housing. Acta Sterol.
2(Suppl. 1):239–44
Turner AM, Greenough WT. 1985. Differen-
tial rearing effects on rat visual cortex syn-
apses. I. Synaptic and neuronal density and
synapses per neuron. Brain Res. 329:
195–203
Uylings HBM, Kuypers K, Diamond M, Velt-
man WAM. 1978. Environmental influ-
ences on neocortex in later life. Prog.
Brain Res. 48:261–74
Volkmar FR, Greenough WT. 1972. Rearing
complexity affects branching of dendrites
in visual cortex of the rat. Science 176:
1445–47
Wallhausser E, Scheich H. 1987. Auditory im-
printing leads to differential 2-deoxy-
glucose uptake and dendritic spine loss in
the chick rostral forebrain. Dev. Brain Res.
31:29–44
Whishaw IQ, Miklyaeva EI. 1996. A rat’s
reach should exceed its grasp: analysis of
independent limb and digit use in the labo-
ratory rat. In Measuring Movement and
Locomotion: From Invertebrates to Hu-
mans, ed. P-K Ossenkopp, M Kavaliers,
PR Sanburg, pp. 135–69. Austin, TX: Lan-
des
Withers GS, Fahrbach SE, Robinson GE.
1993. Selective neuroanatomical plasticity
and division of labour in the honey bee.
Nature 364:238–40
Withers GS, Greenough WT. 1989. Reaching
training selectively alters dendritic
branching in subpopulations of layer II-III
pyramids in rat motor-somatosensory fore-
limb cortex. Neuropsychology 27:61–69
We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.
Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.
Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.
Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.
Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.
Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.
We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.
Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.
You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.
Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.
Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.
You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.
You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.
Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.
We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.
We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.
We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.
Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!
Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality
Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.
We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.
We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.
We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.
We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.