I will provide the winning bidder with (5) peer-reviewed journals to read. After reading the journals then write a 250 word, 4-5 sentence, summary for each of the journals.
* I will provide the sources (after winning bid)
** I will provide the word document template
*** I expect well written, college-level of work.
BAD WORK WILL BE DISPUTED AND I WILL INSTANTLY ASK ADMIN FOR A FULL REFUND.
Annotated Reference Page
Brooks, J. K. (2003). Emotional competencies of leaders: A comparison of managers in a financial organization by performance level (Order No. 3073312). Available from Retrieved from:
https://nuls.idm.oclc.org/login?qurl=https%3A%2F%2F
www.proquest.com%2Fdissertations-theses%2Femotional-competencies-leaders-comparison%2Fdocview%2F305313268%2Fse-2%3Faccountid%3D25320
(Insert 200 word Summery Here)
Khalili, A. (2012). The role of emotional intelligence in the workplace: A literature review. International Journal of Management, 29(3), 355-370. Retrieved from https://nuls.idm.oclc.org/login?qurl=https%3A%2F%2F www.proquest.com%2Fscholarly-journals%2Frole-emotional-intelligence-workplace-literature%2Fdocview%2F1040715974%2Fse-2%3Faccountid%3D25320
(Insert 200 word Summery Here)
Miao, C., Humphrey, R. H., & Qian, S. (2017). A meta-analysis of emotional intelligence and work attitudes. Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology, 90(2), 177–202.
https://doi-org.nuls.idm.oclc.org/10.1111/joop.12167
(Insert 200 word Summery Here)
Nikolaou, I., & Tsaousis, I. (2002). EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN THE WORKPLACE: EXPLORING ITS EFFECTS ON OCCUPATIONAL STRESS AND ORGANIZATIONAL COMMITMENT. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 10(4), 327-342. Retrieved from
https://nuls.idm.oclc.org/login?qurl=https%3A%2F%2Fwww.proquest.com%2Fscholarly-journals%2Femotional-intelligence-workplace-exploring%2Fdocview%2F198703266%2Fse-2%3Faccountid%3D25320
(Insert 200 word Summery Here)
Tischler, L., Biberman, J., & McKeage, R. (2002). Linking emotional intelligence, spirituality and workplace performance: Definitions, models and ideas for research. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 17(3), 203-218. doi:http://dx.doi.org.nuls.idm.oclc.org/10.1108/02683940210423114
(Insert 200 word Summery Here)
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology (2017), 90,
177
–202
© 2016 The British Psychological Society
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com
Ameta-analysis of emotional intelligence andwork
attitudes
Chao Miao1*, Ronald H. Humphrey2 and Shanshan Qian3
1Finance, Accounting and Management Department, Jay S. Sidhu School of Business &
Leadership, Wilkes University, Wilkes-Barre, Pennsylvania, USA
2Department of Leadership and Management, Lancaster University Management
School, Lancaster, UK
3Department ofManagement,College of Business and Economics, TowsonUniversity,
Towson, Maryland, USA
Ourmeta-analysis of emotional intelligence (EI) demonstrates that: First, all three types of
EI are significantly related to job satisfaction (ability EI: q̂ = .08; self-report EI: q̂ = .32; and
mixed EI: q̂ = .39). Second, both self-report EI and mixed EI exhibit modest yet
statistically significant incremental validity (DR2 = .03 for self-report EI andDR2 = .06 for
mixed EI) and large relative importance (31.3% for self-report EI and 42.8% formixed EI) in
the presence of cognitive ability and personality when predicting job satisfaction. Third,
we foundmixed support for themoderator effects (i.e., emotional labour demand of jobs)
for the relationship between EI and job satisfaction. Fourth, the relationships between all
three types of EI and job satisfaction are mediated by state affect and job performance.
Fifth, EI significantly relates to organizational commitment (self-report EI: q̂ = .43; mixed
EI: q̂ = .43) and turnover intentions (self-report EI: q̂ = �.33). Sixth, after controls, both
self-report EI and mixed EI demonstrate incremental validity and relative importance
(46.9% for self-report EI; 44.2% for mixed EI) in predicting organizational commitment.
Seventh, self-report EI demonstrates incremental validity and relative importance (60.9%)
in predicting turnover intentions.
Practitioner points
� Employees with higher emotional intelligence (EI) have higher job satisfaction, higher organizational
commitment, and lower turnover intentions.
� Adding EI measures to the set of personality and cognitive measures currently being used can improve
the ability to assess employee job satisfaction, organizational
commitment, and turnover intentions.
� EI improves job satisfaction by helping employees reduce negative feelings, by increasing positive
feelings, and/or by improving job performance.
� To produce productive and satisfied workers, organizations should incorporate EI in employee
recruitment, training, and development programmes.
Emotional intelligence (EI; Mayer, Roberts, & Barsade, 2008; Mayer & Salovey, 1997) is
defined ‘as the set of abilities (verbal and non-verbal) that enable a person to generate,
recognize, express, understand, and evaluate their own, and others’, emotions in order
*Correspondence should be addressed to Chao Miao, Finance, Accounting and Management Department, Jay S. Sidhu School of
Business & Leadership, Wilkes University, Wilkes-Barre, PA 18766, USA (email: chao.miao@wilkes.edu).
DOI:10.1111/joop.12167
177
to guide thinking and action that successfully cope with environmental demands and
pressures’ (Van Rooy & Viswesvaran, 2004, p. 72). There is a large volume of evidence
both confirming the predictive validity of EI and indicating that EI predicts outcomes
such as academic performance, emotional labour, job performance, organizational
citizenship behaviour, workplace deviance, leadership, life satisfaction, stress, trust,
team process effectiveness, and work–family conflict (Ashkanasy & Daus, 2002; Bar-On,
2000; Gooty, Connelly, Griffith, & Gupta, 2010; Humphrey, 2002, 2013; Jordan,
Ashkanasy, Hartel, & Hooper, 2002; Kellett, Humphrey, & Sleeth, 2006; Kluemper,
DeGroot, & Choi, 2013; O’Boyle, Humphrey, Pollack, Hawver, & Story, 2011; Winkel,
Wyland, Shaffer, & Clason, 2011). However, the relationships between EI and work
attitudes remain indeterminate and unclear. Differences in effect sizes between primary
studies also suggest a need to meta-analyse the relationships between EI and work
attitudes. It is also important to understand how EI influences work attitudes, and so we
unpack its relationship with job satisfaction by evaluating mediating and moderating
factors in this relationship. Finally, as organizations consider developing the EI of their
employees, it is important to understand how this variance affects worker attitudes.
In this study, we performed a meta-analysis on the relationships between EI and three
important work attitudes: job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover
intentions. EI may influence work attitudes in a variety of ways. Job satisfaction reflects
appraisal-based reactions towards one’s job, meaning that favourable evaluations of work
characteristics produce job satisfaction and unfavourable appraisals of work character-
istics engender job dissatisfaction (Breaux, Munyon, Hochwarter, & Ferris, 2009; Weiss,
2002). EI may have a considerable influence on howpeople appraise their jobs because EI
consists, in part, of the ability to reason effectively about events that produce positive or
negative emotions. Consequently, EI may have a strong influence on how employees
interpret and react to work events.
Job performance is a key work event, and to the extent that employees find that
performing well at work helps them meet their personal goals, then high jo
b
performance should increase job satisfaction (Locke, 1969) and other work attitudes.
Thus, it is not only the level of the performance, but the employees’ perceptual
processes and personal goals that determine whether job performance increases job
satisfaction (Munyon, Hochwarter, Perrew�e, & Ferris, 2010). EI may be a characteristic
that predisposes employees to see job performance in a light that enhances job
satisfaction. Although many models of attitudes and behaviours assume that attitudes
cause behaviours, self-perception theory (Bem, 1967) suggests that people also observe
their behaviours in order to infer their own attitudes. There is considerable evidence that
job performance can influence attitudes (Judge, Thoresen, Bono, & Patton, 2001; Locke
& Latham, 2002). Prior meta-analyses have established that EI is positively related to job
performance (O’Boyle et al., 2011). Thus, EI should have an indirect path through job
performance to job satisfaction.
These same processes may also occur when employees observe their other work
behaviours (i.e., besides their direct job performance) and the other aspects of their work
environment. In other words, EI may help cast a rosy glow over a wide variety of work
events and help employees interpret them in a positive light, one that promotes positive
affect and diminishes negative affect. Employees who then observe their positive mood
and a positive workplace will naturally then infer that they have high job satisfaction.
Because positive affect and negative affect are important mediators according to Affective
Events Theory (AET;Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), EI’s effects onwork attitudes should be
at least partially mediated by positive and negative state affect.
178 Chao Miao et al.
Our research has several key purposes. No prior meta-analyses have examined EI
and work attitudes, so the following relationships have not been tested using meta-
analytic techniques for establishing the most accurate estimates of effects sizes,
incremental validity, moderators, and mediator effects. We have addressed this by
first using meta-analysis to more accurately determine the overall size of the
relationships between EI and work attitudes (job satisfaction, organizational
commitment, and turnover intentions). Second, we use meta-analyses to control
for personality and cognitive intelligence and to test for EI’s incremental
predictability and relative importance when predicting work attitudes. Third, we
test for an important moderator of the EI–job satisfaction relationship. Fourth, we
examine whether the EI–job satisfaction relationship is mediated by state affect and
by job performance.
Theory and hypotheses
Employee EI and work attitudes
The classification of EI. The construct of EI has received substantial attention from
researchers in the fields of psychology andmanagement (Joseph&Newman, 2010;Kellett
et al., 2006; Kluemper et al., 2013;Mayer et al., 2008). EI is argued to be apredictor of job
performance (Goleman, 1995) and effective leadership (Walter & Bruch, 2009; Walter,
Cole, & Humphrey, 2011). To make sense of this considerable research, Ashkanasy and
Daus (2005, p. 441) categorized EI research into three streams. These have become
known as ability EI (stream 1), self-report EI (stream 2), and mixed EI (stream 3). To show
that EI measures can satisfy the traditional criteria for intelligence measures by having
objective right and wrong answers, Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, and Sitarenios (2003)
developed and refined their measure – MSCEIT V2.0. MSCEIT V2.0 is a representative
ability EI measure. Researchers have found that subscales of the MSCEIT V2.0 have
predictive ability for importantwork-related variables evenwhen controlling for cognitive
intelligence and the Big Five personality traits (Kluemper et al., 2013). Abilitymeasures of
EI have also been shown to be related to emotion-focused coping,which in turn facilitates
performance (Gooty, Gavin, Ashkanasy, & Thomas, 2014). Other researchers believe that
self-reports are an excellent way to assess EI because intrapersonal processes, such as an
awareness of one’s emotions, are most easily measured by self-assessments of internal
states (Petrides & Furnham, 2003). These researchers often conceptualize EI as a trait
rather than as an ability (Petrides, 2009a, 2009b; Petrides & Furnham, 2003). Represen-
tativemeasures in the stream2 self-reports category include theAssessing Emotions Scales
(Schutte et al., 1998), theWorkgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (Jordan et al., 2002),
and the Wong & Law Emotional Intelligence Scale (Wong & Law, 2002). Representative
mixed EI measures include the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (Bar-On, 2000) and
the Emotional and Social Competency Inventory (Boyatzis, Brizz, & Godwin, 2011). Like
the stream 2 self-reports, mixed EI measures use self-report measures; however, they
include a broader set of variables and competencies as well as traits.
It is noted that these three streams of EI are related yet still distinct from each
other in a number of ways. O’Boyle and his colleagues demonstrated that ‘Because
stream 3 measures overlap both in their measurement method and in the content of
their questions, while stream 2 measures only overlap with regard to the use of self-
reports, stream 3 measures should show higher relationships with personality factors
than stream 2 measures. . …stream 3 measures, unlike stream 2, include measures of
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 179
personality factors not directly related to EI, so it is likely that these measures will
overlap more with similar personality measures’ (2011, p. 793). It is worthwhile to
point out that some overlap between EI and other constructs is reasonable and is
indicative of construct validity because EI should be related to personality variables
such as emotional stability (O’Boyle et al., 2011). Their meta-analytic results
confirmed this prediction and showed that corrected correlations between personality
and both stream 1 ability EI and stream 2 self-report EI range from weak to moderate,
whereas the corrected correlations between personality and stream 3 mixed EI range
from moderate to strong.
Meta-analytic findings of EI. There are multiple meta-analytic reviews that investigated
the relationship between EI and job performance. Van Rooy and Viswesvaran (2004)
performed a meta-analysis and reported a .23 operational validity of EI in predicting
performance; they concluded that EI is indeed a valuable predictor of performance. They
also found that overall EI has small to moderate corrected correlations with personality
traits and a small corrected correlationwith cognitive ability. Joseph and Newman (2010)
meta-analytically integrated the research on EI and job performance, and proposed
a
cascading model of EI with emotional labour as a moderator. O’Boyle et al. (2011)
performed ameta-analysis of EI and contributed to the cumulative scientific knowledge by
improving the two aforementioned meta-analyses. For instance, O’Boyle and his
colleagues included more studies and examined how each type of EI measure correlated
with Big Five personality measures and cognitive ability. They employed dominance
analysis to assess the relative importance of each EI stream in predicting job performance.
They found that all three streamsof EI correlatedwith jobperformance and that self-report
EI andmixed EI exhibited incremental validities over and above cognitive intelligence and
the five factor model (FFM) of personality in predicting job performance. Dominance
analyses also demonstrated that all three streams of EI showed meaningful relative
importance for the prediction of job performance in the presence of the FFM and
cognitive intelligence.
Job satisfaction. Job satisfaction is one of the most influential, important, and popular
constructs in the area of organizational psychology because it is a predictor of many
critical behavioural, attitudinal, and health-related outcomes, such as organizational
citizenship behaviour, counterproductive work behaviour, task performance, organiza-
tional commitment, turnover intention, turnover, withdrawal cognitions and behaviours,
and physical and psychological health outcomes (Judge & Kammeyer-Mueller, 2012;
Schleicher, Hansen, & Fox, 2011). There exist multiple definitions of job satisfaction.
Weiss (2002) pointed out that the attitudinal approach to defining job satisfaction is the
most accepted one in the literature. This approach conceptualizes job satisfaction as
having both affective (emotional) and cognitive (belief) bases (Fisher, 2000; Weiss, 2002;
Weiss, Nicholas, & Daus, 1999). The affective base of job satisfaction refers to one’s
feelings about an attitude object, whereas the cognitive base of job satisfaction refers to
one’s beliefs about an attitude object (Schleicher et al., 2011). This conceptualization of
job satisfaction dovetails with goal setting theory, which suggests that job satisfaction, at
its core, reflects goal achievement at work because both affective and cognitive bases of
job satisfaction are influenced by one’s progress towards goal achievement (Diener, Suh,
Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Locke, 1969, 1976; Locke, Cartledge, &Knerr, 1970). Goals serve as
180 Chao Miao et al.
the reference criteria for satisfaction versus dissatisfaction, meaning that for any given
trial, the achievement of goals produces satisfaction and failure to reach goals creates
dissatisfaction (Locke & Latham, 2002). Across trials, the more goals one reaches, the
higher one’s satisfaction is.
EI – job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intentions. Emo-
tional intelligence should positively relate to job satisfaction and organizational commit-
ment, and be negatively related to turnover intentions. Emotionally intelligent individuals
are able to regulate their emotions,meaning they are less likely to leave an organization due
to emotional shocks and so may have reduced turnover intentions and greater
organizational embeddedness (Mitchell, Holtom, Lee, Sablynski, & Erez, 2001; Mitchell
& Lee, 2001). Similarly, we would expect EI to positively predict organizational
commitment, as employees view work as instrumental in achieving their work-related
goals. As previously mentioned, job satisfaction consists of job appraisals, such that
satisfactory assessments of work characteristics produce job satisfaction and negative
judgements of work characteristics create job dissatisfaction (Breaux et al., 2009; Weiss,
2002). EI encompasses the ability to reason productively about positive and negative
workplace events and thus shouldhave a strong influenceonhowemployees interpret and
respond towork events.Whenhigh job performance helps employeesmeet their personal
goals, it should increase job satisfaction (Locke, 1969; Locke & Latham, 2002) and
organizational commitment and thereby reduce turnover intentions. Because EI improves
job performance (O’Boyle et al., 2011), it should indirectly influence job satisfaction.
Emotionally savvy individuals are inclined to interpret their jobs as more satisfying and
rewarding rather than threatening and hostile (Fox & Spector, 2000; Kong & Zhao, 2013;
Thoresen, Kaplan, Barsky, Warren, & de Chermont, 2003; Walter & Bruch, 2009). This is
because emotionally intelligent individuals are more resilient, are more likely to bounce
back from negative feelings, and are more adept at evaluating and regulating their own
emotions (Sy, Tram, & O’Hara, 2006). Emotionally intelligent individuals have a greater
understanding of the causes of stress. Consequently, they know how to craft effective
plans to deal with negative outcomes in order to maintain positive feelings and high job
satisfaction. This may be one reason why people high on EI have better physical and
mental health, according to twometa-analyses (Martins, Ramalho,&Morin, 2010; Schutte,
Malouff, Thorsteinsson, Bhullar, & Rooke, 2007). In addition, people with high EI can
accurately read others’ emotions, and reading others’ emotions helps people understand
how to respond to others and how to act in appropriate ways in a variety of social
situations (Byron, 2007). As a result, employees with high EI should have positive social
relationshipswith others in theworkplace, and this should result in higher
job satisfaction
and organizational commitment (Goleman, 1995; Kafetsios & Zampetakis, 2008).
Empirical findings support links between EI and overall job satisfaction (Kafetsios &
Zampetakis, 2008; Sy et al., 2006;Wong& Law, 2002) and between EI and organizational
commitment and turnover intentions (Jordan & Troth, 2011).
Hypothesis 1: EI should positively relate to job satisfaction and organizational commitment, and EI
should negatively relate to turnover intentions.
Incremental validity and relative importance of EI. Emotional intelligence denotes
variation in the extent to which people can resolve a set of problems involving emotions,
thus differentiating EI from other intelligence factors that primarily centre on cognitive
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 181
processes (Côt�e, 2014; Côt�e & Miners, 2006; Mayer et al., 2008). EI refers to a general
intelligence in the realm of emotions, whereas cognitive intelligence refers to a general
intelligence in the realmof cognition (Côt�e, 2014; Côt�e&Miners, 2006). As such, EI differs
from cognitive intelligence due to its unique representation of intelligence in the domain
of emotion. EI differs from personality as well, because personality does not reflect one’s
ability/intelligence, whereas EI does (Joseph & Newman, 2010). Due to these reasons, EI
has unique content and has often displayed incremental validity in predicting outcomes
over othermeasures of intelligence, socio-emotional traits, and personality factors, which
has already been supported by meta-analytic findings (Côt�e, 2014; Mayer et al., 2008).
Meta-analytic findings have demonstrated that the overlap between all three types of EI
and cognitive ability is weak tomoderate and the overlap between all three types of EI and
FFM is weak to moderate in general (O’Boyle et al., 2011). It is worthwhile to point out
that someoverlap is reasonable and is indicative of construct validity, because EI should be
related to personality variables such as emotional stability (O’Boyle et al., 2011). EI should
also relate to cognitive ability because it is a form of intelligence (Côt�e, 2014).
Taken altogether, despite the overlap between EI and cognitive ability and personality,
there is still much unique variance in EI that cannot be explained by personality and
cognitive ability, and this unique variance may predict job satisfaction, organizational
commitment, and turnover intentions above and beyond personality and cognitive ability
(Côt�e, 2014; O’Boyle et al., 2011; Sy et al., 2006). We accordingly offer the following
hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2: EI should contribute incremental validity and relative importance in the presence of
the FFM and cognitive ability when predicting job satisfaction, organizational
commitment, and turnover intentions.
Differential validity of EI. Personality is a good predictor of job satisfaction because
personality traits reflect one’s affective disposition and influence one’s interpretation of
job characteristics and one’s mood at work (Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002). On the other
hand, cognitive ability is a weak predictor of job satisfaction because cognitive ability is a
cognitive trait, whereas job satisfaction is primarily determined by affective dispositions,
such as personality traits like extraversion and neuroticism (Ganzach, 1998; Staw, Bell, &
Clausen, 1986). To support these arguments, meta-analytic findings have demonstrated
that personality is a good predictor of job satisfaction (Judge et al., 2002), whereas
cognitive ability is a weak predictor of job satisfaction (Gonzalez-Mul�e, Carter, & Mount,
2014).
Mixed EI has the highest correlation with personality, self-report EI the next highest,
and ability EI the lowest correlation with personality (O’Boyle et al., 2011). In addition,
ability EI has the highest correlation with cognitive ability, while self-report EI and mixed
EI have small correlations with cognitive ability. Overall, this suggests that
ability EI
measures may be similar to cognitive intelligence measures in their impact on job
satisfaction and thus have fairly small correlations with job satisfaction. In a similar vein,
mixed EI measures have the highest associations with personality measures and thus
should have the largest correlation with job satisfaction. We thus propose the following
hypothesis:
Hypothesis 3: Mixed EIwill show the strongest relationshipwith job satisfaction, self-report EI the
next strongest, and ability EI the weakest relationship with job satisfaction.
182 Chao Miao et al.
The mediating role of affect
Emotional intelligence may be a characteristic that inclines employees to view a wide
variety of organizational events in amanner that augments positive affect. Consistentwith
self-perception theory (Bem, 1967), employeesmay observe their positivemoods at work
and infer that they have high job satisfaction. This is consistent with AET Weiss &
Cropanzano, 1996),which also provides explanations for the positive link between EI and
job satisfaction through state affect. State affect refers to ‘what one is feeling at any given
moment in time’ (Thoresen et al., 2003, p. 915). State positive affect (SPA) refers to
pleasant emotions such as feeling active, alert, and energetic at any givenmoment in time,
whereas state negative affect (SNA) refers to the momentary experience of anger, fear,
nervousness, and other negative emotions at any given moment in time (Watson, 2000;
Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Meta-analytic findings have indicated that SPA is
positively related to job satisfaction and personal accomplishment and negatively related
to emotional exhaustion and depersonalization, whereas SNA is negatively related to job
satisfaction and personal accomplishment and positively related to emotional exhaustion
and depersonalization (Thoresen et al., 2003).
Affective Events Theory suggests that each individual should have an average affective
mood level and that somepeople tend tobeon thepositive half,whereas others tend to be on
the negative half. Further, responding to discrete ‘affective events’ in the workplace will
influence affective responses, thus leading to affective, attitudinal, andbehavioural outcomes;
as such, this average mood level can be either diminished or raised by negative or positive
events at work. Hence, affective reactions generated byworkplace events (i.e., SPA and SNA)
create ebb and flow in job satisfaction (Ashkanasy & Humphrey, 2011; Humphrey, 2013;
Johnson, 2009; Walter & Bruch, 2009; Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996; Weiss et al., 1999).
Building onAET,we argue that there are twoprominent categories of reasonswhy EI is
associated with job satisfaction – enhancement of SPA and reduction in SNA. Job
satisfaction has an affective (i.e., feeling) component (Weiss, 2002; Weiss et al., 1999).
We propose that EI may contribute to the affective base of job satisfaction by increasing
SPA and decreasing SNA. Emotionally intelligent individuals are able to identify and
interpret cues that activate self-regulatory action in order to cultivate SPA and circumvent
SNA (Karim, 2009; Mayer & Salovey, 1997). High EI individuals are better at handling
affective processes because they can accurately perceive and monitor their own feelings
and precisely process emotional information in order to effectively respond to their
feelings. This allows them to develop appropriate strategies to regulate SNA andmaintain
SPA (Dong, Seo, & Bartol, 2013). They are sensitive and reactive to positive emotion-
invoking experiences at work, thus making them feel more positive (more SPA) and less
negative (less SNA) at work (De Clercq, Bouckenooghe, Raja, & Matsyborska, 2014). As
SPA is positively related to job satisfaction, whereas SNA is negatively related to job
satisfaction (Thoresen et al., 2003), high EI persons can increase their job satisfaction by
regulating their emotions to experience more intense SPA and less SNA.
A considerable number of studies have examined EI and job satisfaction, and the ample
number of studies has allowed us to test for mediators and moderators. However, fewer
studies have been conducted on the relationships between EI and organizational
commitment and turnover intentions, sowecould not examinemediators andmoderators
for these outcome variables. We advance the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 4: SPA mediates the relationship between EI and job satisfaction.
Hypothesis 5: SNA mediates the relationship between EI and job satisfaction.
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 183
Goal setting theory and the mediating role of job performance
Prior meta-analytic evidence has confirmed a positive relationship between EI and job
performance (Joseph & Newman, 2010; O’Boyle et al., 2011). EI may increase job
performance because emotionally intelligent individuals are able to regulate their
emotions in order to experience positive emotions. Positive emotions widen employees’
behavioural repertoires, increase their behavioural flexibility, and boost their attentional
scope, thus resulting in increased job performance (Judge & Kammeyer-Muellar, 2008).
In line with goal setting theory, job satisfaction is an outcome of goal-directed
performance, because one’s progress towards goal accomplishment (i.e., goal-directed
performance) influences job satisfaction (Locke, 1969; Locke & Latham, 2002). Across
trials, the better one performs, the more goals one accomplishes and the higher job
satisfaction one has. Judge et al. (2001) pointed out that if effective job performance
supports the accomplishment of major goals in work, individuals should have higher job
satisfaction as a result.
Taken altogether, we propose that job performance should mediate the relationship
between EI and job satisfaction because EI enables one to attain one’s performance goals,
and obtaining goals increases job satisfaction (Locke, 1976). Consistent with self-
perception theory, employees observe their level of performance and perceive a
corresponding level of job satisfaction (Bem, 1967). Emotionally savvy individuals have a
better understanding of themselves, and this increases both their ability to set self-
motivating goals and their chances of achieving performance goals that lead to job
satisfaction (Kafetsios & Zampetakis, 2008; Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Spence, Oades, &
Caputi, 2004;Wong & Law, 2002). For example, emotionally intelligent individuals know
how to recognize their supervisors’ attitudes from emotional cues; moreover, they know
how to regulate their own emotion to act and communicate in ways that foster better
social relationshipswith their supervisors (Wong& Law, 2002),which in turn should lead
to higher performance appraisals and job satisfaction.
Emotionally intelligent individuals also regulate their emotions to deter the draining of
resources that cause burnout, to quickly bounce back from negative feelings, and to
maintain positive feelings so that they can preserve and replenish cognitive and emotional
resources. According to the job demands–resources model, conserving these cognitive
resources should enable employees tomore effectively accomplish theperformance goals
that lead to positive outcomes such as job satisfaction (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007;
Crawford, LePine, & Rich, 2010; Hobfoll, 2001). We suggest the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 6: Job performance mediates the relationship between EI and job satisfaction.
Moderator
Emotional labour. Emotional labour refers to ‘the management of feeling to create a
publicly observable facial and bodily display’ (Hochschild, 1983, p. 7). Jobs that involve
emotional labour include face-to-face or voice-to-voice contact with the public, produce
an emotional state in another person, and allow employers to exercise a degree of control
over the emotional activities of employees (Hochschild, 1983). Thus, emotional labour
requires the act of both displaying the appropriate emotion (i.e., conforming to a display
rule) and regulating both feelings and expressions to forward organizational goals
(Ashforth&Humphrey, 1993; Grandey, 2000).Meta-analysis has related emotional labour
to employee well-being, job satisfaction, and organizational attachment (Hulsheger &
Schewe, 2011).
184 Chao Miao et al.
It is likely that the association between EI and job satisfaction is conditioned by work
contexts (Côt�e, 2014). One such contextual variable is the emotional labour demand of
jobs. Prior findings indicate that EI should predict criteria more strongly in jobs that
involve high emotional labour, because these jobs require employees to regulate their
emotional expressions, and thus involve a high level of emotional regulation (Humphrey,
Ashforth, &Diefendorff, 2015; Johnson & Spector, 2007; Joseph&Newman, 2010;Wong
& Law, 2002). Ameta-analysis found that people high on EI aremore likely to use themost
effective form of emotional labour (Wang, Seibert, & Boles, 2011). The choice of
emotional labour demand as a contextual variable is consistent with trait activation
theory, which suggests that traits should more strongly predict outcomes when a context
has trait-relevant cues that activate the expression of traits (Tett & Burnett, 2003; Tett &
Guterman, 2000).
We have predicted that the relationship between EI and job satisfaction will be
strongerwhen a job requires high levels of emotional labour.Whena job requires frequent
customer/interpersonal interaction (i.e., high emotional labour demand), the expression
of EI should be activated because employees need to relymore on their EI to regulate their
emotions in order to prevent emotional and cognitive resources from being drained, and
to effectively maintain and enhance job satisfaction. Where there is infrequent customer/
interpersonal interactions (i.e., low emotional labour demand), the expression of EI may
be suppressed because this job does not demand the use of EI to handle interpersonal
interactions. Thus, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 7: Emotional labour demand moderates the relationship between EI and job
satisfaction such that the relationship becomes stronger when emotional labour
demand is high.
Method
Literature search
We applied several search techniques tomaximize the likelihood of capturing all relevant
studies. We set the range of dates starting from the earliest date of each database, journal,
and conference to year 2014. We used a list of keywords (and several variations in them)
for search, such as emotional intelligence, emotional competency, emotional ability, job
satisfaction, work satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover
intention.
First, we searched electronic databases, such as ABI/INFORM, EBSCO Host (e.g.,
Academic Search Complete and Business Source Complete), Google, Google Scholar,
JSTOR, ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, PsycNET (e.g., PsycInfo and PsycArticles), and
Social Science Citation Index. Second, major journals in psychology and management
were also searched, such as Academy of Management Journal, Administrative Science
Quarterly, Journal of Applied Psychology, Journal of Management, Journal of
Organizational Behavior, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology,
Organization Science, and Personnel Psychology. Third, we searched major management
conferences, such as the Academy of Management, the SouthernManagement Association,
and the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. We contacted scholars who
have published in the EI domain to ask for unpublished manuscripts, correlation matrices,
and rawdata, andwe completed our search inOctober 2014.Weused the English language
to search for relevant studies. Our search returned a few articles written in foreign
languages that had English titles and abstracts. Two authors of this paper are bilingual and
were able to read some of these articles. Our search yielded 1,036 articles.
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 185
Inclusion criteria
A study was deemed eligible for being included in the present meta-analysis if it met the
following criteria. First, primary studies had to be empirical and quantitative. All
qualitative studies were excluded. Second, primary studies had to report the correlation
coefficients for the relationships between EI and job satisfaction, between EI and
organizational commitment, between EI and turnover intentions, and/or between EI and
state affect. When no such information existed in primary studies, sufficient statistics
needed to be provided in such studies to allow the conversion into effect sizes (we
employed Lipsey andWilson’s (2001) as well as Peterson and Brown’s (2005) methods to
perform effect size conversions). Third, primary studies had to use real employee samples
in their research design. Studies based on non-employee samples (e.g., student samples)
were eliminated from our meta-analysis. Fourth, a study had to use scales designed to
measure EI. Studies that used proxy measures of EI (e.g., self-monitoring scales) were not
eligible. When the above criteria were applied to screen the articles, it resulted in 119
studies. A list of tables describing the included studies and a list of references of the studies
included in the present meta-analytic review were uploaded as online supplemental
materials (see Tables S3–S10 in supplemental materials).
Coding procedures
We coded different categories of EI (i.e., ability EI, self-report EI, and mixed EI) based on
Ashkanasy and Daus (2005) and O’Boyle et al. (2011). We coded emotional labour
demands according to the methods developed by Joseph and Newman (2010). The
occupations where there are frequent customer/interpersonal interactions that require
emotion regulation were coded as high emotional labour demand jobs. The occupations
where there are infrequent interpersonal/customer interactions that demand less
emotion regulation were coded as low emotional labour demand jobs. Joseph and
Newman (2010) categorized 191 jobs into high versus low emotional labour demand and
we used their categorization to code the emotional labour demand of the studies that we
found.We adhered to the coding rules developed by Thoresen et al. (2003) to code state-
based affect (i.e., SPA and SNA). The studies where respondents were asked to rate their
experiences of positive affect and negative affect over the previous week (or less) were
coded as state affect. As argued by Thoresen et al. (2003), this one-week rule was in line
with Watson’s (2000) definition of state affect.
Two coders participated in coding and independently coded each sample. The initial
coding agreement was 95%. Coding discrepancies were resolved through discussion.
Another author of this paper was invited to join the discussion to solve any remaining
coding disagreement when two coders could not reach consensus after discussion. All
codingdisagreementwashandled and resolved, and a100%consensuswasfinally achieved.
Meta-analytic procedures
We performed psychometric meta-analysis by using the procedures developed by Hunter
and Schmidt (2004) to synthesize collected data. Statistical artefacts can have systematic
downward bias effects on effect sizes, and one source of statistical artefacts ismeasurement
error (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). We thereby corrected for measurement errors in both
independent and dependent variables for each individual correlation. We noted that some
primary studies did not report the reliability. Thus, we imputed the missing reliability for
both independent and dependent variables by using the mean of reliabilities of the studies
186 Chao Miao et al.
that reported reliability information (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004; also see supplements for
more details). We presented corrected sample-size-weighted mean correlation (q̂) as the
estimate of populationmean correlation.Wecalculated corrected 95%confidence intervals
to determine the statistical significance of effect sizes. Effect sizes are considered to be
statistically significant when corrected 95% confidence intervals do not include zero. We
performedmoderator analyses by usingHunter and Schmidt’s (1990) approach (i.e., z-test).
This test allows the examination of the statistical significance of between-group effect size
difference.We computed Varart%, 80% credibility intervals, andQ statistic to determine the
potential existence of moderators. Varart% denotes the percentage of the variance in q̂
explained by statistical artefacts. Hunter and Schmidt (2004) suggested that moderators
may exist if statistical artefacts explain less than 75% of the variance in the meta-analytic
correlations. We also reported corrected 80% credibility intervals to explore the potential
existence of moderators because Whitener (1990) recommended that a wide 80
%
credibility interval indicates the possible existence of moderators. In addition, a statistically
significant Q statistic suggests that heterogeneity exists in effect size distribution (i.e., the
potential existence of moderators).
We createdmeta-analytically derived corrected correlationmatrices (see Tables S11(a)
to S12 in supplementalmaterials) and performed hierarchicalmultiple regression, relative
weight analyses, and meta-analytic structural equation modelling (Johnson, 2000;
Johnson & LeBreton, 2004; Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). Along with all effect sizes
derived from the present study, we also used corrected effect sizes from prior meta-
analytic reviews to complete the input correlation matrices for these three analyses. We
computed harmonic mean sample size (Viswesvaran &Ones, 1995) because sample sizes
differed across the cells in the correlationmatrices. Harmonicmean sample size produces
more conservative estimates because lessweight is given to large samples (Colquitt, Scott,
& LePine, 2007).
Results
Main and moderator effects
Because of limited sample sizes for ability EImeasures,wewere not able to examine ability
EI’s relationships with either turnover intentions or organizational commitment.
Likewise, there were not enough studies to allow us to perform meta-analysis on the
mixed EI–turnover intentions relationship. In the following sections, we will provide the
results for EImeasures onlywhen the number of studies and sample sizes are large enough
to justify them. Table 1 contains the results of the relationships between EI and job
satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intentions based on psychometric
meta-analysis. The relationship between ability EI and job satisfaction (k = 13,N = 1,927)
was positive and statistically significant (q̂ = .08) because the corrected 95% confidence
interval spanned from .01 to .15 and did not include zero. The effect sizes for the
relationships between the other two types of EI and job satisfaction (q̂ = .32 for self-report
EI and q̂ = .39 for mixed EI) show similar patterns of results. We repeated the same
procedures and found that self-report EI positively relates to organizational commitment
(q̂ = .43) and negatively relates to turnover intentions (q̂ = �.33). In addition, mixed EI
positively relates to organizational commitment (q̂ = .43). As such, Hypothesis 1, which
proposed that EI should positively relate to job satisfaction and organizational commit-
ment and negatively relate to turnover intentions, is supported.
We observed that there were substantial variations across effect sizes for three major
distributions for the relationship betweenEI and job satisfaction, because far less than75%
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 187
T
a
b
le
1
.
P
sy
ch
o
m
e
tr
ic
m
e
ta
-a
n
al
ys
is
re
su
lt
s
k
N
� r o
SD
r
q̂
SD
q
V
ar
a
rt
%
C
o
rr
e
ct
e
d
9
5
%
C
I
C
o
rr
e
ct
e
d
8
0
%
C
R
Q
Si
gn
ifi
ca
n
t
d
iff
e
re
n
ce
x
.
A
b
il
it
y
E
I
–
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
a
c
ti
o
n
1
3
1
,9
2
7
.0
7
.1
2
.0
8
.
1
0
4
9
.0
1
to
.1
5
�.
0
5
to
.
2
0
2
7
.9
0
**
y
,
z
E
m
o
ti
o
n
al
la
b
o
u
r
a.
H
ig
h
5
6
0
9
.1
0
.1
6
.1
3
.1
5
3
2
.0
6
to
.2
0
�.
0
7
to
.
3
3
1
7
.0
8
**
–
b
.
L
o
w
4
4
9
6
.0
7
.1
2
.0
9
.1
0
5
3
.0
3
to
.
1
4
�.
0
4
to
.2
1
7
.6
4
†
–
A
b
ili
ty
E
I
–
st
at
e
p
o
si
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
2
3
7
3
�.
0
6
.
1
1
�.
0
7
.0
9
4
5
�.
1
8
to
.0
5
�.
1
9
to
.0
5
4
.3
9
*
A
b
ili
ty
E
I
–
st
at
e
n
e
ga
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
2
3
7
3
�.
3
2
.0
6
�.
3
9
.0
0
1
0
0
�.
4
2
to
�.
3
5
�.
3
9
to
�.
3
9
1
.6
3
y
.
S
e
lf
-r
e
p
o
rt
E
I
–
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
a
c
ti
o
n
6
6
2
0
,1
1
6
.2
8
.1
1
.3
2
.1
1
2
4
.
2
9
to
.3
5
.1
8
to
.4
7
3
5
0
.6
2
**
*
x
,
z
†
E
m
o
ti
o
n
al
la
b
o
u
r
a.
H
ig
h
3
4
1
1
,5
1
6
.3
1
.0
9
.3
5
.0
8
3
3
.3
2
to
.3
9
.2
5
to
.4
6
1
0
8
.8
1
**
*
b
b
.
L
o
w
1
0
2
,4
9
7
.1
7
.1
3
.2
1
.1
4
2
4
.1
6
to
.
2
6
.0
4
to
.3
8
4
8
.2
5
**
*
a
S
e
lf
-r
e
p
o
rt
E
I
–
o
rg
a
n
iz
a
ti
o
n
a
l
c
o
m
m
it
m
e
n
t
3
0
7
,6
7
5
.3
6
.1
7
.4
3
.1
7
1
3
.3
8
to
.4
8
.2
2
to
.6
4
2
5
9
.5
3
**
*
S
e
lf
-r
e
p
o
rt
E
I
–
tu
rn
o
v
e
r
in
te
n
ti
o
n
s
1
7
5
,0
0
4
�.
2
8
.2
1
�.
3
3
.2
5
6
�.
4
0
to
�.
2
5
�.
6
4
to
�.
0
1
3
2
3
.7
7
**
*
Se
lf-
re
p
o
rt
E
I
–
st
at
e
p
o
si
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
3
8
8
9
.4
2
.1
1
.4
7
.1
1
2
0
.4
0
to
.5
4
.3
3
to
.6
0
1
5
.7
9
**
*
Se
lf-
re
p
o
rt
E
I
–
st
at
e
n
e
ga
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
3
8
8
9
�.
3
6
.1
7
�.
4
2
.1
8
1
0
�.
5
1
to
�.
3
2
�.
6
4
to
�.
1
9
4
2
.5
8
**
*
z
.
M
ix
e
d
E
I
–
Jo
b
S
a
ti
sf
a
c
ti
o
n
4
1
7
,0
7
6
.3
3
.2
3
.3
9
.2
7
8
.3
1
to
.4
8
.0
5
to
.7
3
1
3
5
1
.2
5
**
*
x
,
y
†
E
m
o
ti
o
n
al
la
b
o
u
r
a.
H
ig
h
2
0
3
,7
2
7
.3
3
.1
2
.3
9
.1
2
2
9
.3
4
to
.4
4
.2
3
to
.5
5
7
7
.6
6
**
*
–
b
.
L
o
w
1
4
2
,2
6
4
.3
9
.3
2
.4
6
.3
7
4
.3
3
to
.5
8
�.
0
2
to
.9
3
9
3
3
.9
6
**
*
–
M
ix
e
d
E
I
–
o
rg
a
n
iz
a
ti
o
n
a
l
c
o
m
m
it
m
e
n
t
2
6
3
,8
6
7
.3
6
.2
2
.4
3
.2
4
1
1
.3
5
to
.5
1
.1
2
to
.7
4
5
0
0
.4
1
**
*
M
ix
e
d
E
I
–
st
at
e
p
o
si
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
1
4
7
5
.2
3
.0
0
.3
1
.0
0
N
A
.3
1
to
.3
1
.3
1
to
.3
1
N
A
M
ix
e
d
E
I
–
st
at
e
n
e
ga
ti
ve
af
fe
ct
1
4
7
5
�.
1
8
.0
0
�.
2
4
.0
0
N
A
�.
2
4
to
�.
2
4
�.
2
4
to
�.
2
4
N
A
N
ot
es
.
K
,
n
u
m
b
er
o
f
in
d
ep
e
n
d
en
t
sa
m
p
le
s;
N
,
sa
m
p
le
si
ze
;
� r o
,
u
n
co
rr
e
ct
ed
sa
m
p
le
-s
iz
e-
w
ei
gh
te
d
m
ea
n
co
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
;
SD
r,
sa
m
p
le
-s
iz
e-
w
ei
gh
te
d
st
an
d
ar
d
d
ev
ia
ti
o
n
o
f
o
b
se
rv
ed
m
ea
n
co
rr
el
at
io
n
s;
q̂,
co
rr
ec
te
d
sa
m
p
le
-s
iz
e-
w
e
ig
h
te
d
m
ea
n
co
rr
el
at
io
n
;
SD
q
,
sa
m
p
le
-s
iz
e-
w
ei
gh
te
d
st
an
d
ar
d
d
e
vi
at
io
n
o
f
co
rr
ec
te
d
m
e
an
co
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
s;
V
ar
ar
t%
,p
er
ce
n
ta
ge
o
fv
ar
ia
n
ce
in
q̂
ex
p
la
in
e
d
b
y
st
at
is
ti
ca
la
rt
ef
ac
ts
;c
o
rr
e
ct
e
d
9
5
%
C
I,
co
rr
ec
te
d
9
5
%
co
n
fi
d
e
n
ce
in
te
rv
al
;c
o
rr
ec
te
d
8
0
%
C
R
,c
o
rr
e
ct
e
d
8
0
%
cr
ed
ib
ili
ty
in
te
rv
al
;Q
,a
st
at
is
ti
c
u
se
d
to
as
se
ss
th
e
h
et
er
o
ge
n
ei
ty
in
ef
fe
ct
si
ze
s
ac
ro
ss
st
u
d
ie
s;
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
t
d
iff
er
en
ce
=
le
tt
er
s
in
th
is
co
lu
m
n
co
rr
es
p
o
n
d
to
th
e
le
tt
er
s
in
ro
w
s
an
d
in
d
ic
at
e
th
at
ef
fe
ct
si
ze
s
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
tl
y
d
iff
er
fr
o
m
o
n
e
an
o
th
er
at
.0
5
le
ve
l.
T
h
e
le
tt
e
rs
w
it
h
Ԡ
’d
en
o
te
th
e
st
at
is
ti
ca
ls
ig
n
ifi
ca
n
ce
at
.1
0
le
ve
l.
T
h
e
si
gn
‘–
’s
u
gg
es
ts
th
er
e
is
n
o
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
t
b
et
w
e
en
-g
ro
u
p
d
iff
er
en
ce
.z
-t
es
t
is
u
se
d
to
ev
al
u
at
e
th
e
st
at
is
ti
ca
ls
ig
n
ifi
ca
n
ce
o
fb
et
w
ee
n
-g
ro
u
p
d
iff
er
en
ce
in
ef
fe
ct
si
ze
s.
E
I,
e
m
o
ti
o
n
al
in
te
lli
ge
n
ce
.F
o
r
cl
ar
it
y
o
f
re
p
o
rt
in
g,
th
e
m
et
a-
an
al
yt
ic
d
is
tr
ib
u
ti
o
n
s
th
at
re
fe
r
to
th
e
m
ai
n
ef
fe
ct
o
f
ea
ch
st
re
am
o
f
E
I
o
n
ea
ch
w
o
rk
at
ti
tu
d
e
w
er
e
in
d
ic
at
ed
in
b
o
ld
ch
ar
ac
te
rs
.
†
p
<
.1
0
;*
p
<
.0
5
;*
*p
< .0 1 ;* ** p < .0 0 1 .
188 Chao Miao et al.
of the variance in q̂ (Varart%) was explained by statistical artefacts. This met Hunter and
Schmidt’s (2004) 75% rule for indicating the potential existence ofmoderators.Q statistics
were significant as well, which further confirmed our conclusion that effect size
distributions were heterogeneous for all three types of EI. Therefore, performing further
moderator analyses was justified.
The results of the effect size differences among different types of EI are shown in the
last column of Table 1. This column also displays the results of all other moderator
analyses as well. We performed z-tests to determine the statistical significance of the
between-group differences. Our results indicate that ability EI has the lowest relationship
with job satisfaction compared to the other two types of EI (self-report EI vs. ability EI,
Dq̂ = .24, p < .05; mixed EI vs. ability EI, Dq̂ = .31, p < .05). The relationship between mixed EI and job satisfaction is marginally significantly larger than the relationship between self-report EI and job satisfaction (Dq̂ = .07, p < .1). We therefore concluded that Hypothesis 3 is supported (see Table 2).
Emotional labour was a significant moderator only for the self-report EI–job
satisfaction relationship. Thus, there was mixed support for Hypothesis 7.
Incremental validity, relative weight analyses, and meta-analytic structural
equation modelling
Incremental validity analysis
Table 3 displays the results of incremental validity analysis based on the hierarchical
multiple regression analysis.When thedependent variable is job satisfaction, the firstmodel
demonstrates that cognitive ability and the FFM in combination account for 15% (p < .001)
of the variance in job satisfaction. The second, third, and fourth models illustrate the
incremental validity of each type of EI in the presence of cognitive ability and the FFM. The
second model shows that ability EI contributes no incremental validity (p = ns) in the
Table 2. Summary of results for all hypotheses
Hypotheses Results
Hypothesis 1: EI should positively relate to job satisfaction and
organizational commitment, and EI should negatively relate to
turnover intentions
Supported
Hypothesis 2: EI should contribute incremental validity and relative
importance in the presence of the FFM and cognitive ability
when predicting job satisfaction, organizational commitment,
and turnover intentions
Supported
for self-report EI
and mixed EI, but not for
ability EI
Hypothesis 3: Mixed EI will show the strongest relationship with
job satisfaction, self-report EI the next strongest, and ability EI
the weakest relationship with job satisfaction
Supported
Hypothesis 4: SPA mediates the relationship between EI and
job satisfaction
Supported
Hypothesis 5: SNA mediates the relationship between EI and
job satisfaction
Supported
Hypothesis 6: Job performance mediates the relationship between
EI and job satisfaction
Supported
Hypothesis 7: Emotional labour demand moderates the relationship
between EI and job satisfaction such that the relationship becomes
stronger when emotional labour demand is high
Supported only
for self-report EI
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 189
presence of cognitive ability and the FFM. On the other hand, the third and the fourth
models show that both self-report EI and mixed EI contribute an additional 3% (p < .001) and6%(p < .001) of variance, respectively, aboveandbeyondcognitive ability and theFFM.
When the dependent variable is organizational commitment, self-report EI and mixed
EI contribute an additional 9% (p < .001) and 8% (p < .001) of variance, respectively,
above and beyond cognitive ability and the FFM.When the dependent variable is turnover
intentions, self-report EI contributes an additional 8% (p < .001) of variance above and beyond cognitive ability and the FFM.
Relative weight analysis
Because the predictors in our regression model are correlated, we performed relative
weight analysis to determine the relative importance of each predictor in predicting
employee job satisfaction. Table 3 displays the results of relative weight analysis for all
three types of EI in the last two columns of eachmodel. Ability EI only contributed 1.3% of
the explained variance, alongwith aR2 contribution of .00 inModel 2. It failed tomeet our
threshold for a small effect (see the section of supplemental notes in supplemental
materials for details about how we determined the criteria of small, medium, and large
effects). Further, ability EI demonstrated the least relative importance compared to all
other predictors in Model 2.
Unlike ability EI, self-report EI andmixed EI all demonstrated relative importance in the
presence of the FFM and cognitive ability. Self-report EI is the most dominant predictor in
Model 3, capturing 31.3% of the explained variance along with an R2 contribution of .06.
The second most dominant predictor in Model 3 was extraversion (RW% = 23.2;
R2 = .04), and the least dominant predictor was cognitive ability (RW% = 1.4; R2 = .00).
Mixed EI is themost dominant predictor relative to the FFM and cognitive ability in Model
4, contributing 42.8% of the explained variance as well as a R2 contribution of .09. The
secondmost dominant predictor was extraversion (RW% = 17.2; R2 = .04), and the least
dominant predictor was cognitive ability (RW% = 1.5; R2 = .00). Mixed EI hadmore than
twice the relative importance of the second most dominant predictor (i.e., extraversion).
With regard to organizational commitment, self-report EI and mixed EI demonstrated
impressive relative importance of 46.9% (R2 = .12) and 44.2% (R2 = .11), respectively.
When the dependent variablewas turnover intentions, self-report EI showed large relative
importance of 60.9% (R2 = .09). Because of the large effects for both self-report andmixed
EI, we hold that Hypothesis 2 is supported, but note that there is scale-based moderation
with regard to ability measures.
Meta-analytic structural equation modelling
We performed meta-analytic structural equation modelling to test the hypotheses related
tomediation.Mediationwould exist if the testwere to showa significant indirect path.We
separated mixed EI from both ability EI and self-report EI when performing mediation
testing, because mixed EI has moderate and high multicollinearity with ability EI and self-
report EI, respectively. The presence of multicollinearity would inflate standard errors,
reduce statistical power, cause the issues of bouncing betas, and produce uninterpretable
results (Schwab, 2005). We still kept ability EI and self-report EI together when testing
mediation, because the correlation between ability EI and self-report EI was just .12,
which did not cause multicollinearity issues.
190 Chao Miao et al.
T
a
b
le
3
.
H
ie
ra
rc
h
ic
al
m
u
lt
ip
le
re
gr
e
ss
io
n
an
d
re
la
ti
ve
w
e
ig
h
t
an
al
ys
e
s
fo
r
al
l
th
re
e
st
re
am
s
o
f
E
I
in
p
re
d
ic
ti
n
g
JS
,O
C
,a
n
d
T
I
D
V
=
JS
M
o
d
e
l1
M
o
d
e
l2
M
o
d
e
l3
M
o
d
e
l4
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
C
o
gn
it
iv
e
ab
ili
ty
.0
7
**
*
.0
0
3
2
.0
.0
6
**
*
.0
0
3
1
.7
.0
6
**
*
.0
0
3
1
.4
.0
7
**
*
.0
0
3
1
.5
N
e
u
ro
ti
ci
sm
�.
1
8
**
*
.0
4
6
3
0
.3
�.
1
8
**
*
.0
4
6
3
0
.0
�.
1
4
**
*
.0
3
6
1
9
.7
�.
0
7
**
*
.0
3
1
1
4
.8
E
x
tr
av
e
rs
io
n
.2
5
**
*
.0
5
1
3
3
.5
.2
5
**
*
.0
5
1
3
3
.3
.2
2
**
*
.0
4
3
2
3
.2
.1
6
**
*
.0
3
6
1
7
.2
O
p
e
n
n
e
ss
�.
1
6
**
*
.0
0
7
4
.8
�.
1
6
**
*
.0
0
7
4
.9
�.
1
9
**
*
.0
1
1
5
.7
�.
2
1
**
*
.0
1
4
6
.6
A
gr
e
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
.0
0
.0
0
9
5
.9
�.
0
0
.0
0
8
5
.6
�.
0
0
.0
0
8
4
.1
�.
0
3
*
.0
0
7
3
.4
C
o
n
sc
ie
n
ti
o
u
sn
e
ss
.1
4
**
*
.0
3
6
2
3
.4
.1
4
**
*
.0
3
5
2
3
.2
.0
9
**
*
.0
2
7
1
4
.6
.1
1
**
*
.0
2
8
1
3
.7
A
b
ili
ty
E
I
.0
2
.0
0
2
1
.3
Se
lf-
re
p
o
rt
E
I
.2
1
**
*
.0
5
8
3
1
.3
M
ix
e
d
E
I
.3
2
**
*
.0
8
9
4
2
.8
R
2
.1
5
**
*
.1
5
**
*
.1
8
**
*
.2
1
**
*
D
R
2
.0
0
.0
3
**
*
.0
6
**
*
H
ar
m
o
n
ic
m
e
an
N
6
,6
8
1
5
,5
8
9
6
,0
1
1
6
,5
4
1
D
V
=
O
C
M
o
d
e
l1
M
o
d
e
l2
M
o
d
e
l3
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
C
o
gn
it
iv
e
ab
ili
ty
�.
1
5
**
*
.0
2
0
1
1
.7
�.
1
6
**
*
.0
2
2
8
.5
�.
1
4
**
*
.0
2
0
8
.1
N
e
u
ro
ti
ci
sm
.0
1
.0
1
1
6
.4
.0
8
**
*
.0
0
9
3
.3
.1
4
**
*
.0
1
1
4
.7
E
x
tr
av
e
rs
io
n
.1
8
**
*
.0
4
6
2
6
.8
.1
3
**
*
.0
3
5
1
3
.7
.0
7
**
*
.0
3
1
1
2
.6
O
p
e
n
n
e
ss
.1
0
**
*
.0
1
8
1
0
.7
.0
4
**
.0
1
3
5
.0
.0
3
*
.0
1
3
5
.3
A
gr
e
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
.1
0
**
*
.0
2
5
1
4
.8
.0
9
**
*
.0
2
2
8
.5
.0
6
**
*
.0
2
0
8
.2
C
o
n
sc
ie
n
ti
o
u
sn
e
ss
.2
1
**
*
.0
5
0
2
9
.6
.1
3
**
*
.0
3
6
1
4
.1
.1
7
**
*
.0
4
2
1
6
.9
Se
lf-
re
p
o
rt
E
I
.3
5
**
*
.1
2
1
4
6
.9
M
ix
e
d
E
I
.3
8
**
*
.1
0
8
4
4
.2
R
2
.1
7
**
*
.2
6
**
*
.2
5
**
*
D
R
2
.0
9
**
*
.0
8
**
*
H
ar
m
o
n
ic
m
e
an
N
4
,0
2
2
4
,1
0
7
4
,3
2
3
C
on
tin
ue
d
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 191
T
a
b
le
3
.
(C
on
tin
ue
d)
D
V
=
T
I
M
o
d
e
l1
M
o
d
e
l2
b
R
W
R
W
%
b
R
W
R
W
%
C
o
gn
it
iv
e
ab
ili
ty
.0
6
**
*
.0
0
4
5
.9
.0
7
**
*
.0
0
5
3
.4
N
e
u
ro
ti
ci
sm
.2
0
**
*
.0
3
7
5
6
.6
.1
4
**
*
.0
2
7
1
9
.4
E
x
tr
av
e
rs
io
n
�.
0
6
**
.0
0
5
7
.3
�.
0
1
.0
0
4
2
.6
O
p
e
n
n
e
ss
.0
7
**
*
.0
0
2
3
.4
.1
2
**
*
.0
0
6
4
.5
A
gr
e
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
�.
0
3
.0
0
7
1
0
.1
�.
0
2
.0
0
6
3
.9
C
o
n
sc
ie
n
ti
o
u
sn
e
ss
�.
0
4
†
.0
1
1
1
6
.7
.0
4
†
.0
0
7
5
.2
Se
lf-
re
p
o
rt
E
I
�.
3
2
**
*
.0
8
6
6
0
.9
R
2
.0
7
**
*
.1
4
**
*
D
R
2
.0
8
**
*
H
ar
m
o
n
ic
m
e
an
N
3
,6
3
3
3
,7
6
1
N
ot
es
.
b,
st
an
d
ar
d
iz
e
d
re
gr
e
ss
io
n
w
e
ig
h
ts
;
D
V
,
d
e
p
e
n
d
e
n
t
va
ri
ab
le
;
E
I,
e
m
o
ti
o
n
al
in
te
lli
ge
n
ce
;
JS
,
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
;
O
C
,
o
rg
an
iz
at
io
n
al
co
m
m
it
m
e
n
t;
T
I,
tu
rn
o
ve
r
in
te
n
ti
o
n
s;
h
ar
m
o
n
ic
m
e
an
N
,h
ar
m
o
n
ic
m
e
an
sa
m
p
le
si
ze
;R
W
,r
e
la
ti
ve
w
e
ig
h
t;
R
W
%
,p
e
rc
e
n
ta
ge
o
fr
e
la
ti
ve
w
e
ig
h
t
(c
o
m
p
u
te
d
b
y
d
iv
id
in
g
in
d
iv
id
u
al
re
la
ti
ve
w
e
ig
h
t
b
y
th
e
su
m
o
fi
n
d
iv
id
u
al
re
la
ti
ve
w
e
ig
h
t
an
d
m
u
lt
ip
ly
in
g
b
y
1
0
0
);
R
2
,m
u
lt
ip
le
co
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
s;
D
R
2
,i
n
cr
e
m
e
n
ta
lc
h
an
ge
in
R
2
.I
t
is
n
o
te
d
fr
o
m
T
ab
le
S1
1
(a
)
th
at
at
b
iv
ar
ia
te
le
ve
l,
o
p
e
n
n
e
ss
is
n
o
t
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
tl
y
re
la
te
d
to
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
(q̂
=
.0
2
)
an
d
ag
re
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
is
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
tl
y
an
d
p
o
si
ti
ve
ly
re
la
te
d
to
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
(q̂
=
.1
7
).
H
o
w
e
ve
r,
as
d
e
m
o
n
st
ra
te
d
in
T
ab
le
3
,M
o
d
e
l1
sh
o
w
s
th
at
o
p
e
n
n
e
ss
is
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
tl
y
an
d
n
e
ga
ti
ve
ly
re
la
te
d
to
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
(b
=
�.
1
6
),
w
h
e
re
as
ag
re
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
is
n
o
t
si
gn
ifi
ca
n
tl
y
re
la
te
d
to
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
(b
=
.0
0
).
O
u
r
re
su
lt
s
ar
e
co
n
si
st
e
n
t
w
it
h
Ju
d
ge
et
al
.’s
(2
0
0
2
)
m
e
ta
-a
n
al
yt
ic
fi
n
d
in
gs
,
w
h
ic
h
al
so
sh
o
w
e
d
so
m
e
in
co
n
si
st
e
n
ci
e
s
b
e
tw
e
e
n
b
iv
ar
ia
te
an
d
m
u
lt
iv
ar
ia
te
re
su
lt
s
o
fo
p
e
n
n
e
ss
an
d
ag
re
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
.A
cc
o
rd
in
g
to
Ju
d
ge
et
al
.(
2
0
0
2
),
o
p
e
n
n
e
ss
is
d
e
sc
ri
b
e
d
as
a
‘d
o
u
b
le
-e
d
ge
d
sw
o
rd
’t
h
at
m
ay
p
ro
m
p
t
in
d
iv
id
u
al
s
to
se
n
se
b
o
th
th
e
go
o
d
an
d
th
e
b
ad
m
o
re
d
e
e
p
ly
.
A
lt
h
o
u
gh
ag
re
e
ab
le
in
d
iv
id
u
al
s
sh
o
u
ld
e
x
p
e
ri
e
n
ce
h
ig
h
e
r
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
d
u
e
to
th
e
ir
m
o
ti
va
ti
o
n
to
ac
h
ie
ve
in
te
rp
e
rs
o
n
al
in
ti
m
ac
y,
it
m
ay
o
n
ly
b
e
a
gr
e
at
p
re
d
ic
to
r
o
f
jo
b
sa
ti
sf
ac
ti
o
n
in
so
ci
al
o
cc
u
p
at
io
n
s
w
h
e
re
tr
ai
t
ag
re
e
ab
le
n
e
ss
is
m
o
re
re
le
va
n
t.
†
p
<
.1
0
;*
p
<
.0
5
;*
*p
<
.0
1
;*
**
p
<
.0
0
1
.
192 Chao Miao et al.
We used meta-analytic structural equation modelling to compare a list of alternative
models (see Table S14 in supplemental materials). For Test 1 in Table S14, we assessed
how state affect and job performance mediate the relationships between ability EI, self-
report EI, and job satisfaction. We compared all the other models with Model 1 – a partial
mediationmodelwith direct paths fromboth ability EI and self-report EI to job satisfaction.
v2 difference test showed that the differences between all three alternative models and
Model 1 are consistently not statistically significant, meaning that making themodel more
parsimonious does not worsen model fit. We chose Model 4 (full mediation model)
because it is themost parsimonious one among all fourmodels and it also fits the data very
well, v2(2) = 3.53 (p = .17), CFI = 1.00, NFI = 1.00, GFI = 1.00, SRMR = .01.
We applied the same method for Test 2 in Table S14, where we assessed how SPA,
SNA, and job performance mediate the relationship between mixed EI and job
satisfaction. Although v2 difference test showed that partial mediation model (Model 1)
demonstrates better model fit than full mediation model (Model 2; Dv2[1] = 88.26), we
still decided to choose full mediation model (Model 2) due to three reasons. First,
sample size greatly influences the v2 difference and our meta-analytic sample size was
large (Kline, 2011). Therefore, even a negligible difference between models may still
have produced a statistically significant v2 statistic in the present study (Berry,
Lelchook, & Clark, 2012). Second, partial mediation model (Model 1) is a saturated
model and we cannot derive any conclusion from this model. As such, Model 2, a non-
saturated model, is more preferable relative to Model 1. Third, full mediation (Model 2)
not only displays acceptable model fit (CFI = .92, NFI = .92, GFI = .98, SRMR = .05),
but is also more parsimonious than Model 1. Hence, we opted to choose Model 2 in
Test 2 due to the aforementioned reasons. Both chosen models based on the results of
model comparison were indicated with bold characters in Table S14.
Figure 1 presents the results of the examination of mediation, along with all
standardized path coefficients for all chosen models. Figure 1a corresponds to Model 4
under Test 1 in Table S14. Figure 1b corresponds to Model 2 under Test 2 in Table S14.
With regard to Figure 1a, we assessed how SPA, SNA, and job performance mediated
the relationship between ability EI and self-report EI and job satisfaction. We performed
three sets of mediation tests – Sobel test, Aroian test, and Goodman test. For instance, the
indirect paths from self-report EI to job satisfaction through SPA (b = .15) and SNA
(b = .08) were statistically significant. Similarly, the indirect effect from self-report EI to
job satisfaction through job performance (b = .04)was statistically significant aswell.We
repeated the same procedures for all the other models in Figure 1. We found that all
indirect paths were statistically significant. As such, all mediation hypotheses (Hypothe-
ses 4–6; see Table 2 for specific hypotheses) are supported. The results of mediation
examination are shown directly below each figure.
Publication bias analyses
Weperformed three different types of publication bias analyses and found no evidence of
publication bias inflating reported effect sizes (see supplemental materials for details).
Discussion
Emotion is an integral part of organizational life and is often functional for the
organization, and the proper management of emotions can lead to increased job
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 193
satisfaction (Ashforth &Humphrey, 1995).We presented the first meta-analytic review of
the relationship between employee EI and employee job satisfaction and found a positive
and significant relationship between all three types of EI and job satisfaction. In addition,
EI is also positively related to organizational commitment and negatively related to
turnover intentions. Thus, emotionally savvy individuals are not only high-performing
(O’Boyle et al., 2011) but are also more satisfied with their jobs.
Theoretical implications
Although the relationship between EI and job satisfaction is positive and statistically
significant, the variation in effect sizes across studies is substantial (according to Hunter
and Schmidt’s 75% rule and Q statistic) for the relationships between all three types of EI
and job satisfaction. We found that the relationship between self-report EI and job
satisfaction is higher when emotional labour demand is high. This coincides with Joseph
and Newman’s (2010) findings, suggesting that when a job involves frequent customer/
interpersonal interaction (i.e., high emotional labour demand) it requires employees to
use their EI to regulate their emotions. However, emotional labour was not a moderator
for either ability EI or mixed EI. This may be because recent research suggests that
emotional labour is used in a wide variety of jobs (Humphrey et al., 2015). These mixed
findings warrant more research on the EI–emotional labour relationship.
The pattern of results upholds the categorization of EI measures into three streams/
types (Ashkanasy & Daus, 2005; O’Boyle et al., 2011). Due to differential relationships
with cognitive ability and personality, we found that mixed EI has the highest association
with employee job satisfaction (q̂ = .39), self-report EI the next highest (q̂ = .32), and
ability EI the lowest relationship with employee job satisfaction (q̂ = .08). These results
are consistent with our expectation because ability EI is more cognitively loaded and thus
should have the lowest relationship with job satisfaction, because cognitive ability is a
AEI: Mediation effect of SPA –.128[.026] × .311[.029] = –.04***
AEI: Sobel Test: –4.47; Aroian test = –4.46; Goodman test = –4.49
AEI: Mediation effect of SNA –.345[.025] × –.222[.028] = .08 ***
AEI: Sobel Test: 6.87; Aroian test = 6.86; Goodman test = 6.89
AEI: Mediation effect of JP .207[.028] × .155[.027] = .03***
AEI: Sobel Test: 4.53; Aroian test = 4.51; Goodman test = 4.56
SEI: Mediation effect of SPA .485[.026] × .311[.029] = .15***
SEI: Sobel Test: 9.30; Aroian test = 9.29; Goodman test = 9.31
SEI: Mediation effect of SNA –.379[.025] × –.222 [.028] = .08***
SEI: Sobel Test: 7.03; Aroian test = 7.01; Goodman test = 7.04
SEI: Mediation effect of JP .275[.028] × .155[.027] = .04***
SEI: Sobel Test: 4.96; Aroian test = 4.94; Goodman test = 4.98
Mediation effect of SPA .310[.026] × .311[.026] = .10***
Sobel Test: 8.44; Aroian test = 8.43; Goodman test = 8.46
Mediation effect of SNA –.240[.026] × –.222[.025] = .05***
Sobel Test: 6.40; Aroian test = 6.38; Goodman test = 6.42
Mediation effect of JP .280[.026] × .155[.025] = .04***
Sobel Test: 5.37; Aroian test = 5.36; Goodman test = 5.39
AEI
SEI
SPA
SNA JS
MEI
SPA
SNA JS
–.13***
JP
JP
–.34***
.21***
.49***
–.38***
.28***
.31***
–.22***
.16***
.31***
–.24***
.28***
.31***
–.22***
.16***
(a) (b)
Figure 1. Path models of the mediating roles of state affect and job performance in the relationship
between emotional intelligence (EI) and job satisfaction. Note. Standardized path coefficients are
reported. Standard errors are reported in brackets. AEI, ability EI; SEI, self-report EI; MEI, mixed EI; JS, job
satisfaction; SPA, state positive affect; SNA, state negative affect; JP, job performance. (a) Model 4 under
Test 1 in Table S14. (b) Model 2 under Test 2 in Table S14. Fit indices for each model are reported in
Table S14. We omitted covariance for clarity of reporting. ***p < .001.
194 Chao Miao et al.
weak predictor of job satisfaction (Gonzalez-Mul�e et al., 2014). Mixed EI has the largest
relationship with other personality traits and should thus have the strongest relationship
with job satisfaction because personality is amuch better predictor of job satisfaction than
cognitive ability (Judge et al., 2002).
Our results indicate that both self-report EI and mixed EI not only display incremental
validity above and beyond cognitive ability and the FFM, but that they also show large
relative importance (31.3% relative importance for self-report EI and 42.8% relative
importance formixed EI) in the explained variance in job satisfaction. In particular,mixed
EI alone impressively accounts for nearly half of the explained variance in job satisfaction
compared to cognitive ability (1.5% relative importance) and the FFM (55.7% relative
importance for five personality traits as a whole set). We found similar effects for the
incremental validity and relative importance of EI with regard to organizational
commitment and turnover intentions. These findings are consistent with – and add to
priormeta-analytic findings on –howEI contributes relative importancewith regard to job
performance (O’Boyle et al., 2011).
Our study also explored the theoretical mechanisms through which EI influences job
satisfaction. Building on goal setting theory and self-perception theory (Bem, 1967; Locke,
1976), we found that the relationship between EI and job satisfaction is mediated by both
state affect and job performance. EI may be a characteristic that causes employees to see
both their work performance and their job in a rosy light, one which promotes positive
affect. Employees high on EI may then observe their positive affect at work and deduce
that they have high job satisfaction. Building on goal setting theory and self-perception
theory (Bem, 1967; Locke, 1976), we weaved prior meta-analytic findings on EI–job
performance relationships into our mediation model and found that job performance
mediates the relationship between EI and job satisfaction. This shows EI’s relevance to the
goal setting literature and indicates that EI helps employees to reach their performance
goals. Employeesmay thendeduce their own level of job satisfaction from their level of job
performance. These findings open multiple avenues for future research on EI, goals, and
work criteria. Locke and Latham (2002) suggested a set of moderators (e.g., goal
importance, goal commitment, and task complexity), and future researchers may
consider developing models that include these additional moderators in order to derive a
more thorough picture of the interrelationships among EI, goals, and work criteria.
Limitations and future directions
First, there were a small number of samples for some of our meta-analytic distributions,
which makes the results subject to second-order sampling error. For the same reason, we
were not able to analyse some moderators for some types of EI. Therefore, we encourage
readers to exercise caution when interpreting our results based on a small number of
samples, and we acknowledge that the results based on a small number of samples are
preliminary. This partly explains why the results of our moderator analyses are
inconsistent across three EI types. Moderator testing in meta-analysis is a low power
test (Steel & Kammeyer-Mueller, 2002). Therefore, if the number of samples across
different levels of moderators is small (ability EI distributions in particular), then the
results of moderation can hardly be significant, which is why we identified some
inconsistencies in our results across three types of EI. We thus encourage readers to
interpret the results of moderator analyses based on a small number of samples with
caution.
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 195
Second, the present meta-analytic review was dominated by the studies using cross-
sectional design. Future studies should use longitudinal designs and conduct advanced
analyses, such as latent growthmodelling (Bliese & Ployhart, 2002), to draw robust causal
inferences.
Third, at bivariate level, we found a significant moderator effect of the emotional
labour demand of jobs on the relationship between self-report EI and job satisfaction. We
suspect that this moderator may also function in our mediation model in such a way that
people under high emotional labour demands may have high job satisfaction with high EI
through affect or job performance. This moderated mediation model may help us better
integrate our variables. However, we cannot use meta-analysis to test this model because
moderated mediation models have to be tested based on raw data, whereas ours – like all
other meta-analyses – is also based on correlation matrices without raw data. For this
reason, we encourage future studies to collect primary data to assess the moderated
mediation model described here.
Practical implications
Job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intentions are important
attitudes related tomany criticalworkplace outcomes, such as jobperformance, turnover,
profits, and psychological well-being. Our investigations provide insights and evidence
regarding the importance of employees’ EI in determining employees’ work attitudes. To
produce satisfied and productive workers, organizations can incorporate EI in employee
education, training, and development (Walter et al., 2011).
Job satisfaction is a very important form of employee job attitude in organizations,
because job satisfaction is known to improve physical andpsychological health outcomes,
to be positively related to organizational commitment, organizational citizenship
behaviour, and task performance, and to be negatively related to turnover intention,
turnover, and withdrawal cognitions and behaviours (Schleicher et al., 2011). Impor-
tantly, our research findings suggest a low-cost, yet effective, way to staff an organization
with satisfied employees, which is to hire emotionally intelligent people. Incorporating a
measure of EI during the selection process would help an organization to find satisfied
employees because emotionally intelligent employees aremore satisfied, according to our
research findings. Nonetheless, hiring people high in EI does not mean that organizations
are free of their obligations to reduce workplace stress and strain and to improve overall
working conditions. Organizations with good values can increase employees’ organiza-
tional commitment and reduce turnover intentions (Abbott, White, & Charles, 2005).
Equally importantly, organizations that are perceived to support their employees have
employees who are more committed (Loi, Hang-Yue, & Foley, 2006).
Although ability EI tests did not show incremental validity, they may still have
considerable practical importance. Their objective nature means that they are not
susceptible to test takers’ self-serving biases, so they may be useful when hiring new
employees, and also for giving feedback to current employees who are resistant to advice
from their peers (O’Boyle et al., 2011; Walter et al., 2011). For practitioners who care
little about the overlap between self-report and mixed EI and other psychological
constructs, our results suggest that one should consider utilizing self-report/peer-report EI
measures, because the validity of both self-report EI and mixed EI in predicting job
satisfaction ismuch larger than that of ability EI.We also recommend the use ofmixed EI as
a shorthand alternative to a lengthy battery of a few traditional personnel tests, because
mixed EI captures a compound of different constructs and demonstrates reasonable
196 Chao Miao et al.
criterion-related validity. Because self-report measures and mixed measures show
incremental validity over cognitive ability and personality measures, organizations that
have lengthy batteries of such measures can still increase their ability to predict job
satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intentions by incorporating self-
report and/or mixed EI measures.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Associate Editor Tim Munyon and the anonymous reviewers for their
knowledgeable advice and guidance. They helped with theory development, statistical
analysis, and presentation andwriting. Tim’s advice was invaluable, and the article’s focus and
clarity was much improved with his help. In addition, Editor Sharon Clarke also made useful
suggestions that made the paper more focused and concise. Thanks to all for their help.
References
Abbott, G.N.,White, F. A.,&Charles,M.A. (2005). Linking values andorganizational commitment: A
correlational and experimental investigation in twoorganizations. Journal ofOccupational and
Organizational Psychology, 78, 531–551. doi:10.1348/096317905X26174
Ashforth, B. E., &Humphrey, R.H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity.
Academy of Management Review, 18, 88–115. doi:10.5465/AMR.1993.3997508
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1995). Emotion in the workplace: A reappraisal. Human
Relations, 48, 97–125. doi:10.1177/001872679504800201
Ashkanasy, N. M., & Daus, C. S. (2002). Emotion in theworkplace: The new challenge formanagers.
Academy of Management Executive, 16, 76–86. doi:10.5465/AME.2002.6640191
Ashkanasy,N.M.,&Daus,C. S. (2005). Rumors of thedeath of emotional intelligence in organizational
behavior are vastly exaggerated. Journal ofOrganizational Behavior,26, 441–452. doi:10.1002/
job.320
Ashkanasy, N. M., & Humphrey, R. H. (2011). Current emotion research in organizational behavior.
Emotion Review, 3, 214–224. doi:10.1177/1754073910391684
Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands–resources model: State of the art. Journal
of Managerial Psychology, 22, 309–328. doi:10.1108/02683940710733115
Bar-On, R. (2000). Emotions and social intelligence: Insights from the emotional quotient inventory.
In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory,
development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace (pp. 363–
388). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance
phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200. doi:10.1037/h0024835
Berry, C.M., Lelchook, A.M.,&Clark,M.A. (2012). Ameta-analysis of the interrelationships between
employee lateness, absenteeism, and turnover: Implications for models of withdrawal behavior.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33, 678–699. doi:10.1002/job.778
Bliese, P. D., & Ployhart, R. E. (2002). Growth modeling using random coefficient models: Model
building, testing, and illustrations.Organizational ResearchMethods,5, 362–387. doi:10.1177/
109442802237116
Boyatzis, R., Brizz, T., & Godwin, L. (2011). The effect of religious leaders’ emotional and social
competencies on improving parish vibrancy. Journal of Leadership &Organizational Studies,
18, 192–206. doi:10.1177/1548051810369676
Breaux, D.M.,Munyon, T. P., Hochwarter,W. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2009). Politics as amoderator of the
accountability – Job satisfaction relationship: Evidence across three studies. Journal of
Management, 35, 307–326. doi:10.1177/0149206308318621
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 197
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317905X26174
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.1993.3997508
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872679504800201
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AME.2002.6640191
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.320
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.320
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1754073910391684
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683940710733115
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0024835
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.778
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442802237116
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442802237116
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1548051810369676
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206308318621
Byron, K. (2007). Male and female managers’ ability to read emotions: Relationships with
supervisors’ performance ratings and subordinates’ satisfaction ratings. Journal of Occupa-
tional and Organizational Psychology, 80, 713–733. doi:10.1348/096317907X174349
Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: A
meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking and job performance. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 92, 909–927. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.909
Côt�e, S. (2014). Emotional intelligence in organizations. Annual Review of Organizational
Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 459–488. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-
031413-091233
Côt�e, S., &Miners, C. T. (2006). Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 51, 1–28. doi:10.2189/asqu.51.1.1
Crawford, E. R., LePine, J. A., & Rich, B. L. (2010). Linking job demands and resources to employee
engagement and burnout: A theoretical extension and meta-analytic test. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 95, 834–848. doi:10.1037/a0019364
De Clercq, D., Bouckenooghe, D., Raja, U., & Matsyborska, G. (2014). Unpacking the goal
congruence–organizational deviance relationship: The roles ofwork engagement and emotional
intelligence. Journal of Business Ethics, 124, 695–711. doi:10.1007/s10551-013-1902-0
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of
progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
Dong, Y., Seo, M. G., & Bartol, K. (2013). No pain, no gain: An affect-based model of developmental
job experience and the buffering effects of emotional intelligence. Academy of Management
Journal, 57, 1056–1077. doi:10.5465/amj.2011.0687
Fisher, C. D. (2000). Mood and emotions while working: Missing pieces of job satisfaction? Journal
of Organizational Behavior, 21, 185–202. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<185:: AID-JOB34>3.0.CO;2-M
Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (2000). Relations of emotional intelligence, practical intelligence, general
intelligence, and trait affectivity with interview outcomes: It’s not all just ‘G’. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 21, 203–220. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<203::AID- JOB38>3.0.CO;2-Z
Ganzach, Y. (1998). Intelligence and job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 526–
539. doi:10.2307/256940
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York, NY:
Bantam Books.
Gonzalez-Mul�e, E., Carter, K., & Mount, M. K. (2014). Is ignorance really bliss? A meta-analysis of
the relationship between general mental ability and attitudes. Working Paper.
Gooty, J., Connelly, S., Griffith, J., &Gupta, A. (2010). Leadership, affect and emotions: A state of the
science review. The Leadership Quarterly, 21, 979–1004. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.005
Gooty, J., Gavin, M. B., Ashkanasy, N. M., & Thomas, J. S. (2014). The wisdom of letting go and
performance: The moderating role of emotional intelligence and discrete emotions. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 87, 392–413. doi:10.1111/joop.12053
Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotion regulation in theworkplace: A newway to conceptualize emotional
labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 95–110. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95
Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). The influence of culture, community, and the nested-self in the stress process:
Advancing conservation of resources theory.Applied Psychology: An International Review,50,
337–370. doi:10.1111/1464-0597.00062
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feelings. Berkeley,
CA: University of California
Press.
Hulsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-
analysis of three decades of research. Journal ofOccupationalHealth Psychology,16, 361–389.
doi:10.1037/a0022876
Humphrey, R. H. (2002). The many faces of emotional leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 13,
493–504. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00140-6
198 Chao Miao et al.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317907X174349
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.909
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091233
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091233
http://dx.doi.org/10.2189/asqu.51.1.1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019364
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-1902-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0687
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<185::AID-JOB34>3.0.CO;2-M
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<185::AID-JOB34>3.0.CO;2-M
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<203::AID-JOB38>3.0.CO;2-Z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<203::AID-JOB38>3.0.CO;2-Z
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256940
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/joop.12053
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00062
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022876
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00140-6
Humphrey, R. H. (2013). Effective leadership: Theory, cases, and applications. Los Angeles, CA:
Sage.
Humphrey, R. H., Ashforth, B. E., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2015). The bright side of emotional labor.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36, 749–769. doi:10.1002/job.2019
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in
research findings. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in
research findings. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Johnson, J. W. (2000). A heuristic method for estimating the relative weight of predictor variables
in multiple regression. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 35, 1–19. doi:10.1207/S15327906
MBR3501_1
Johnson, S. K. (2009). Do you feel what I feel? Mood contagion and leadership outcomes. The
Leadership Quarterly, 20, 814–827. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.06.012
Johnson, J. W., & LeBreton, J. M. (2004). History and use of relative importance indices in
organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 238–257. doi:10.1177/1094428
104266510
Johnson, H. A. M., & Spector, P. E. (2007). Service with a smile: Do emotional intelligence, gender,
and autonomy moderate the emotional labor process? Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 12, 319. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.319
Jordan, P. J., Ashkanasy, N. M., Hartel, C. E. J., & Hooper, G. S. (2002). Workgroup emotional
intelligence: Scale development and relationship to team process effectiveness and goal focus.
Human Resource Management Review, 12, 195–214. doi:10.1016/S1053-4822(02)00046-3
Jordan, P. J., & Troth, A. (2011). Emotional intelligence and leader member exchange: The
relationship with employee turnover intentions and job satisfaction. Leadership &
Organization Development Journal, 32, 260–280. doi:10.1108/01437731111123915
Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and
cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 54–78. doi:10.1037/a0017286
Judge, T. A., Heller,D.,&Mount,M.K. (2002). Five-factormodel of personality and job satisfaction: A
meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 530–541. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.530
Judge, T. A., & Kammeyer-Muellar, J. D. (2008). Affect, satisfaction, and performance. In N. M.
Ashkanasy & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Research companion to emotion in organizations (pp. 136–
151). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.
Judge, T. A., & Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D. (2012). Job attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 63,
341–367. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100511
Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction–job performance
relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376–407.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.376
Kafetsios, K., & Zampetakis, L. A. (2008). Emotional intelligence and job satisfaction: Testing the
mediatory role of positive and negative affect at work. Personality and Individual Differences,
44, 712–722. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.004
Karim, J. (2009). Emotional intelligence and psychological distress: Testing the mediatory role of
affectivity. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 5, 20–39. doi:10.5964/ejop.v5i4.238
Kellett, J. B., Humphrey, R. H., & Sleeth, R. G. (2006). Empathy and the emergence of task and
relations leaders. The Leadership Quarterly, 17, 146–162. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.12.003
Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY:
Guilford.
Kluemper, D. H., DeGroot, T., & Choi, S. (2013). Emotion management ability: Predicting task
performance, citizenship, and deviance. Journal of Management, 39, 878–905. doi:10.1177/
0149206311407326
Kong, F., & Zhao, J. (2013). Affective mediators of the relationship between trait emotional
intelligence and life satisfaction in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 54,
197–201. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.028
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 199
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.2019
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.06.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428104266510
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428104266510
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.12.4.319
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-4822(02)00046-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/01437731111123915
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017286
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.530
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100511
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.376
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v5i4.238
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.12.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206311407326
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206311407326
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.028
Locke, E. A. (1969). What is job satisfaction? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
4, 309–336. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(69)90013-0
Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. InM. D. Dunnette (Ed.),Handbook of
industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1297–1349). Chicago, IL: Rand-McNally.
Locke, E. A., Cartledge, N., & Knerr, C. S. (1970). Studies of the relationship between satisfaction,
goal-setting, and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 5, 135–
158. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(70)90011-5
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task
motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.
57.9.705
Loi, R., Hang-Yue, N., & Foley, S. (2006). Linking employees’ justice perceptions to organizational
commitment and intention to leave: The mediating role of perceived organizational support.
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79, 101–120. doi:10.1348/
096317905X39657
Martins, A., Ramalho, N., & Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship
between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 554–
564. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abilities: Emotional intelligence.Annual
Review of Psychology, 59, 507–536. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093646
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997).What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey &D. J. Sluyter (Eds.),
Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 3–25).
New York, NY: Basic Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence with
the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion, 3, 97–105. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97
Mitchell, T. R., Holtom, B. C., Lee, T. W., Sablynski, C. J., & Erez, M. (2001). Why people stay: Using
job embeddedness to predict voluntary turnover.Academy ofManagement Journal, 44, 1102–
1121. doi:10.2307/3069391
Mitchell, T. R., & Lee, T. W. (2001). 5. The unfolding model of voluntary turnover and job
embeddedness: Foundations for a comprehensive theory of attachment. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 23, 189–246. doi:10.1016/S0191-3085(01)23006-8
Munyon, T. P., Hochwarter,W. A., Perrew�e, P. L., & Ferris, G. R. (2010). Optimism and the nonlinear
citizenshipbehavior – Job satisfaction relationship in three studies. Journal ofManagement,36,
1505–1528. doi:10.1177/0149206309350085
O’Boyle, E. H., Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., Hawver, T. H., & Story, P. A. (2011). The relation
between emotional intelligence and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 32, 788–818. doi:10.1002/job.714
Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 90, 175–181. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175
Petrides, K. V. (2009a). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire
(TEIQue). InC. Stough,D.H. Saklofske& J. D. A. Parker (Eds.),Assessing emotional intelligence:
Theory, research, and applications (pp. 85–101). New York, NY: Springer Science.
Petrides, K. V. (2009b). Technical manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire
(TEIQue). London, UK: London Psychometric Laboratory.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioral validation in two
studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European Journal of
Personality, 17, 39–57. doi:10.1002/per.466
Schleicher, D. J., Hansen, S. D., & Fox, K. E. (2011). Job attitudes andwork values. In S. Zedeck (Ed.),
APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology, vol. 3: Maintaining, expanding,
and contracting the organization. APA handbooks in psychology (pp. 137–189).Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., & Dornheim, L.
(1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and
Individual Differences, 25, 167–177. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4
200 Chao Miao et al.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(69)90013-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(70)90011-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.705
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.705
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317905X39657
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/096317905X39657
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093646
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3069391
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-3085(01)23006-8
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206309350085
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.714
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.466
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Bhullar, N., & Rooke, S. E. (2007). Ameta-analytic
investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and
Individual Differences, 42, 921–933. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.003
Schwab, D. P. (2005). Research methods for organizational studies. New York, NY: Psychology
Press.
Spence, G., Oades, L. G., & Caputi, P. (2004). Trait emotional intelligence and goal self-integration:
Important predictors of emotional well-being? Personality and Individual Differences, 37,
449–461. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.001
Staw, B.M., Bell, N. E., &Clausen, J. A. (1986). The dispositional approach to job attitudes: A lifetime
longitudinal test. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31, 56–77. doi:10.2307/2392766
Steel, P. D., & Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D. (2002). Comparing meta-analytic moderator estimation
techniques under realistic conditions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 96–111. doi:10.1037/
0021-9010.87.1.96
Sy, T., Tram, S., & O’Hara, L. A. (2006). Relation of employee andmanager emotional intelligence to
job satisfaction and performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 461–473. doi:10.1016/
j.jvb.2005.10.003
Tett, R. P.,&Burnett, D.D. (2003). Apersonality trait-based interactionistmodel of jobperformance.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 500–517. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.500
Tett, R. P., &Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait expression, and cross-situational
consistency: Testing a principle of trait activation. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 397–
423. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2000.2292
Thoresen, C. J., Kaplan, S. A., Barsky, A. P., Warren, C. R., & de Chermont, K. (2003). The affective
underpinnings of job perceptions and attitudes: A meta-analytic review and integration.
Psychological Bulletin, 129, 914–945. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.914
Van Rooy, D. L., & Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence: A meta-analytic investiga-
tion of predictive validity and nomological net. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 71–95.
doi:10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00076-9
Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing: Combining psychometric meta-analysis and
structural equations modeling. Personnel Psychology, 48, 865–885. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.
1995.tb01784.x
Walter, F., & Bruch, H. (2009). An affective events model of charismatic leadership behavior: A
review, theoretical integration, and research agenda. Journal of Management, 35, 1428–
1452. doi:10.1177/0149206309342468
Walter, F., Cole, M. S., &Humphrey, R. H. (2011). Emotional intelligence: Sine qua non of leadership
or folderol? The Academy of Management Perspectives, 25, 45–59. doi:10.5465/AMP.2011.
59198449
Wang,G., Seibert, S. E., &Boles, T. L. (2011). Synthesizingwhatwe knowand looking ahead: Ameta-
analytical review of 30 years of emotional labor research. Research on Emotion in
Organizations, 7, 15–43. doi:10.1108/S1746-9791(2011)0000007006
Watson, D. (2000).Mood and temperament. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of
positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Weiss, H. M. (2002). Deconstructing job satisfaction: Separating evaluations, beliefs and affective
experiences. Human Resource Management Review, 12, 173–194. doi:10.1016/S1053-4822
(02)00045-1
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the
structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L.
Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Weiss, H. M., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. S. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective
experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in affective experiences over time.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 1–24. doi:10.1006/obhd.1999.
2824
Emotional intelligence and work attitudes 201
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392766
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.1.96
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.1.96
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2005.10.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2005.10.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.3.500
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.2000.2292
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.6.914
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00076-9
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1995.tb01784.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1995.tb01784.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0149206309342468
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2011.59198449
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2011.59198449
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/S1746-9791(2011)0000007006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-4822(02)00045-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-4822(02)00045-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1999.2824
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1999.2824
Whitener, E. M. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in meta-analysis.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 315–321. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.75.3.315
Winkel, D. E., Wyland, R. L., Shaffer, M. A., & Clason, P. (2011). A new perspective on psychological
resources: Unanticipated consequences of impulsivity and emotional intelligence. Journal of
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 84, 78–94. doi:10.1348/2044-8325.002001
Wong, C.-S., & Law, K. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on
performance and attitude: An exploratory study. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 243–274.
doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1
Received 13 October 2015; revised version received 26 November 2016
Supporting Information
The following supporting informationmay be found in the online edition of the article:
Table S1. Dimensions/facets represented by three types of EI.
Table S2. Job satisfaction measures.
Table S3. Main codes for the studies included in the EI – JS meta-analysis.
Table S4. Main codes for the studies included in the EI – OC meta-analysis.
Table S5. Main codes for the studies included in the EI – TI meta-analysis.
Table S6. Main codes for the studies included in the MSCEIT dimensions – JS meta-
analysis.
Table S7. Main codes for the studies included in the WLEIS dimensions – JS meta-
analysis.
Table S8. Main codes for the studies included in the EQ-i Dimensions – JS meta-
analysis.
Table S9. Main codes for the studies included in the EI – job satisfaction dimensions
meta-analysis.
Table S10. Main codes for the studies included in the EI – state affect meta-analysis.
Table S11. (a) Meta-analytically derived corrected intercorrelation matrix for
hierarchical multiple regression and relative weight analyses for predicting job
satisfaction. (b) Meta-analytically derived corrected intercorrelation matrix for
hierarchical multiple regression and relative weight analyses for predicting organiza-
tional commitment. (c) Meta-analytically derived corrected intercorrelation matrix for
hierarchical multiple regression and relative weight analyses for predicting turnover
intention.
Table S12. Meta-analytically derived corrected intercorrelation matrix for Figure 1 (a)
and (b).
Table S13. Supplemental psychometric meta-analysis results.
Table S14. Comparison of the fit of the alternative models.
Appendix S1. Supplemental notes.
202 Chao Miao et al.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.75.3.315
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/2044-8325.002001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1
Copyright of Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology is the property of Wiley-
Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder’s express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 355
The Role of Emotional Intelligence in the Workplace:
A Literature Review
Ashkan Khalili
Multimedia University, Malaysia
Emotional intelligence has become a familiar issue between educators, counselors and
business leaders due to a considerable role in the workplace. Namely, accurate self-
assessment together with conflict management are just some significant influences of
emotional intelligence within organisations that would result in increasing organisational
effectiveness. Hence the purpose of this manuscript is to provide a comprehensive
literature review on the emergence of emotional intelligence in the workplace as well
as discussing main theories of emotional intelligence, based on the relevant literature
and previous empirical studies.
Emotional Intelligence (EI) in the Workplace
Traditionally, negligence (Eriksson, 2004), avoidance (Turnbull, 1999), irrational and
negation (Fineman, 1993), weakness and inability to control oneself have been viewed in
emotional themes within organisational life. Recently, role of emotions in the workplace
have started to exert positively through organisational scholars (Fineman, 1997;
Domagalski, 1999; Turnbull 1999; Fisher & Ashkanasy, 2000; Brief and Weiss, 2002;
Jordan and Troth, 2002). “texture” of organizing (Fineman, 1993) and also enterprise
impacts on individual’s thought, feeling in the workplace (Brief & Weiss, 2002), belong to
emotion supposition (Fineman, 1993). Excellent organisations are places where feelings
are managed, matured or removed. Feelings are perceived as irrelevant to job activity
but emotions get in the way of effective performance (Fineman, 1996).
Many researches that have concentrated on emotion in the workplaces have asserted
that organisations are “emotional places” (Armstrong, 2000), “incubators of emotions”
(Muchinsky, 2000) or “emotional arenas” (Fineman, 2000) and it always stimulates
doubt and commotion (French, 2001). That is to say, few researches have concentrated
on how emotions are experimented in the workplace (Pekrun & Frese, 1992), comprising
organisation psychic pressure and stress (Fineman, 2000). Since emotions and feelings are
at the core of the human experiment (Muchinsky, 2000), very nature of the “organisation”
of work pertain to what individuals do with their sensations (Fineman, 1993).
One important aspect of work performance is work affect (Kafetsios & Zampetakis,
2008). Muchinsky (2000) claimed that emotions have many differences which cover
from pleasurable experiments of our existence which are positive experiences to the
negative ones that are the most noxious. Individual’s job-related behaviour is reflected
from affective or emotional experiments in the work place that generate cognition (Weiss
& Cropanzano, 1996).
Brief and Weiss (2002) proposed that while firms can impact on one’s feelings, thoughts
and actions, individual’s feelings, thoughts and actions likewise can impress the enterprise
356 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
which they are employed. Indeed, work environment is an emotion-eliciting place;
therefore individuals are required to employ “emotional labor”.
Hochschild (1983) identified emotional labor as “the management of feeling to create
a publicly observable facial and bodily display for a wage”. Certainly, emotion in
the organisation is such a communicable phenomenon that impact on other workers’
emotions. Sanchez-Burks and Huy (2007) claimed that due to emotional contagion
which is an automatic, non-conscious psychological process, people experiment shared
thrills. In other words, interaction in the workplace causes spreading or transferring
thrills from an individual to others (Eriksson, 2004). Goleman et al. (2001) stated that
when the group is more uninterrupted, emotions shared are stronger.
Therefore, the rising testimony of this emotional side of work exhibits one of the
fundamental motives of growth makes it worth looking into the concept of emotional
intelligence (EI).
Indeed, emotional intelligence plays a considerable role in the workplace. Within the
past 30 years research investigating factors that contribute to success in workplace
have resulted in distinguishing factors that are affiliated to workplace intelligence.
These researches used quantified data on performance from countless organisations
and industries and have contributed to the knowledge base in emotional intelligence
(Lynn, 2002).
Moreover, The 1990s showed increasing attention on the part of researchers and
practitioners on the construct of emotional intelligence. This interest reflects the theoretical
proposition that individuals who are high in emotional intelligence are seemingly more
successful in work-related and non-work aspects of life than low-emotionally intelligent
ones. Wolfe and Caruso (2004, p.3) asserted emotions include essential information that
assists us “to be better at what we do”. In 1998, Goleman reviewed analyses of studies
of about 500 organisations around the world, “point to the paramount place of emotional
intelligence in excellence on the job—in virtually any job” (Goleman, 1998, p. 6). He
indicates organisations become leaders and rise to the top position while they have the
highest emotional intelligence measure. Another significant finding that he discovered
during reviewing these studies is star employees have more emotional intelligence (EI)
than other employees. He found emotional intelligence is important twice as much as
analytic and technical skill for those organisations.
Emotional intelligence has been investigated since early 1990s, but solely in recent years
become pertinent to the workplace. Weisinger (1998) proposed the existence of a direct
linkage between emotional intelligence and success at work. In accord with Bridget
Murray (1998), individuals who are at the top of their field are not only good at their
works. This means that, emotional intelligence (EI) takes more than intelligence quotient
(IQ) to achieve success at workplace (Goleman, 1995). That is to say, besides bosses and
corporate leaders, employees also need high portion of emotional intelligence, because
they present the organisation to the public. Cherniss (2000) found several scholars
suggested that there is a positive and direct relationship between the level of emotional
intelligence (EI) and workplace performance. Cooper (1997) claimed that well managed
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 357
emotions can assist ones to extend trust, loyalty and commitment, with their selves, their
groups and their firms.
Various occupations call for different types of emotional intelligence. For instance,
success in sales requires the empathic competency to identify the mood of the customer
and the interpersonal skill to make a decision when to present a product and when to
keep quiet. And also, success in painting or professional tennis demands a more one
form of self-discipline and motivation. Thus emotional intelligence affects just about
everything you do at work. “Even when you work in a solitary setting, how well you
work has a lot to do with how well you discipline and motivate yourself” (Goleman, as
cited in Murray, 1998, p. 3).
In 1993, Spencer and Spencer managed an analysis at L’Oreal Company and discovered
that sales agents who were elected dependent on their emotional competencies sold
significantly more than agents who were selected using standard recruitment techniques.
The study exhibited that sales agents who were selected based on emotional ability sold
an average of $91,370 more than those in the control group annually. Continually, the
research also showed that salespeople selected based upon emotional competence had
63% less turnovers in the first year than those selected in a typical way (Spencer &
Spencer, 1993).
Another issue that must be considered in emotional intelligence and workplace is gender
differences and acquisition of emotional intelligence which found interesting results.
Goleman (1998, p. 285) claimed that “men and women seem equally able to increase
their emotional intelligence”. In a study by Steven Stein (as cited in Murray, 1998), 4,500
men and 3,200 women were assessed for their emotional intelligence. He discovered that
women scored higher than men on empathy and social responsibility while men surpassed
women on stress tolerance and self-confidence. His findings suggested women and men
are equally emotionally intelligent, but they are different in these spheres. Khalili (2011)
conducted an empirically investigation of emotional intelligence within employees of
a private small and medium enterprise (SME) in Iran. He found that women are more
emotionally intelligence than men but there is no significant difference between them.
Age has been considered in emotional intelligence acquisition. Goleman (1998, p.285)
said researches show that, “maturity remains an advantage; it may be slightly harder to
‘teach young dogs new tricks’”. Mayer and Salovy (as cited in Goleman, 1998) discovered
emotional intelligence increases significantly by age with an apex happening in the forties.
Fariselli, Ghini and Freedman (2006) examined the relationship between emotional
intelligence and age in the workplace. They chose 405 American workers between 22-
70 years. They discovered that emotional quotient (EQ) score increases slightly by age.
They concluded that while a slight majority of older individuals are higher in EQ, there
are many young people with higher EQ scores than their older counterparts. As well,
they discussed that emotional intelligence is an important and crucial competence that
leaders require to take into consideration in the current work context (Fariselli, Ghini
& Freedman, 2006).
358 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
Another part that research in the emotional intelligence area has concentrated considerably
is leadership which is a fundamental workplace quality. Fleishman and Harris (1962)
noted that before commencing research in the field of emotional intelligence, the Ohio
State Leadership investigations informed leaders who were capable of establishing mutual
trust, respect, and certain warmth and close relationship with members of their group
were more effective. This consequence is not surprising given that many scholars have
debated that effective leadership is fundamentally contingent on the leader’s competencies
to solve the composite social difficulties which can arise in organisations (Mumford,
Zaccaro, Harding, Jacobs, & Fleishman, 2000).
Other field of interest in the workplace is the cost-effectiveness of emotional intelligence.
Several researches have stated the economic value of employing staff based on emotional
intelligence. Based on a report to Congress, the US General Accounting Office (GAO)
(1998) indicated the amount saved when the United States Air Force used Bar-On’s
Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i) to select program recruiters. By choosing those
persons who scored highest in Bar-On’s Emotional Quotient Inventory as recruiters, they
increased their ability to select successful recruiters by threefold and saved $3 million
per year. Boyatzis (1999) discovered that when partners in a multinational consulting
organisation were estimated on emotional intelligence competencies, partners who scored
above the median on nine or more competencies delivered $1.2 million more profit than
did other partners. Industry in the United States is losing between $5.6 and $16.8 billion
annually, by not following training guidelines established to raise emotional intelligence
(EI) in the workplace (Cherniss & Goleman, 1998). They discovered that the influence
of training employees in emotional and social skills with programs which followed their
guidelines was higher than for other programs, and by not executing these programs
enterprises were receiving less of an influence and as a result losing money.
Emotional intelligence has formed a hypothesis as impacting a myriad of workplace
variables such as performance, job satisfaction, absenteeism, organisational commitment
and leadership (Rozell, Pettijohn & Parker, 2002).
Thi Lam and Kirby (2002) investigated the linkage between emotional intelligence and
cognitive-based performance using US university graduates. Thi Lam and Kirby placed
participants in stressful situations to replicate a modern work environment and measured
emotional intelligence with the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS).
Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) is a test of ability rather than a self-
report measure. The test-taker performs a series of tasks that are designed to estimate the
individual’s competency to be aware of, identify, understand, and work with emotion.
However, they discovered that overall emotional intelligence, emotional perception, and
emotional regulation uniquely describe person cognitive-based performance over and
beyond the level attributable to general intelligence (Thi Lam & Kirby, 2002).
Nikolaou and Tsaousis (2002) investigated the relevance of emotional intelligence
and source of occupational stress on a sample of 200 professionals in mental health
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 359
institutions in Greece. They found a negative relationship among emotional intelligence
and stress at workplace, indicating that high scorers in overall emotional intelligence
endured less stress identifying with occupational environment. As well, they discovered
a positive relation between emotional intelligence and organisational commitment,
which suggested that emotional intelligence is a determining factor of employees’
loyalty to the organisations. Regarding overall emotional intelligence score, they did not
address significant differences among males and females. An important relationship was
discovered between emotional intelligence, age and education. They also found job type
influences overall emotional intelligence scores, but no influence was found among job
type and emotional intelligence subscales (Nikolaou & Tsaousis, 2002).
Oginska-Bulik (2005) investigated a study in Poland with the aim to see if emotional
intelligence plays an important role in forming the interaction among individuals and
their work environment. The results approved a necessary role of emotional intelligence
on being aware of occupational stress and intercepting employees from negative health
outcomes. Oginska-Bulik (2005) indicated a higher degree of emotional intelligence in
women rather than men.
David McClelland (1998) investigated division heads of a global food and beverage
company and found that 50% of division directors hired using standard methods left
within two years, mostly due to poor performance. When the firm began selecting based
upon emotional abilities like initiative, self-confidence, and leadership, only 6% left in
two years. He also discovered that the divisions conducted by leaders strong in emotional
intelligence competencies exceed in performance yearly revenue targets by a margin of
15-20% more high than divisions with leaders that did not show emotional intelligence
abilities (McClelland, 1998).
In 1998, Daniel Goleman (1998) claimed by reviewing researches of nearly 500
companies around the world concluded that leaders have high degrees of emotional
intelligence. Goleman noted leaders are highly emotionally intelligent because (1) they
present the company to the public, (2) act reciprocally with the highest number of people
inside and outside the organisation, and (3) set the tone for staff morale. In 1998, Goleman
conducted a research which took into consideration competence models for 181 various
job positions from 121 companies. Management in each firm was asked to agree on a
profile indicating which factors were required for an individual to exhibit excellence
in a specific job. He discovered that 67% of the competencies management outlined as
determining factors of excellence within a job was related to emotional abilities. Just
few years later, he formed a hypothesis that emotional competence is a learned capability
based on emotional intelligence that causes outstanding performance at work. Dulewicz
and Higgs (2000) reported alike results. They researched one thousand managers, over
a seven-year period, concentrating on their abilities and their advancement through the
organisation. They discovered that emotional intelligence factors contributed 36% to
an individual’s progression, while intellectual intelligence contributed 27% through the
organisation.
360 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
Major Theories of Emotional Intelligence (EI)
The increasing penchant in emotions as well as growing awareness of the role that it
plays in business and in life is in great part due to the propagation of research over the
past decade on emotions generally and EI specifically (Jamali, D. et al., 2008). Generally,
emotional intelligence can be classified into two sets.
Firstly, it is the ability model matured by Mayer and Salovey that concentrates upon
using intelligence to guide emotions (Weinberger, 2004). Weinberger mentioned that as
of 2004, Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso were the only scholars promoting a more limited
ability (cognitive) model. Mayer and Salovey (1997) thought that emotional intelligence
is based initially in mental abilities. David Caruso was another scholar that joined
Mayer and Salovey. Mayer, Salovey and Caruso (2004) identified the diverging of the
foundational components of emotional intelligence based on the specific “bent” of the
foundational theorists. They improved a measurement of emotional intelligence called
the Mayer, Salovey, Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT).
Secondly, it is called the mixed-model (Bar-On, 1988 and Goleman, 1998) where
emotional intelligence is not solely depending on cognition, but upon a socio-emotion
centered in personality traits.
Precisely, Mayer and Salovey, (1997) focused upon the relevance between emotion and
cognition, while Bar-On (2000) concentrated upon emotional intelligence influences
on performance and well being, and Goleman (1995, 1998, Goleman et al., 2002)
concentrated on emotional intelligence as a competency model. Three theories that
have generated the most willingness in terms of research and applications are namely
the theories of Bar-On (1988, 2000), Mayer and Salovey (1997), and Goleman (1998).
Bar-On (1988, 2000)
The first theory which emerged is Bar-On theory in 1988. He coined the term of
“emotional quotient” (EQ) in his doctoral dissertation which is an analogue to intelligent
quotient (IQ).
Bar-On (1997) matured his own mixed-model construct of emotional intelligence, which
is tested and appraised by the Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-I). His construct
follows a 1-5-15 breakdown of assessing individual’s level of emotional intelligence.
There is an overall EQ score (1), with each of the five compound scales receiving a
score (5), and likewise 15 subscales, each receiving its own individual score. The five
social and emotional abilities subscales are sorted as “intrapersonal”, “interpersonal”,
“stress management”, “adaptability”, and “general mood” which all together impact an
individual’s ability to challenge with environmental demands effectively.
Hence, his model framed emotional intelligence in the concept of personality theory
that in case of general model of psychological well-being and adaption is best viewed
(Goleman, 2001). That is to say, the 15 subscales are divided up in accord with their
respective composite scales. All Bar-On EQ-I scales and the emotional intelligence
competencies and skills which assessed by each scale are addressed in Table 1.
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 361
Mayer and Salovey (1997)
Mayer and Salovey (1997) framed emotional intelligence within a developmental model
of intelligence which occurred as a result of a realization that traditional measures
of intelligence was not capable to measure individual differences in the emotional
intelligence ability to realize, process and effectively conduct emotions. Thus, they
derived a model based on cognitive consideration which outlines the specific mental
brilliance for distinguishing and culling emotions (Goleman, 2001).
Their model (Figure 1) is inclusive four tiers of abilities with the elaboration of emotional
skill which is increased from basic emotional perception to more complex processes of
unifying emotion and cognition.
The first tire includes abilities that allow one to perceive, estimate, and express emotions.
These types of abilities comprise identifying individual’s own and other’s emotions,
manifesting one’s emotions and distinguishing the expressions of emotion in others. In the
second tire skills involve using emotions to facilitate and prioritize idea (Goleman, 2001).
That is to say, using the emotions to assist in judgment as well as recognizing that mood
swings can lead to a thoughtfulness of alternative viewpoints, and understanding that
Table 1: The Bar-On model of emotional-social intelligence (ESI)
EQ-I SCALES The EI competencies and skills assessed by each scale
Intrapersonal
Self-regard
Emotional self-
awareness
Assertiveness
Independence
Self-actualization
Self-awareness and self-expression:
To accurately perceive, understand and accept oneself
To be aware of and understand one’s emotions
To effectively and constructively express one’s emotions and oneself
To be self-reliant and free of emotional dependency on others
To strive to achieve personal goals and actualize one’s potential
Interpersonal
Empathy
Social responsibility
Interpersonal
relationship
Social awareness and interpersonal relationship:
To be aware of and understand how others feel
To identify with one’s social group and cooperate with others
To establish mutually satisfying relationships and relate well with others
Stress management
Stress tolerance
Impulse control
Emotional management and regulation:
To effectively and constructively manage emotions
To effectively and constructively control emotions
Adaptability
Reality-testing
Flexibility
Problem-solving
Change management:
To objectively validate one’s feelings and thinking with external reality
To adopt and adjust one’s feelings and thinking to new situations
To effectively solve problems of a personal and interpersonal nature
General mood
Optimism
Happiness
Self-motivation:
To be positive and look at the brighter side of life
To feel content with oneself, others and life in general
362 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
a shift in emotional state and perspective can facilitate various kinds problem solving.
The third tier comprises of abilities namely labeling and discriminating among emotions,
understanding complex mixture of feelings and formulating rules about feelings. The
last (forth) tire is the general skill to manage emotions to uphold some social goals. This
level is more complex one of emotional intelligence, because abilities at forth tire allow
individuals to selectively engage in or separate from emotions to monitor and marshal
emotions in themselves and others (Goleman, 2001).
In 2002, Caruso and Salovey by using their ability-based construct identified “
Emotional
Blueprint” which equips managers in their emotional skills. In their manuscript, they
identified an “Emotional Blueprint” through which managers can appraise and mature
their emotional intelligence competencies. Their construct of emotional intelligence
concentrates upon the cognitive abilities of an individual to be capable of identify, use,
understand, and manage emotions. In other words, they posed emotional intelligence
which combines feeling with thinking. What appear later are the main themes of emotional
intelligence in Caruso and Salovey manuscript (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2002, p. 8):
1) “Read People: Identifying Emotions. Emotions contain data. They are signals to us
about significant events going on in our world, whether it’s our internal world, social
world, or the natural environment.”
2) “Get in the Mood: Using Emotions. How we feel influences how we think and what
we think about. Emotions direct our attention to important events; they ready us for a
certain action, and they help guide our thought processes as we solve problems.”
3) “Predict the Emotional Future: Understanding Emotions. Emotions are not random
events. They have underlying causes, they change according to a set of rules, and they
Figure 1: Mayer and Saloveys’ EI Model
Figure 1: Mayer and Saloveys’ EI Model
ADOPTED FROM: Bar-On and Parker (2000)
Emotional Perception
Registering and deciphering of
feelings and emotions
Emotional Integration
Integration of emotions into the
cognitive system, altering
cognition/thought
Emotional Management
Regulation of emotions and the
monitoring of their expression
Emotional Understanding
Assimilation, understanding,
reasoning with emotions
Emotional
Intelligence
Adopted from: Bar-On and Parker (2000)
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 363
can be understood. Knowledge of emotions is reflected by our emotion vocabulary and
our ability to conduct emotional what-if analyses.”
4) “Do It with Feeling: Managing Emotions. Because emotions contain information and
influence thinking, we need to incorporate emotions intelligently into our reasoning,
problem solving, judging, and behaving.”
Goleman (1998)
Goleman (1998) introduced his emotional intelligence based on performance theory
(that is merit based) including a separable set of abilities that integrate cognitive and
affective skills. He identified five dimensions of emotional intelligence in his proposed
framework (Table 2), comprising self-awareness, self-management, self-motivation,
social awareness (empathy) and social skills.
Goleman’s (1998) five dimensions model of emotional intelligence are described as
below:
1) Self-Awareness is allied with the competency to be conscious of which emotions,
moods, and impulses individual is experiencing and why. This also comprises
individual’s consciousness of the results of his or her feelings on others.
2) Self-Regulation pertains to the competency to keep individual’s own emotions and
impulses in check, to remain calm in potentially volatile conditions, and to maintain
composure notwithstanding individual’s emotions.
3) Motivation refers to the ability to remain concentrated upon goals irrespective of
obstacles, to operate from hope of prosperity rather than fear of inability to succeed,
delaying satisfaction, and to accept change to achieve goals.
4) Empathy represents individual’s competency to understand the feelings transmitted
through verbal and nonverbal messages, to prepare emotional support to people when
required, and to understand the linkages among others’ emotions and behaviour.
5) Social Skills is allied with individual’s competency to deal with difficulties without
behaving those who work with him or her, to not permit own or others’ negative
feelings to restrain collaboration, and to handle affective conflict with tact and
diplomacy.
That is to say, his five dimensions of EI classified into two broad categories (Figure 2),
which are personal competence and social competence in dealing with one’s own self
and others respectively (Goleman, 1995).
Table 2 illustrates Goleman’s model of emotional intelligence in 1998, which identified
five domains of emotional intelligence (EI). That earlier model consists of twenty-five
competencies, which divided into two broad competencies.
First category is personal competencies which mean understanding and conducting
emotions in oneself that comprise of three dimensions: Self-Awareness, Self-Regulation,
and Motivation. Second category is social competencies which mean knowing and
364 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
managing emotions in others that include two dimensions: Social-Awareness (Empathy)
and Social Skills (Bar-On & Parker, 2000; Kunnanatt, 2004).
The refinement model (Table 3) brings from latter statistical analyses by his colleague
Richard Boyatzis that supported decomposing the five dimensions into the four domains:
Self-Awareness which is being aware of what one feels, Self-Management considers the
Table 2: Goleman’s Five Dimensions Model of EI
THE EMOTIONAL COMPETENCE FRAMEWORK
P
er
so
na
l s
ki
lls
(
ho
w
w
e
m
an
ag
e
ou
rs
el
ve
s)
Self-awareness
Emotional awareness
Accurate self-
assessment
Self-confidence
Knowing one’s internal states, preferences, resources and intuitions
Recognizing one’s emotions and their effects
Knowing one’s strengths and limits
A strong sense of one’s self-worth and capabilities
Self-regulation
Self-control
Trustworthiness
Conscientiousness
Adaptability
Innovation
Managing one’s internal impulses and resources
Keeping disruptive emotions and impulses in check
Maintaining standards of honesty and integrity
Taking the responsibility for personal performance
Flexibility in handling change
Being comfortable with novel ideas, approaches, and new information
Motivation
Achievement drive
Commitment
Initiative
Optimism
Emotional tendencies that guide or facilitate reaching goals
Striving to improve or meet a standard of excellence
Aligning with goals of the group or organisation
Readiness to act on opportunities
Persistence in pursuing goals despite obstacles and setbacks
So
ci
al
s
ki
lls
(
ho
w
w
e
m
an
ag
e
re
la
tio
ns
hi
ps
)
Empathy
Understanding others
Developing others
Service orientation
Leveraging diversity
Political awareness
Awareness of other’s feelings, needs, and concerns
Sensing others’ feelings and perspectives, and taking active interest
in their concerns
Sensing others’ development needs and bolstering their abilities
Anticipating, recognizing, and meeting customers’ needs
Cultivating opportunities through different kinds of people
Reading a group’s emotional currents and power relationships
Social Skills
Influence
Communication
Conflict management
Leadership
Change catalyst
Building bonds
Collaboration &
cooperation
Team capabilities
Adeptness and inducing desirable responses in others
Wielding effective tactics for persuasion
Listening openly and sending convincing messages
Negotiating and resolving disagreements
Inspiring and guiding individuals and groups
Initiating or managing change
Nurturing instrumental relationships
Working with others toward shared goals
Creating group synergy in pursuing collective goals
Adopted from: Goleman’s (1998) Book “Working With Emotional Intelligence”
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 365
ability to regulate distressing influences like anxiety and anger and to restrain emotional
impulsivity, Social Awareness which comprises the competency of Empathy, and
Relationship Management or Social Skill which in a fundamental sense, the effectiveness
of our relationship skills depend on our ability to adjust ourselves to or affect the emotions
of another person. That ability in turn builds on other domains of emotional intelligence,
especially Self-Management and Social Awareness. If we cannot control our emotional
tantrums or impulses and lack Empathy, there is less possibility we will be effective in
our relationships (Boyatzis, Goleman, & Rhee, 2000).
Indeed, in their analysis of data on workplace effectiveness, they discovered that
Emotional Self-Awareness is required for effective Self-Management, which in turn
anticipates greater Social Skill. A secondary pathway runs from Self-Awareness to Social
Awareness (specifically Empathy. Finally, Relationship Management depends on a base
of Self-Management and Empathy, each of which in turn requires Self-Awareness.
Figure 2: Goleman’s Emotional Intelligence Two Broad Categories
Figure 2: Goleman’s Emotional Intelligence Two Broad Categories
ADOPTED FROM: Bar-On and Parker (2000) and Kunnanatt (2004)
Personal Competence
• Self-Awareness
• Self-Management
• Self-Motivation
Social Competence
• Social Awareness
• Social Skills
Emotional Intelligence
Adopted from: Bar-On and Parker (2000) and Kunnanatt (2004)
Table 3: Goleman’s Refinement Model of EI (2001)
Personal Competence Social Competence
Recognition
Self-Awareness
-Emotional self-awareness
-Accurate self-assessment
-Self-confidence
Social Awareness
-Empathy
-Organisational awareness
-Service
Regulation
Self-Management
-Emotional self-control
-Transparency
-Adaptability
-Achievement
-Initiative
-Optimism
Relationship Management
-Inspirational leadership
-Influence
-Developing others
-Change catalyst
-Conflict management
-Building bonds
-Teamwork & collaboration
366 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
Goleman (1998) and Bar- On (1997) perceived emotional intelligence as a mixed-model
that combines not only cognitive abilities, but also personality traits also. Goleman (1998)
Working with Emotional Intelligence and Goleman et al. (2002) Primal Leadership, are
two books that further purified Goleman’s broad definition of emotional intelligence
into domains (2001, p. 39)(Table 3). These fields reflect an intrapersonal component as
well as an interpersonal component. These two broad parts make up Goleman’s whole
construct, but then they are broken down further into specific competencies (2001, p.39):
1) Personal Competence: These abilities determine how we conduct ourselves.
a) Self-Awareness
• Emotional self-awareness: Reading one’s own emotions and recognizing their
impact; using “gut sense” to guide decisions
• Accurate self-assessment: Knowing one’s strengths and limits
• Self-confidence: A sound sense of one’s self-worth and capabilities
b) Self-Management
• Emotional self-control: Keeping disruptive emotions and impulses under control
• Transparency: Displaying honesty and integrity; trustworthiness
• Adaptability: Flexibility in adapting to changing situations or overcoming obstacles
• Achievement: The drive to improve performance to meet inner standards of
excellence
• Initiative: Readiness to act and seize opportunities
• Optimism: Seeing the upside in events
2) Social Competence: These abilities determine how we conduct relationships.
a) Social Awareness
• Empathy: Sensing others’ emotions, understanding their perspective, and taking
active interest in their concerns
• Organisational awareness: Reading the currents, decision networks, and politics
at the organisational level
• Service: Recognizing and meeting follower, client, or customer needs
b) Relationship Management
• Inspirational leadership: Guiding and motivating with a compelling vision
• Influence: Wielding a range of tactics for persuasion
• Developing others: Bolstering others’ abilities through feedback and guidance
• Change catalyst: Initiating, managing, and leading in a new direction
• Conflict management: Resolving disagreements
• Building bonds: Cultivating and maintaining a web of relationships
• Teamwork and collaboration: Cooperation and team building
This model (Table 3) is a refinement of the model that Goleman used in 1998. That earlier
framework (Table 2) identified five domains, or dimensions, of emotional intelligence
that comprised twenty-five competencies. Three dimensions—Self-Awareness, Self-
Regulation, and Motivation—depicted personal abilities, that is, knowing and conducting
emotions in oneself. Two dimensions—Empathy and Social Skills—depicted social
abilities, that is, knowing and conducting emotions in others. The current model reflects
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 367
recent statistical analyses by his colleague Richard Boyatzis that supported collapsing the
twenty-five competencies into twenty, and the five domains into the four ones (Boyatzis,
Goleman, & Rhee, 2000).
Certainly, these three main models are the substructure for other emotional intelligence
theories and constructs. Over passing decades, theorists have generated definition of
emotional intelligence. Namely, Zeidner et al. (2004) by analyzing of the literature,
mediate that there are two models of emotional intelligence as below:
1. Mental ability models: these models are concentrates on aptitude for processing
affective information, in which emotional intelligence is viewed as a well defined and
conceptually related collection of cognitive abilities for the processing of emotional
information and regulating emotion adaptively, namely Mayer and Salovey ability
model (1997).
2. Mixed models: in these models emotional intelligence conceptualize as a various
construct, including aspects of personality and ability to comprehend, assimilate,
understand and conduce emotions. These models include motivational factors and
drastic dispositions. Bar-On (1988-2000) and Goleman (1998) can be classified as
mixed models.
Discussion
Nature of organisations has been changed in the last two decades. Firms have been
decided to move toward knowledge, team-based and client-oriented vocations which
results in raising individuals’ autonomy at all levels of enterprises. Moreover criteria
of success have been changed in the workplace. Previously, individuals examined just
by their smart but in recent years organisations seek to hire individuals who are more
emotionally intelligence.
Based on review of the relevant literature, three main theories have been perceived in
emotional intelligence domain: (1) Bar-On (1988, 2000). His model framed emotional
intelligence in the concept of personality theory which in case of general model of
psychological well-being and adaption is best viewed (Goleman, 2001). (2) Mayer and
Salovey (1997). They derived a model base on cognitive consideration which outlines
the specific mental brilliance for distinguishing and culling emotions (Goleman, 2001).
(3) Goleman (1998). Goleman (1998) introduced his emotional intelligence model based
on performance theory (that is merit based) including a separable set of abilities that
integrate cognitive and affective skills.
Therefore, there are two broad categories of models in this area: (1) Ability model:
Matured by Mayer and Salovey that concentrates upon using intelligence to guide
emotions (Weinberger, 2004). Mayer and Salovey (1997) thought that emotional
intelligence is based initially in mental abilities. (2) Mixed model: Matured by Goleman
(1998) and Bar-On (1988), where emotional intelligence is not solely depending on
cognition, but upon a socio-emotion centered in personality traits. Definitely, Mayer
and Salovey focused upon the relevance between emotion and cognition, while Bar-On
concentrated upon emotional intelligence influences on performance and well being, and
Goleman relied on emotional intelligence as a competency model.
368 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
References
Armstrong, D. (2000). Emotions in Organizations. Paper presented ISPSO Symposium.
London.
Barchard, K. A., & Hakstian, A. R, (2004). The nature and measurement of emotional
intelligence abilities: basic dimensions and their relationships with other cognitive ability
and personality variables. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 437–62.
Bar-On, R. (1988). The development of a concept of psychological well-being.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rhodes University, South Africa.
Bar-On, R. (1997). Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i): Technical manual.
Toronto: Multi-Health System.
Bar-On, R. (2000). Emotional and social intelligence: Insights from the Emotional
Quotient Inventory (EQ-i). In R. Bar-On and J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), Handbook of
emotional intelligence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bar-On, R., & Parker, J. D. A. (2000). The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory: Youth
Version (EQ-i:YV) Technical Manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.
Boyatzis, R. E. (1999). Using tipping points of emotional intelligence and cognitive
competencies to predict financial performance of leaders. Psicothema, 18, 124-131.
Boyatzis, R. E., Goleman, D., & Rhee, K. (2000). Clustering competence in emotional
intelligence: Insights from the Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI) In R. Bar-On &
J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory, development,
assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace (pp. 343-362). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. (2002). Organizational behavior: Affect in the workplace.
Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 279-307.
Cherniss, C. (2000). Social and emotional competence in the workplace. In Bar-on, R.,
& Parker, J.D.A. (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence (pp. 433-458). San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cherniss, C. & Goleman, D. (1998). Bringing Emotional Intelligence to the Workplace.
Retrieved June 16, 2011 from www.eiconsortium.org.
Cooper, R.K., & Sawaf, A. (1997). Executive eq: Emotional intelligence in leadership
and organizations. New York, NY: Berkley Publishing Group.
Domagalski, T.A. (1999). Emotion in organizations, Human Relations, 52(6), 833-852.
Eriksson, C. B. (2004). The effects of change programs on employees’ emotions.
Personnel Review. 33(1), 110-126.
Fariselli, L., Ghini, M. and Freedman, J. (2006). Emotional Intelligence and Age.
Retrieved April 02, 2011, from http://www.6seconds.org/sei/ wp-age.php.
Fineman, S., (1993). Emotion in organizations, (Ed.) London: Sage Publications.
Fineman, S. (1996). Emotion and organizing. In C. Hardy, S. Clegg, & W. Nord (Eds),
Handbook of organization studies. London: Sage.
International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012 369
Fineman, S. (1997). Emotion and management learning. Management Learning, 28,
13-25.
Fineman, S. (2000). Emotions in organizations, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fisher, C. D., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2000). Emotions in organizations. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 21, 121–234.
Fleishman, E., & Harris, E. F. (1962). Patterns of leadership behaviour related to employee
grievances and turnover. Personnel Psychology, 15, 43-56.
French, R. (2001). Negative Capability: managing the confusing uncertainties of change.
Journal of Organizational Change Management, 14, 480-492.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Goleman, D. (2001). An EI-based theory of performance. In C. Cherniss & D. Goleman
(Eds.), The Emotionally Intelligent Workplace. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Jamali, D., Sidani, Y., & Abu-Zaki, D. (2008). Emotional Intelligence and management
development implications: Insights from the Lebanese context. Journal of Management
Development, (27) 3, 348 – 360.
Jordan, PJ & Troth, AC. (2002). Emotional intelligence and conflict resolution:
Implications for human resource development, in JL Callahan & EE McCollum (eds.),
Advances In Developing Human Resources, Special Edition Perspectives of Emotion
and Organizational Change, 4 (1), 62–79.
Kafetsios K, Zampetakis LA (2008). Emotional intelligence and job satisfaction: Testing
the mediatory role of positive and negative affect at work. Pers Indiv Differ, 44(3),
710-720.
Khalili, A. (2011). Gender Differences in Emotional Intelligence Among Employees of
Small and Medium Enterprise: An Empirical Study. Journal of International Management
Studies, 6(2), 184-193.
Kunnanatt, J. T. (2004). Emotional Intelligence: The New Science of Interpersonal
Effectiveness. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 15(4), 489-495.
Lynn, A. B. (2002). The Emotional Intelligence Activity Book. New York: HRD Press.
Muchinsky, P. M. (2000). Emotions in the workplace: The neglect of organizational
behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 801-805.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In I. P. Salovey &
D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence (pp. 3-13).
New York: Basic Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional
Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.
370 International Journal of Management Vol. 29 No. 3 Part 2 Sept 2012
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory,
findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 60, 197–215.
McClelland, David C. (1998). Identifying competencies with behavioral-event interviews.
Psychological Science, 9(5), 331-340.
Mumsford, M.D., Zaccaro, S.J., Harding, F.D., Owen Jacobs, T., & Fleishman, E.A.
(2000). Leadership skills for a changing world: Solving complex social problems.
Leadership Quarterly, 11(1), 11-35.
Murray, B. (1998). Does “emotional intelligence” matter in the workplace?
APA Monitor, 29(7).
Nikolaou, I. & Tsaousis, I. (2002). Emotional intelligence in the workplace: exploring
its effects on occupational stress and organizational commitment. International Journal
of Organizational Analysis, 10, 327-342.
Oginska- Bulik, N. (2005). Emotional intelligence in the workplace: exploring its effects
on occupational stress and health outcomes in human service workers. International
Journal Occupational Medicine and Environmental Health, 18(2), 167-75.
Pekrun, R. and Frese, M. (1992). Emotions in work and achievement, In: Cooper, C. L. and
Robertson, D. T. (Eds), International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
7, (pp. 153-200). St. Edmundsbury Press, John Wiley & Sons Ltd., Chichester.
Rozell, E. J., Pettijohn, C. E. & Parker, R. (2002). An empirical evaluation of emotional
intelligence: The impact on management development. Journal of Management
Development 21, 272-289.
Spencer, L. M. Jr., Spencer, S. M. (1993). Competence at work: Models for superior
performance. New York: Wiley.
Thi Lam L. & Kirby S.L. (2002). Is Emotional Intelligence and advantage? An exploration
of the impact of emotional and general intelligence on individual performance.
The Journal of Social Psychology, 142(1), 133-143.
Turnbull, S. (1999). Emotional labour in corporate change programmes: the effect
of organizational feeling rules on middle managers, Human Resources Development
International, 2(2), 125-146.
Weisinger, H. (1998). Emotional intelligence at work. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Weinberger, L. A. (2004). An examination of the relationship between emotional
intelligence and leadership style. Human Resource Development Review, 54(1),
1151-1158.
Wolfe, C.J., Caruso, D.R. (2004). Emotional intelligence. Shelton, CT: New Haven
Consulting Group.
Zeidner, M., Matthews, G. & Roberts, R. (2004). Emotional intelligence in the workplace:
a critical review, Applied Psychology. An International Review, 53(3), 371-99.
Contact email address: ashkankhalili@ymail.com
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE IN THE WORKPLACE: EXPLORING ITS EFFECTS ON …
Nikolaou, Ioannis;Tsaousis, Ioannis
International Journal of Organizational Analysis; 2002; 10, 4; ProQuest One Academic
pg. 327
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Linking emotional intelligence, spirituality and workplace …
Tischler, Len;Biberman, Jerry;McKeage, Robert
Journal of Managerial Psychology; 2002; 17, 3; ProQuest One Academic
pg. 203
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ABSTRACT
BROOKS, JONI KING. Emotional competencies of leaders: A comparison of
managers in a financial organization by performance level. (Under the direction of James
Burrow)
As companies increasingly are required to do more with less, seemingly “soft”
skills, based on emotions, are associated with leadership effectiveness and organizational
success. Research suggests that emotional “competencies,” such as those related to
empathy, adaptability, self-control, emotional self-awareness, ability to develop others, and
so forth, contribute significantly to leader
effectiveness.
The purpose of this study is to determine if differences in EI levels exist among high
performers in one organization. Specifically, the study assesses whether managers in this
financial organization who are deemed most effective according to their performance
ratings exhibit higher emotional intelligence competencies than managers with lower
performance ratings. The researcher also compared EI levels and various demographic
characteristics of the
sample.
The data were gathered from a sample of 57 from the study population—79 members
of the management team of a large regional financial organization selected to participate in
an internal study on emotional intelligence.
The participating managers completed self-
report versions of the Emotional Competence Inventory 2.0 (ECI 2.0) (Hay/McBer,
2002).
They, in turn, asked others with whom they work closely to complete 360-degree versions
of the instrument, providing feedback on the participants. Participants also completed an
instrument that gathered demographic data, including title, position, area, management
tenure, gender, educational level, and type of degree, and their most recent performance
ratings (three, four, or five on a five-point scale).
The researcher analyzed the ECI scores provided by the Hay Group, performance
ratings, and demographic data via a statistical analysis computing system, SAS 8. She
employed the general linear model (GLM) version of analysis of variance (ANOVA), T-
test, and union-intersection test methods.
Findings indicated that high ratings and high emotional intelligence were not
significantly related at a 95 percent confidence level. They were significantly related on
three competencies—Achievement, Adaptability, and Optimism—at a 90 percent
confidence level. The EI average scores of the sample differed significantly from the
average scores of other groups in the ECI North American Database on three competencies:
Achievement, Accurate Self-assessment, and Emotional Self-control. Statistically
significant differences between Sales and Support functions were evident in three
competencies: Empathy, Emotional Self-awareness, and Transparency. For all three,
Support EI levels were greater than Sales EI levels. Position, title, gender, and management
tenure did not
significantly impact the emotional
intelligence of the sample.
While educational level did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of
the sample, statistically significant differences existed between those with a Bachelor of
Science degree and those with a Bachelor of Arts degree on four competencies: Conflict
Management, Emotional Self-awareness, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness. EI
levels of those with BS degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BA degrees in
Conflict Management, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness while EI levels of those
with BA degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BS degrees on one competency:
Emotional Self-awareness.
Results of the self versus total others ratings indicated that statistically significant
differences exist in four competencies: Emotional Self-control, Influence, Inspirational
Leadership, and Self-confidence. For each of the four competencies, EI levels of total
others were greater than self-reported EI levels.
Emotional Competencies of Leaders:
A Comparison of Managers in a Financial Organization by
Performance Level
By
Joni King Brooks
A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty of North
Carolina State University in partial fulfillment of the
requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Education
Adult and Community College Education
Raleigh
2002
Approved by:
____________________________ _____________________________
Dr. James L. Burrow Dr. Paula Berardinelli
Chair of Advisory Committee
____________________________ _____________________________
Dr. John Pettitt Dr. Michael Vasu
UMI Number: 3073312
________________________________________________________
UMI Microform 3073312
Copyright 2003 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.
____________________________________________________________
ProQuest Information and Learning Company
300 North Zeeb Road
PO Box 1346
Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1346
ii
Biography
Joni King Brooks earned a bachelor’s degree in journalism, with a double major in
speech communications, at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (1981). She holds a
master’s degree in public administration from North Carolina State University (1992). Her
research interests include leadership and workplace performance.
Ms. Brooks works as a manager of public relations and marketing communications with
a telecommunications solutions provider. She has 20 years of experience in communications,
marketing, and public relations. Her career, which began in the news media, has encompassed
roles in a number of organizations, including those focused in non-profit, health-care, education,
research, banking, and high tech.
Ms. Brooks is the daughter of Carolyn Riggs King and the late William E. (Eddie)
King. She resides in Raleigh, N.C., with her husband, Ricky, and son, Austin.
iii
Acknowledgements
Along the long and curving road that I have traveled to complete this degree, I have
been very fortunate to have the support of numerous family members, friends, and
acquaintances. I sincerely appreciate all who have encouraged and assisted me.
In particular, I am indebted to the members of my advisory committee. Each has
offered support and guidance, helping me attain this ultimate academic goal. Dr. Michael Vasu
has been a part of my entire post-graduate academic life. He taught my very first graduate class
and served as my advisor during my master’s program. I sincerely appreciate his guidance and
support. Dr. Paula Berardinelli kindly stepped in to fill the void left by a committee member
who retired before I completed the
degree.
She has offered much encouragement when I was
discouraged and assistance, even trying to help me find a company to participate in my study
after one backed out far into the process. Dr. John Pettitt has served as a helpful advisor both as
an instructor and committee member.
Dr. James Burrow, the chair the advisory committee, has provided constant guidance
and aid. Dr. Burrow steered me through the doctoral requirements, helping me succeed along
the way. He gave willingly of his time and expertise, and his efforts are most appreciated.
My friends and family have provided support to enable me to achieve this goal. First, I
want to thank my parents, Carolyn Riggs King and the late William E. (Eddie) King, who
provided me with the foundation to achieve and instilled in me the value of education and
success. My parents taught me perseverance and to roll with the punches, reminding me that life
is not always fair. My mother is a source of continuous encouragement, support, and love. I
know my late father and late brother, Edwin, would be proud of this accomplishment.
I sincerely appreciate all who helped me identify a company to participate—which
proved to be an arduous task. I am very grateful to those in the participating organization who
contributed greatly to the completion of the research study.
iv
Finally, to my husband, Ricky, and son, Austin, thank you for loving, supporting, and
inspiring me. Ricky, you have given unselfishly and are always there to pick up the pieces,
enabling me to achieve this dream.
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
LIST OF TABLES ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………… IX
CHAPTER I…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..1
INTRODUCTION AND PROBLEM STATEMENT……………………………………………………………………………..1
Emotional Intelligence……………………………………………………………………………………………………..4
EI and IQ ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….6
EI and Leadership …………………………………………………………………………………………………………..7
PURPOSE OF THE STUDY ………………………………………………………………………………………………………..8
ANALYSIS …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..9
Research Questions and Hypotheses ………………………………………………………………………………….9
Applicability …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………15
TERMS DEFINED…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………16
RESEARCH DESIGN ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………..16
DELIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY…………………………………………………………………………………………….17
Population ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………18
Sample Selection and Identification of Managers ………………………………………………………………18
Gathering of Survey Information……………………………………………………………………………………..18
SUMMARY…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………19
CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE …………………………………………………………………20
ORGANIZATION OF THE LITERATURE …………………………………………………………………………………….20
LEADERSHIP………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………21
Leadership Behavior Theory …………………………………………………………………………………………..22
Leadership versus Management ………………………………………………………………………………………23
EMOTIONS…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………27
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE………………………………………………………………………………………………….29
vi
EI competencies…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….32
EI and EQ…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….34
Moving Ahead……………………………………………………………………………………………………………….35
EI History …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….36
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND LEADERSHIP ………………………………………………………………………..38
Johnson & Johnson Emotional Competencies and Leadership Study ……………………………………39
EI Training …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..41
EI AND DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS …………………………………………………………………………….43
EI and Functional and Position Differences ……………………………………………………………………..43
EI and Gender ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………43
EI AND PERFORMANCE IN THE FINANCE SECTOR …………………………………………………………………….45
MEASURING EI…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..45
COMPARING SELF AND OTHERS’ RATINGS……………………………………………………………………………..47
PERFORMANCE APPRAISAL AS A MEASURE OF EFFECTIVENESS …………………………………………………48
SUMMARY…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………49
CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY………………………………………………………………………………………..51
METHODS AND PROCESSES ………………………………………………………………………………………………….53
Research Design ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………53
Data Needs and Collection Steps……………………………………………………………………………………..53
Instrumentation……………………………………………………………………………………………………………..54
Reliability …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….56
Validity ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..58
Scale Design …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………60
PERFORMANCE APPRAISAL SYSTEM OF PARTICIPATING COMPANY ……………………………………………60
Sales Performance Analysis ……………………………………………………………………………………………61
Support Performance Analysis ………………………………………………………………………………………..62
Demographic Form ……………………………………………………………………………………………………….63
vii
Population and Sample…………………………………………………………………………………………………..64
Sample Selection and Identification of Managers ………………………………………………………………65
Gathering of Survey Information……………………………………………………………………………………..65
2002 Performance Summary …………………………………………………………………………………………..67
Data collection procedures……………………………………………………………………………………………..67
RESEARCH QUESTIONS, HYPOTHESES, AND DATA ANALYSIS……………………………………………………69
Analysis of Research Question One………………………………………………………………………………….69
Analysis of Research Question Two………………………………………………………………………………….71
Analysis of Research Question Three ……………………………………………………………………………….73
CHAPTER IV FINDINGS ……………………………………………………………………………………………………78
PURPOSE OF THE STUDY………………………………………………………………………………………………………78
RESEARCH QUESTIONS, NULL HYPOTHESES, AND ALTERNATIVE HYPOTHESES ……………………………..78
Research Question One ………………………………………………………………………………………………….79
Research Question Two ………………………………………………………………………………………………….81
ANALYSIS …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………85
Detailed Research Questions and Null and Alternative Hypotheses Review…………………………..87
2002 Performance Summary …………………………………………………………………………………………..88
ADDITIONAL FINDINGS: SELF VERSUS OTHERS RATINGS………………………………………………………….106
SUMMARY……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….108
CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS………………………113
SUMMARY……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….113
RESEARCH QUESTIONS AND HYPOTHESES ……………………………………………………………………………114
KEY FINDINGS………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….120
DISCUSSION OF KEY FINDINGS……………………………………………………………………………………………122
LIMITATIONS……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………128
RECOMMENDATIONS …………………………………………………………………………………………………………129
viii
RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ………………………………………………………………………131
FINAL COMMENTS…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….133
REFERENCE LIST……………………………………………………………………………………………………………135
APPENDIX A EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE LEADERSHIP COMPETENCIES. …………..143
APPENDIX B ECI ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………..147
APPENDIX C CHRONBACH’S ALPHA COEFFICIENTS…………………………………………………157
APPENDIX D TEST-RETEST STABILITY ……………………………………………………………………….158
APPENDIX E SALES APPRAISAL FORM………………………………………………………………………..159
APPENDIX F SUPPORT APPRAISAL CHECKLIST…………………………………………………………168
APPENDIX G DEMOGRAPHICS………………………………………………………………………………………173
APPENDIX H STATISTICAL ANALYSIS …………………………………………………………………………174
APPENDIX I CONSENT FORMS………………………………………………………………………………………190
ix
LIST OF TABLES
TABLE 2.1 EMOTIONS: CONVENTIONAL VS. HIGH-PERFORMANCE ………………………………………………………………28
TABLE 2.2 ADDITIONAL CONVENTIONAL VS. HIGH-PERFORMANCE EMOTIONS ……………………………………………..28
TABLE 4.1 MEAN SCORES FOR EACH COMPETENCY BY RATING…………………………………………………………………..89
TABLE 4.2 RELATIONSHIP OF HIGH RATINGS TO HIGH EI SCORES………………………………………………………………..91
TABLE 4.3 RELATIONSHIP OF EI COMPETENCIES AND PERFORMANCE RATINGS AT 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE ……92
TABLE 4.4 EI COMPARISONS OF BANK SAMPLE WITH OTHER SAMPLES…………………………………………………………94
TABLE 4.5 RELATIONSHIP OF EI COMPETENCIES AND POSITIONS AT 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE LEVEL ……………..95
TABLE 4.6 RELATIONSHIP OF EI COMPETENCIES AND TITLES AT 95-PERCENT CONFIDENCE LEVEL…………………..96
TABLE 4.7 RELATIONSHIP OF GENDER AND EI A 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE LEVEL ………………………………………..98
TABLE 4.8 MEANS SCORES BY GENDER IN DESCENDING ORDER ………………………………………………………………….99
TABLE 4.9 SIGNIFICANT RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN EI AND AREA……………………………………………………………..100
TABLE 4.10 SUPPORT MANAGERS’ HIGHEST- AND LOWEST-RATED COMPETENCIES ……………………………………..101
TABLE 4.11 SALES MANAGERS’ HIGHEST- AND LOWEST-RATED COMPETENCIES …………………………………………101
TABLE 4.12 COMPARISONS OF EI MEANS FOR SALES AND SUPPORT FUNCTIONS …………………………………………102
TABLE 4.13 RELATIONSHIP OF EI AND TENURE A 95-PERCENT CONFIDENCE LEVEL …………………………………….103
TABLE 4.14 RELATIONSHIP OF EI AND EDUCATIONAL LEVEL AT 95 PERCENT CONFIDENCE LEVEL…………………104
TABLE 4.15 EI SCORES SIGNIFICANTLY RELATED TO BA VERSUS BS DEGREES …………………………………………..106
TABLE 4.16 STATISTICALLY SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES ………………………………………………………………………….107
TABLE 4.17 EI COMPETENCY SCORES FOR SELF AND TOTAL OTHERS’ RATINGS FROM HIGHEST TO LOWEST …….107
TABLE 4.18 SUMMARY OF NULL AND ALTERNATIVE HYPOTHESIS TESTING…………………………………………………109
TABLE A1 SELF-AWARENESS COMPETENCIES ………………………………………………………………………………………..143
TABLE A2 SELF-MANAGEMENT COMPETENCIES……………………………………………………………………………………..144
TABLE A3 SOCIAL AWARENESS COMPETENCIES …………………………………………………………………………………….145
TABLE A4 RELATIONSHIP MANAGEMENT COMPETENCIES………………………………………………………………………..146
TABLE C CHRONBACH’S ALPHA COEFFICIENTS………………………………………………………………………………………157
TABLE D TEST-RETEST STABILITY COEFFICIENTS …………………………………………………………………………………..158
x
TABLE H1 ANOVA FOR RATINGS……………………………………………………………………………………………………….174
TABLE H2 ANOVA FOR POSITION ………………………………………………………………………………………………………176
TABLE H3 ANOVA FOR TITLES ………………………………………………………………………………………………………….178
TABLE H4 ANOVA FOR GENDER………………………………………………………………………………………………………..180
TABLE H5 ANOVA FOR AREA ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………182
TABLE H6ANOVA FOR TENURE …………………………………………………………………………………………………………184
TABLE H7 T-TEST FOR EDUCATIONAL LEVEL ……………………………………………………………………………………….186
TABLE H8 T-TEST FOR BA AND BS……………………………………………………………………………………………………..188
TABLE H9 T-TEST FOR SELF AND TOTAL OTHERS ……………………………………………………………………………………189
1
Chapter I
Introduction and Problem Statement
The times, they are still “a changing.” Companies today are very different from
those of the past. Globalization, increased competition, and technology advances are
requiring organizations, management teams, and employees to do more with less—while
retaining the flexibility to address changes.
According to Cherniss and Adler (2000), factors contributing to the turbulence
resulting from changes in the marketplace include technical innovation, global
competition, and pressures from institutional investors. Among technical innovations,
they cite organizational downsizing. “As organizations shrink, people who remain are
more accountable and more visible. They must interact with more peers, more
subordinates, and more customers” (p. 4).
The highly competitive environment driving today’s organizations has negative
effects. Cooper and Sawaf (1997) stated that the intellect required to make companies
competitive has resulted in dramatic costs to employees. The costs include “crumbling
trust, jarring uncertainty, greater distance between managers and those they manage,
stifled creativity, festering cynicism, increasingly volatile anger, and vanishing loyalty
and commitment” (pp. xi-xii).
Motivation, initiative, and cooperation also may be lacking among today’s employees
and may be resulting in less-than-effective organizations. Cherniss and Adler referred to
a 1991 study of American employers by Harris Education Research. Their findings
indicated that more than half of employees in the surveyed organizations “lacked the
2
motivation to continue learning and improving on the job. Four of 10 were not able to
work cooperatively with fellow employees, and just 19 percent of those applying for
entry-level jobs had enough discipline in their work habits” (p. 7).
Kouzes and Posner (1997) explained that both employees’ loyalties to institutions
and institutions’ loyalties to employees are diminishing. They added that the power
within organizations has shifted from the top brass to the people and to those with
technology and the skills to use it. Nearly half the population is cynical, and cynics don’t
participate in improving things. In such a climate, how can a leader possibly mobilize a
seemingly unwilling constituency (p. xviii)? They added, “With movement away from
the powerful bosses to the empowered people, organizational structures are flattening.
Everyone is connected, and knowledge has become synonymous with currency. Kouzes
and Posner contended that organizational success and the ability to compete are
“dependent upon the mental fitness of the workforce” (p. xviii).
Further, they explained, because there are far more products and services today
than were available a decade ago, the marketplace is fragmented. Organizations are
reducing staffs and, what was once a permanent workforce is changing to include
contingent and self-employed workers. “Loyalty and job security, we’re told, have gone
the way of the dodo bird” (p. xix).
However, the situation is not hopeless. “There’s a new search for meaning. With
today’s cynicism, fragmentation, and shifting relationships has come a yearning for a
greater purpose in our lives. … In the race of these new realities, there are countless
opportunities to make a difference. …More than ever, there’s a need for people to seize
these opportunities to lead us to greatness” (Kouzes and Posner, pp. xix-xx).
3
What separates the successful companies from others? According to Rodriguez,
Patel, Bright, Gregory, and Gowing (2002), a distinguishing element is superior
employees. “High-performing people are critical to high-performing organizations” (p.
309). Gordon (1998) added, “Enlightened companies now look for managers of
independent spirit, people with the courage of their convictions. Enlightened companies
want up-and-coming leaders who will speak their minds, boldly and openly” (p. 54).
Other sources, including Cherniss and Adler (2000), George (2000), and Goleman
(1998b), pointed to social and emotional competencies, such as feelings, emotions,
moods, and so forth—as differentiators in today’s competitive and continually changing
work environment. As a manager of a telecommunications company explained, “You
don’t compete with products alone anymore, but how well you use your people”
(Goleman, p. 7). Cherniss and Adler added that because of changes in the domestic and
global marketplace, “Personal qualities, such as resilience, optimism, and initiative, have
become more important in recent years” (pp. 2-3).
Goleman (1998b) contended that in today’s organizations, high performers are not
necessarily the most intelligent or most highly skilled employees. He wrote, “We’re
being judged by a new yardstick; not just by how smart we are, or by our training and
expertise, but also by how well we handle ourselves and each other. This yardstick is
increasingly applied in choosing who will be hired and who will not, who will be let go
and who retained, who passed over and who promoted” (p. 3).
Cherniss and Adler (2000) contended that because of a growing body of research
in competencies as well as personal experiences, employers are modifying their ideas
about what qualifications of an employee are most important. They cited results of a
4
survey conducted by the Department of Labor and American Society for Training and
Development. The survey sought to determine characteristics employers required for
entry-level positions. Results showed that the most important qualities were personal
management (self-esteem, goal setting and motivation, and personal and career
development); interpersonal skills (negotiation and teamwork); and organizational
effectiveness and leadership. The authors referenced a study by Dowd and Liedtka that
found that employers value the same competencies in entry-level managers. Results
indicated that qualities most desired in candidates with Master of Business
Administration degrees were communications skills, interpersonal skills, and initiative.
Identifying personnel possessing such specific qualities as these is not easy. It is
becoming increasingly challenging for organizations to recruit, select, and retain high-
performing employees (Rodriguez et al., 2002). The authors suggested that human
resources personnel consider competency-based programs: “Competencies provide the
foundation through which human resource professionals can contribute to the success of
their organizations” (p. 309).
While the concept of competencies has increased in popularity recently, the
concept is not new. In the 1970s, David McClelland’s research suggested that “academic
aptitude and knowledge content tests alone did not predict high job performance or
success in life, and that individual characteristics or competencies can identify high
performers” (Rodriguez, et al., 2002, p. 309).
Emotional Intelligence
Many have attempted to define the term emotional intelligence or to add their
descriptions to its definition. Some definitions focus on the skills or competencies of an
5
emotionally intelligent person, some have focused on behaviors, and some have focused
on intelligence.
Goleman has worked extensively in the competency area. He is well known for
his research on emotional intelligence. Since his best-selling book on emotional
intelligence in 1995, the term has come into increasingly popular use. Goleman (1998b)
introduced the concept of emotional intelligence and the role of emotions: “The new
measure takes for granted having enough intellectual ability and technical know-how to
do our jobs; it focuses instead on personal qualities, such as initiative and empathy,
adaptability, and persuasiveness” (p. 3). These “personal qualities” are included in the
emotional intelligence framework as EI competencies (See Tables A1-A4, Appendix A).
Goleman (1998b) described emotional intelligence as “the capacity for
recognizing our own feelings and those of others, for motivating ourselves, for managing
emotions well in ourselves and in our relationships” (p. 317). He continued, “Emotional
intelligence determines our potential for learning the practical skills that are based on its
five elements: self-awareness, motivation, self-regulation, empathy, and adeptness in
relationships” (p. 24). Goleman distinguished between emotional intelligence and
emotional competence. Emotional competence is a “learned capability based on
emotional intelligence that results in outstanding performance at work” (p. 24).
“Emotional competence shows how much of that potential we have translated into on-
the-job capabilities” (p. 25). For example, being effective at customer service is an
emotional
competency based on empathy.
According to Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (1998), with emotional intelligence
(EI), one is able to process emotional information, specifically as it involves the
6
perception, assimilation, understanding, and management of emotion. EI consists of four
branches of mental
ability:
1. Emotional identification, perception and expression
2. Emotional facilitation of thought
3. Emotional understanding
4. Emotional management
Cooper and Sawaf (1997) defined emotional intelligence in terms of energy,
relationships, and influence. It is “the ability to sense, understand, and effectively apply
the power and acumen of emotions as a source of human energy, connection, and
influence” (p.xiii). They contended that every business leader wants or needs emotional
intelligence. Subordinates want these qualities in a manager, and people hope for them in
a parent, partner, and sibling. They reported that emotional intelligence contributes to
employee and organizational success, citing examples in the following areas (p. xii):
Decision making
Leadership
Strategic and technical breakthroughs
Open honest communication
Trusting relationships and teamwork
Customer loyalty
Creativity and innovation
EI and IQ
Cacioppo and Gardner (1999) contended that emotion contributes not only to
intelligence but also to a fulfilling life. “The heightened ability to monitor one’s own and
7
others’ emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use the information to guide one’s
thinking and action has proven to be as important a determinant of life success as
traditional measures of intelligence such as IQ” (p.194).
Other theorists (Cooper and Sawaf, 1997; Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cherniss,
2000; and Druskat, 2001) also proposed that EQ is an important as IQ to ensuring
effective performance of workers and success in
organizations.
However, as Cherniss (2000) stated, it is absurd to suggest that cognitive ability is
not related to success in various disciplines and organizations. “We also should keep in
mind that cognitive and non-cognitive abilities are very much related. In fact, there is
research suggesting that emotional and social skills actually help improve cognitive
functioning” (p. 8). Cherniss and Adler (2000) added, “Emotional intelligence and
cognitive ability actually work together for effective action in organizations. Therefore, it
is not surprising that numerous studies point to EI as critical for success at work” (p. 5).
EI and
Leadership
Researchers (including Goleman, 1998b; George, 2002; Cavallo and Brienza,
2002; George and Bettenhausen, 1990; and George, 1995) have linked emotions and
competencies with performance and leadership.
Is high emotional intelligence synonymous with high performance? This was the
contention of Goleman (1998b), who has stated that with the recent emphasis on
flexibility, teams, and a strong customer focus in the workplace, “this crucial set of
emotional competencies is becoming increasingly essential for excellence in every job
and in every part of the world” (p. 29). The studies cited suggest that emotional
8
intelligence is related to the effectiveness and advancement of managers in today’s
organizations.
Purpose of the study
The purpose of this study is to determine if differences in EI levels exist among
high performers in one organization. Specifically, the study assesses whether managers in
this financial organization who are deemed most effective according to their performance
ratings exhibit higher emotional intelligence competencies than managers with lower
performance ratings. The researcher also compared EI levels and various demographic
characteristics of the sample, including position, title, area of employment, gender,
education level, type of degree, and management tenure.
In a study of emotional intelligence and leadership performance, it is important to
examine the concepts related to emotions, emotional intelligence, leadership, leadership
theories, and the
relationship between emotional intelligence and leadership.
The literature indicates that as companies increasingly are required to do more
with less, seemingly “soft” skills, based on emotions, are associated with leadership
effectiveness and organizational success. Research suggests that emotional
“competencies,” such as those related to empathy, adaptability, self control, emotional
self-awareness, and so forth, contribute significantly to leader effectiveness. This study
examines whether levels of emotional competencies are related to performance (as
defined by performance ratings) of managers in one organization.
9
Analysis
The analysis addressed research questions and null hypotheses posed to determine
if, in a sample, performance levels are related to levels of emotional intelligence; if EI
scores of the sample were similar to those of others surveyed using the same instrument;
and if various demographic characteristics of the sample (gender, position, title, sales or
support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree) are related to
emotional intelligence.
Research Questions and Hypotheses
Research Question One: Do high-performing leaders exhibit high emotional
intelligence?
Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18): High performance ratings and high scores on
each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies listed below are not related:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adapt
ability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Developing Others
(H0-8): Empathy
(H0-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-10): Emotional Self-control
10
(H0-11): Influence
(H0-12): Initiative
(H0-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-14): Organizational Awareness
(H0-15): Optimism
(H0-16): Self-confidence
(H0-17): Transparency
(H0-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
Alternative Hypotheses 1 (H1-1)-(H1-18): High performance ratings and high scores
on each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies are related:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Developing Others
(H1-8): Empathy
(H1-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-10): Emotional Self-control
(H1-11): Influence
(H1-12): Initiative
(H1-13): Inspirational Leadership
11
(H1-14): Organizational Awareness
(H1-15): Optimism
(H1-16): Self-confidence
(H1-17): Transparency
(H1-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
Research Question Two: How do emotional intelligence competency scores of the
sample compare with emotional intelligence competency scores of other samples
surveyed using the same survey instrument?
Null Hypotheses 2(H0-1)-(H0-16): There are no significant differences among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence he
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Empathy
(H0-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-9): Emotional Self-control
(H0-10): Influence
(H0-11): Initiative
(H0-12): Inspirational Leadership
12
(H0-13): Organizational Awareness
(H0-14): Optimism
(H0-15): Self-confidence
(H0-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
Alternative Hypotheses 2 (H1-1)- (H1-16): Significant differences exist among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Empathy
(H1-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-9): Emotional Self-control
(H1-10): Influence
(H1-11): Initiative
(H1-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-13): Organizational Awareness
(H1-14): Optimism
(H1-15): Self-confidence
(H1-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
13
Research Question Three: Do various demographic characteristics (position, title,
gender, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
influence scores on 18 emotional intelligence competencies?
Null Hypotheses 3 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor, manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18
competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 3 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor,
manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Null Hypotheses 4 (H0-1)-(H0-18): (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice president, area vice
president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 4 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice
president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
Null Hypotheses 5 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
men and women in the sample.
14
Alternative Hypotheses 5 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of
men and women in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
Alternative Hypotheses 7 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports
for more than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct
reports for fewer
than
five years.
Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers without
advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s degree).
15
Alternative Hypotheses 8 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers
without advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s
degree).
Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with Bachelor of
Arts degrees.
Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
Applicability
If findings from a study of emotional intelligence, such as this one, indicate that
performance levels are tied to EI levels, individuals and organizations may realize a
number of benefits. For individuals, the results of the Emotional Competence Inventory
employed in this study may serve as awareness tools for assessment and development,
providing precise, focused guidance on their emotional strengths in the workplace and
their priorities for improvement. For teams, departments, or business units, the ECI
provides information on emotional strengths in the workplace and priorities for
improvement. It can identify emotional intelligence gaps that might limit effectiveness
(Hay Group, 2002a).
16
If results do not indicate a relationship between performance levels and levels of
EI, individuals and organizations might choose to limit or restrict time and money
invested in programs to identify and enhance EI.
Terms Defined
Emotional intelligence: According to Daniel Goleman (1998b), emotional
intelligence is “the capacity for recognizing our own feelings and those of others, for
motivating ourselves, and for managing emotions well in ourselves and in our
relationships” (p. 317). It refers to 18 social and emotional abilities grouped in four core
areas (Self-awareness, Self-management, Social Awareness, and Relationship
Management) that previous research has indicated are linked to successful performance
in the workplace (Hay Group, 2002c; Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; Cherniss, 2000; and
Goleman, 1998b; George, 2000). Emotional intelligence is also known as called
emotional quotient (EQ).
Emotional competence: “A learned capability based on emotional intelligence that
results in outstanding performance at work” (Goleman, 1998b, p. 24).
Research design
The purpose of this study is to determine if differences in EI levels exist among
high performers in one sample organization. Specifically, the study assesses whether
managers in this financial organization who are deemed most effective according to their
performance ratings exhibit higher emotional intelligence competencies than managers
with lower performance ratings. The researcher also compared EI levels and various
demographic characteristics of the sample. The data were gathered from a sample of 57
17
from the population—79 members of the management team of a large regional financial
organization selected to participate in an internal study on emotional
intelligence.
The participating managers completed self-report versions of the Emotional
Intelligence Inventory 2.0 then asked others with whom they worked closely to rate them
by completing 360-degree versions of the instrument. Participants also completed forms
indicating their most recent performance ratings (three, four, or five on a five-point scale)
and demographical information, including title, position, area, management tenure,
gender, educational level, and type of degree.
The researcher employed a quantitative research design that provides a numeric
description of a fraction of the population—the sample—through the data collection
process of asking questions of people (Creswell, 1994). From the data collection, the
researcher might be able to generalize from a sample to a population in order to make
inferences about characteristics, attitudes, and behaviors of the population.
She analyzed EI scores provided by the Hay Group, performance ratings, and
demographic data via a statistical analysis computing system, SAS 8. The researcher
employed the general linear model (GLM) version of analysis of variance (ANOVA), T-
test, and union-intersection test methods.
Delimitations of the Study
The study is limited in scope; it includes several levels of managers in sales and
support functions in one large organization in the financial services industry. All of the
managers in the sample are high-performing or potentially high-performing managers
selected specifically to participate in an internal study on emotional competency. All
have had ratings of three or higher on a five-point scale for the past two review cycles.
18
Furthermore, performance ratings are based on a performance evaluation process
developed within the organization that has not been validated to discriminate between
high and low performers.
Population
The research study was conducted among managers in one financial organization,
a bank. Based in North Carolina, the bank has branches in three states. The sample of 57
managers included volunteers from the management pool of 79 selected to participate in
an internal study of emotional intelligence.
Sample Selection and Identification of Managers
The sample of 57 was drawn from a population of 79 managers participating in an
internal leadership symposium and/or an internal leadership program, according to the
manager of management and leadership development (personal communication,
November 18, 2002). Of the 79, 59 were asked to participate in the doctoral research
study. Twenty executive-level participants in the bank’s study did not have performance
ratings, and, therefore, did not qualify for participation in the doctoral research study.
Two of the 59 chose not to participate.
Gathering of Survey Information
Data for the study were gathered via the Emotional Competence Inventory 2.0, an
existing instrument. Study participants completed a “self-report” version of the
instrument. Peers, managers, direct reports, and customers evaluated each participant,
completing 360-degree instruments.
19
Summary
As competition in the workplace heightens, what separates highly successful
organizations from others? Many factors contribute to success in organizations. One
factor cited as contributing to success is effective leadership. What makes a leader
effective? As Goleman (1995, 1998b) and others (including Cherniss, 2000; Cherniss and
Adler, 2000; Cooper and Sawaf, 1997; Druskat, 2001; George, 2000) contend, an
effective leader has more than the technical skills to do a job. He or she employs
qualities, such as initiative and empathy, adaptability, and persuasiveness—competencies
of emotional intelligence. Researchers (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; George, 1995, 2002;
George and Bettenhausen, 1990; Goleman, 1998b) have found relationships between
emotions and competencies and performance and leadership. High EI in organizations
has been linked to improved financial results, improved return on investment in change
initiatives, improved productivity, increased retention of top talent, and increased sales
(Hay Group, 2002b).
This study investigates the EI competencies of high-performing managers in a
financial organization to determine if level of performance and demographic
characteristics are related to EI in the sample.
The research report is organized with the following sections:
1. Introduction and problem statement
2. Literature Review
3. Methodology
4. Findings
5. Summary, Conclusions, and
Recommendations
6. Appendices
20
Chapter II Review of the literature
As competition in the workplace heightens, what separates highly successful
organizations from others? Many factors contribute to success in organizations. One
factor cited as contributing to success is effective leadership. What makes a leader
effective? As Goleman (1995, 1998b) and others (including Cherniss, 2000; Cherniss and
Adler, 2000; Cooper and Sawaf 1997; Druskat, 2001; George. 2000) contend, effective
leaders have more than the technical skills to do a job. They employ qualities, such as
initiative and empathy, adaptability, and persuasiveness—competencies of emotional
intelligence. High EI has been linked to high performance by organizational personnel as
shown in a study at Johnson & Johnson (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002). High EI in
organization has been linked to improved financial results, improved return on
investment in change initiatives, improved productivity, increased retention of top talent,
and increased sales (Hay Group, 2002b).
The purpose of this study is to determine if differences in EI levels exist among
high performers in one organization. Specifically, the study assesses whether managers in
this financial organization who are deemed most effective according to their performance
ratings exhibit higher emotional intelligence competencies than managers with lower
performance ratings.
Organization of the Literature
In a study of emotional intelligence and leadership performance, it is important to
examine the concepts related to leadership, emotions, emotional intelligence, and the
relationship between emotional intelligence and leadership.
21
The literature indicates that as companies increasingly are required to do more
with less, seemingly “soft” skills, based on emotions, are associated with leadership
effectiveness and organizational success. Research suggests that emotional
“competencies,” such as those related to empathy, adaptability, self control, emotional
self-awareness, ability to develop others, and so forth, contribute significantly to leader
effectiveness.
In Chapter II, the researcher reviews literature on the following topics:
Leadership
Emotions
Emotional intelligence
Emotional intelligence and leadership
Emotional intelligence and demographic characteristics
Emotional intelligence and performance in the finance sector
Measuring EI
Comparing self and others’ ratings
Performance appraisal as a measure of effectiveness
Leadership
What characterizes an effective leader? Researchers have attempted to define the
concept only to realize that the term means different things to different people. There are
nearly as many definitions of leadership as there are people who have tried to define the
concept (Yukl, 1994).
Yukl (1994) further explained, “Leadership has been defined in terms of
individual traits, behaviors, influence over other people, interaction patterns, role
22
relationships, occupations or an administrative position, and perception by others
regarding legitimacy of influence” (p. 2). Most definitions include an assumption that
leadership involves a process of social influence whereby one person exerts intentional
influence over other people to structure the activities and relationships in a group or
organization.
Successful leaders are typically adaptable to situations, alert to social
environment, ambitious and achievement-oriented, assertive, cooperative, decisive,
dependable, dominant (desire to influence others), energetic (high-activity level),
persistent, self-confident, tolerant of stress, and willing to assume responsibility (Yukl,
1994).
Leadership Behavior Theory
Research has uncovered various leadership styles (patterns of behavior).
According to Hampton, Summer, and Webber (1987):
…leaders’ actions fall into two broad categories, actions that get the work out
(leadership of things) and actions that support and encourage subordinates
(leadership of people). Some social scientists believe that one human being cannot
really be an excellent performer at both, and that a given person tends to be task-
oriented or people-oriented in his or her actions. Others insist that the leader not
only can but must be an excellent performer on both scales. At least one author
gave us a more specific and concrete list of action leaders take to ‘lead’ both
things and people (p. 579).
23
Leadership versus Management
Similar to Hampton, Summer, and Webber’s (1987) description of what leaders
do, theorists (Batten, 1989; Capezio and Morehouse, 1997; Kotter, 1998; Kouzes and
Posner, 1997; Zaleznik, 1998) differentiated between management and leadership.
Management is more about completing tasks while leadership involves influence and
relationships.
Kotter (1998) explained: “Leadership is different from management….
Leadership and management are two distinctive and complementary systems of action.
Each has its own function and characteristic activities. Both are necessary for success in
today’s business environment. Management is about coping with complexity. Its practices
and procedures are largely a response to the emergence of large, complex organizations
in the twentieth century. Leadership, by contrast, is about coping with change”(p. 37).
Zaleznik (1998) agreed that managers and leaders are very different. “Managers’
goals arise out of necessities rather than desires; they excel at diffusing conflicts between
individuals or departments, placating all sides while ensuring that an organization’s day-
to-day business gets done. Leaders, on the other hand, adopt personal, active attitudes
towards goals. They look for the potential opportunities and rewards that lie around the
corner, inspiring subordinates and firing up the creative process with their own energy.
Their relationships with employees and coworkers are intense, and their working
environment is often, consequently, chaotic” (p. 61). To succeed, businesses need both
managers and leaders and must find ways to train excellent managers and simultaneously
develop leaders.
24
Batten (1989) contrasted managers and leaders: “Managers manage inventories,
supplies, data. They are number crunchers. Leaders catalyze, stretch, and enhance people.
They provide transcendent goals, creating a motivational climate. Managers push and
direct. Leaders pull and expect. Leaders are exhilarated by identifying and enhancing
their people’s strengths” (p. 2).
According to Tom Peters, “Management is mostly about ‘to-do’ lists. Leadership
is about tapping the wellsprings of human motivation—and about fundamental relations
with one’s fellows” (Kouzes and Posner, 1997, p. xvi). Capezio and Morehouse (1997)
summed up the differences: “Management is keen on doing things right. Leadership is
more concerned with doing the right things” (p.3).
Blank (1995) explained that the word leadership “inspires images that range from
a power for positive change to a force that can misdirect to a capacity that is often absent
when we need it most” (p. 1). Blank added that determining what constitutes leadership is
difficult at best. “The same leadership approach can work in very different contexts, but a
leadership strategy that works in one particular situation may not work again at another
time under the same conditions” (p. 9).
Capezio and Morehouse (1997) defined leadership as “the ability to influence
individuals or groups to think, feel and take positive action to achieve goals” (p. 1). They
contrasted recent and former views of leadership, explaining that the concept previously
was viewed as “direction from the top” (p. 1). They added that work is not completed at
the top of organizations. They continued, “Leadership comes from a wellspring of talent
and commitment among workers close to the customer and to the work. We call this kind
of leadership self-directed. Self-directed leadership challenges every team member—
25
regardless of level—to help solve problems, improve quality, increase market share and
create the kind of work environment that encourages people to do their best” (p.1).
Kouzes and Posner (1997) contended that “Leadership isn’t the private reserve of
a few charismatic men and women. It’s a process ordinary people use when they’re
bringing forth the best from themselves and others. Liberate the leader in everyone, and
extraordinary things happen” (p. xx).
What do leaders do? Among other responsibilities, Truskie (1999) explains that
leaders perform two tasks:
1. Establish Organizational Direction. Where is the organization headed? And how
is it going to get there? It is the leader’s responsibility to establish direction by
determining vision, mission, business strategy, objectives, and goals.
2. Develop Organizational Effectiveness. Once direction is determined, the
strength, stamina, competencies, and agility of the organization must be
developed by the leader. This enables the organization to serve its mission,
achieve its objectives, meet its goals, and move in its intended direction (p. 109).
Blank contended that leaders and leadership are different. “When people say, ‘We
need better leadership,’ they typically mean, ‘We need a different leader.’ However,
‘leadership’ represents something more than the leader alone; it encompasses the leader
and the follower together” (p.12). He added, “Leadership is not a person, a position, or a
program but a relationship or field of interaction that occurs when the leader and the
follower connect” (Blank, 1995, p.12). Blank posited nine natural laws of leadership (pp.
10-11):
1. A leader has willing followers—allies.
26
2. Leadership is a field of interaction-relationship between leaders and followers—
allies.
3. Leadership occurs as an event.
4. Leaders use influence beyond formal authority.
5. Leaders operate outside the boundaries of organizationally defined procedures.
6. Leadership involves risk and uncertainty.
7. Not everyone will follow a leader’s initiative.
8. Consciousness—information-processing capacity creates leadership.
9. Leadership is a self-referral process. Leaders and followers process information
from their own subjective, internal frame of reference.
According to Batten (1989), one of the most consistent characteristics of high-
performing people is the “ability to take a hit in stride and bounce back” (p. 17). Batten
contended that leadership is finally being recognized as a differentiator between success
and failure in organizations. “It is finally being perceived by large and small
organizations alike that a company’s profit and future are only as strong as its corporate
culture. And an excellent corporate culture can be envisioned and built only by excellent
leaders” (p.17).
It is important to note that different organizations and industries have unique
economic and technological characteristics (Yukl, 1994). Thus, they require different
types of leaders. Even within the same type of organization or the same organization,
skills required of successful leaders may change over time. “The skills needed by an
entrepreneurial manager to build a new organization are not identical to the skills needed
by the chief executive of a large, established organization. The skills needed to lead an
27
organization with a stable, supportive environment are not identical to the skills needed to
lead an organization facing a turbulent, competitive environment” (p. 277).
Many (including Cacioppo and Gardner, 1999; Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cooper
and Sawaf, 1997; Fineman, 1993; and Forgas, 1995; George, 2000; Goleman 1995,
1998b) have proposed that feelings—moods and emotions—are critical in the leadership
process.
Emotions
Emotions are the primary sources of “motivation, information (feedback),
personal power, innovation, and influence. …they inspire and enliven good judgment and
reasoning and are linked to success and profitability. …everything important that happens
to us arouses emotion. Everything” (Cooper and Sawaf, 1997, p. xv). Emotion is
“applying movement, either metaphorically or literally, to core feelings. …Emotions have
long been considered to be of such depth and power that in Latin, for example, they were
described as motus anima, meaning literally ‘the spirit that moves us’” (p. xiii).
Cooper and Sawaf (1997) explained that contrary to what most people believe,
emotions are inherently neither positive nor negative. Emotions:
Serve as the single most powerful source of human energy, authenticity, and
drive, and can offer us a wellspring of intuitive wisdom. In fact, feelings provide
us with vital and potentially profitable information every minute of the day. This
feedback—from the heard, not the head—is what ignites creative genius, keeps
you honest with yourself, shapes trusting relationships, provides an inner compass
for your life and career, guides you to unexpected possibilities, and may even
save your or your organization from disaster… (p. xiii).
28
Cooper (Table 2.1) expanded on the work of the American Quality Foundation,
which describes conventional and high-performance meaning of emotions (Cooper and
Sawaf, 1997 p. xxxii):
Table 2.1 Emotions: Conventional vs. high-performance
Conventional High-performance
Sign of weakness Sign of strength
No place in business Essential in business
Avoid emotions Emotions trigger learning
Confuse Explicate (clarify)
Table them Integrate them
Avoid emotional people Seek out emotional people
Pay attention only to thoughts of Listen for the emotion in
Use of non emotional words Use of emotional words
Cooper (Cooper and Sawaf, 1997) added the following (Table 2.2) to the list:
Table 2.2 Additional conventional vs. high-performance emotions
Conventional High Performance
Interfere with good judgment Essential to good judgment
Distract us Motivate us
Sign of vulnerability Make us real and alive
Obstruct, or slow down, reasoning Enhance, or speed up, reasoning
Form a barrier to control Build trust and connection
Weaken fixed attitudes Activate ethical values
Inhibit the flow of objective data Provide vital information and
feedback
Complicate management planning Spark creativity and innovation
Undermine authority Generate influence without authority
29
Emotional Intelligence
What is emotional intelligence and why is it important? Many have attempted to
define and add their descriptions to the term, emotional intelligence. Some definitions of
emotional intelligence focus on the skills or competencies of an emotionally intelligent
person, some focus on behaviors, and some focus on intelligence.
Goleman has worked extensively in the competency area. He is renowned for his
research in “emotional intelligence.” Since the publishing a best-selling book on the topic
by Goleman in 1995, the term as become increasingly popular. Goleman (1998b)
introduced the concept of emotional intelligence and the role of emotions: “The new
measure takes for granted having enough intellectual ability and technical know-how to
do our jobs; it focuses instead on personal qualities, such as initiative and empathy,
adaptability, and persuasiveness” (p. 3). The “personal qualities” are included in the
emotional intelligence framework as EI competencies (See Table A, Appendix A).
Goleman (1998b) described emotional intelligence as “the capacity for
recognizing our own feelings and those of others, for motivating ourselves, for managing
emotions well in ourselves and in our relationships” (p. 317). He continued, “Emotional
intelligence determines our potential for learning the practical skills that are based on its
five elements: self-awareness, motivation, self-regulation, empathy, and adeptness in
relationships” (p. 24). Goleman distinguished emotional intelligence and emotional
competence. Emotional competence is a “learned capability based on emotional
intelligence that results in outstanding performance at work” (p. 24). “Emotional
competence shows how much of that potential we have translated into on-the-job
30
capabilities” (p. 25). For example, being effective at customer service is an emotional
competency based on empathy.
According to Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (1998), with EI, one is able to process
emotional information, specifically as it involves the perception, assimilation,
understanding, and management of emotion. EI consists of four branches of mental
ability:
1. Emotional identification, perception, and expression
2. Emotional facilitation of thought
3. Emotional understanding
4. Emotional management
Cooper and Sawaf (1997) defined emotional intelligence in terms of energy,
relationships, and influence. It is “the ability to sense, understand, and effectively apply
the power and acumen of emotions as a source of human energy, connection, and
influence” (p. xiii). They contended that every business leader wants or needs emotional
intelligence. Subordinates want these qualities in a manager and people hope for them in
a parent, partner, and sibling. They added that
emotional intelligence:
Motivates us to pursue our unique potential and purpose, and activates out
innermost values and aspirations, transforming them from things we think about
to what we live. …Emotional intelligence requires that we learn to acknowledge
and value feelings—in ourselves and others—and that we appropriately respond
to them, effectively applying the information and energy of emotions in our daily
life and work (p. xiii).
31
Abraham (1999) described emotional intelligence as “the accurate appraisal and
expression of emotion both in the self and in others” (p.210). She added that emotional
intelligence also is “the adaptive regulation of emotion” and the “ability to use emotional
knowledge to solve problems” (p. 210). Cherniss (2000) contended that EI enables one to
know when and how to express emotion as well as control it.
Cooper and Sawaf (2002) reported that emotional intelligence contributes to
success in careers and organizations, citing examples in the following areas (p. xii):
Decision making
Leadership
Strategic and technical breakthroughs
Open honest communication
Trusting relationships and teamwork
Customer loyalty
Creativity and innovation
Cherniss and Adler (2000) continued: Emotional intelligence is the basis for
personal qualities such as realistic self-confidence, personal integrity, knowledge of
personal strengths and weaknesses, resilience in times of change or adversity, self
motivation, perseverance, and the knack for getting along well with others (p. 1).
According to Cooper and Sawaf (1997), emotional intelligence can enhance
reasoning and better channel the energy of emotions:
The exponentially growing science of Emotional Intelligence, bolstered by
hundreds of research studies and management reports, is teaching us every day
how to enhance our reasoning capacities and, at the same time, to make better use
32
of the energy of our emotions, the wisdom of our intuition, and the power inherent
in our ability to connect at a fundamental level with ourselves and those around us
(p. xii).
EI competencies
Based on a review of approximately 200 competency models, Goleman (1998b)
identified 25 social and emotional competencies that most accurately predict superior
performance in many occupations. He organized the competencies into five dimensions
of EI: self-awareness, self-regulation, self-motivation, social awareness, and social skills
(Goleman, 1995). Cherniss and Adler (2000) added that research employing the
Emotional Competence Inventory, a measure of emotion intelligence, by Boyatzis,
Goleman, and Rhee (2002), led to a refined version of the original model. The
competency framework consists of four dimensions (Self-awareness, Self-management,
Social Awareness, and Relationship Management) and 18 competencies, including
Empathy, Achievement Orientation, Optimism, Self-confidence, and Adaptability
(Boyatzis, Goleman, and McKee, 2002, pp. 253-256) (See Table A, Appendix A). The
competencies were distilled in large part from studies by the U.S. Office of Personnel
Management (1996); Spencer and Spencer (1993); and Rosier and Jeffrey (1994,
1995).
Many of the traits described in the literature on leadership are included or implied
in the ECI competency framework based on the work of Boyatzis, Goleman and McKee
(2002). For example, achievement-orientation, inspiration, adaptability and self-
confidence are listed specifically. Others are implied within framework (such as
cooperative, which is related to the characteristics listed in teamwork and collaboration
33
cluster; coping with change (adaptable); encourage (influence); opportunistic (optimism);
motivation (developing others and achievement); persistence (achievement);
and so forth.
According to Goleman (1998b), key competencies match a particular
organization’s “reality.” “Each company and each industry has its own emotional
ecology, and the most adaptive traits for workers will differ accordingly” (p.29). He
contended that the results of nearly 300 company-sponsored studies show that “across a
wide array of jobs, the recipe for excellence gives far more weight to emotional
competencies than to cognitive abilities” (p. 29).
However, there is no recipe of competencies that works in every situation. “The
same competencies can make people excel in different jobs” (Goleman, 1998b, p. 2
8).
Additionally, “The competencies one needs for success may change as one rises through
the ranks; in most large organizations, senior executives need a greater degree of political
awareness than middle managers. Specific positions require specific skills. For the best
nurses, it’s a sense of humor; for bankers, respecting customers’ confidentiality,” (p. 28)
and so forth.
Further, having high emotional intelligence does not guarantee that a person has
learned the emotional competencies that matter for work; it means that the person has
excellent potential to learn them (Goleman, 1998b). For example, a person might be
highly empathic but has “not learned the skills based on empathy that translate into
superior customer service, top-flight coaching or mentoring, or the ability to bring
together a diverse work team” (p. 25).
34
EI and EQ
Cacioppo and Gardner (1999) contended that emotion contributes not only to
intelligence but also to a fulfilling life. “The heightened ability to monitor one’s own and
other’s emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use the information to guide one’s
thinking and action has proven to be as important a determinant of life success as
traditional measures of intelligence such as IQ” (p.194).
Other theorists (Cherniss, 2000; Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cooper and Sawaf,
1997; and Druskat, 2001) proposed that one’s Emotional Quotient (EQ) is an important
as Intelligence Quotient (IQ) to ensuring effective performance of managers and the
organizations in which they work.
Cherniss and Adler (2000) explained that more people now appreciate the
importance of EI for individual and organizational effectiveness. However, for many
years, most people assumed it was cognitive ability that mattered most. A growing body
of research indicates that cognitive ability, while important, is not the major contributor
to success. “No longer do they [employers] emphasize technical skills and raw
intelligence alone. Employers increasingly recognize that, given sufficient cognitive
ability and technical skills to hold the job, social and emotional competencies matter
most.” The authors continued, “In fact, IQ accounts for at most 25 percent of the
variance in individual success (p.6). It may account for less than five percent: “IQ may
be related to as little as 4 percent of real-world success. In other words, over 90 percent
may be related to other forms of intelligence” (Cooper and Sawaf, 1997, p. xxv).
Technical expertise adds to success, but even cognitive ability and technical expertise
35
leave much to be explained. “Thousands of bright, technically able individuals fail to
reach their potential because they lack sufficient EI” (Cherniss and Adler, 2000, p. 4).
Druskat (2001) added that many executives accept that EI is as crucial as IQ to an
individual’s effectiveness. She pointed out that most studies have centered on emotional
intelligence as an individual competency even though most work is completed in teams.
She proposed that emotional intelligence is critical to a group’s effectiveness and that
teams can enhance EI and, thereby, increase overall performance.
However, as Cherniss (2000), stated that it is absurd to suggest that cognitive
ability is not related to success in various disciplines and organizations. “We also should
keep in mind that cognitive and non-cognitive abilities are very much related. In fact,
there is research suggesting that emotional and social skills actually help improve
cognitive functioning” (p.8). Cherniss and Adler (2000) added, “Emotional intelligence
and cognitive ability actually work together for effective action in organizations.
Therefore, it is not surprising that numerous studies point to EI as critical for success at
work” (p.5).
Moving Ahead
Cooper and Sawaf (1970) suggested that IQ will become less and less of a factor
used to gauge intelligence. They explained even though IQ and mathematical models
have guided much of the organizational design to date, there are suggestions that
“emerging models of organizational intelligence will be based far more on the principles
of EQ and biological systems. Accordingly, it will treat people, markets, ideas, and
organizations as unique and alive, generative and interactive, and inherently capable of
change, learning, growth, inspiration, creativity, synergy, and transformation” (p. xxviii).
36
Cooper and Sawaf (1997) contended, “We are in the beginning stages of what
many authorities believe will be the next revolution in business. By design, no blood will
be shed in this sweeping transformation from old to new, just a host of preconceived
notions” (p.xi). They continued:
It all began with a series of studies on Emotional Intelligence indicating that
people who are intellectually the brightest are often not the most successful,
whether in business or in their personal lives. Over the past several years, EQ has
become widely accepted as a shorthand expression for the Emotional Intelligence
equivalent of IQ. Emerging research suggests that a technically proficient
executive or professional with a high EQ is someone who picks up more deftly,
and more quickly than others the budding conflicts that need resolution, the team
and organizational vulnerabilities that need addressing, the gaps to be leaped or
filled, the hidden connections that spell opportunity and the murky, mysterious
interactions that seem most likely to prove golden—and profitable (p. xi).
EI History
According to Cherniss (2000), originally psychologists focused on cognitive
aspects of intelligence, such as memory and problem solving. There were, however,
researchers who recognized early on that non-cognitive aspects were important, too.
Much of the earlier work was forgotten until 1983, when Howard Gardner began to write
about multiple intelligences. Gardner (1983) suggested that intrapersonal and
interpersonal intelligence is as important as the type of intelligence measured by IQ and
similar tests.
37
Gardner (1993) began a study in the 1970s that culminated in 1983 in the
publishing of a book, Frames of mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligence, that
introduced the theory of multiple intelligences. He used the term multiple intelligences to
describe “an unknown number of human capacities, ranging from musical intelligences to
the intelligence involved in understanding oneself; ‘intelligences’ to underscore that these
capacities were as fundamental as those historically captured within the IQ test” (pp. xi-
xii). Gardner (1993) described seven intelligences:
1. Linguistic intelligence—the type exhibited best by poets
2. Logical-mathematical intelligence—logical, mathematical, and scientific
ability
3. Spatial intelligence—ability to form a mental model of a spatial world
4. Musical intelligence—the type exhibited by famous composers and musicians
5. Bodily-kinesthetic intelligence—ability to solve problems or design products
using one’s whole body
6. Personal intelligence—ability to understand other people
7. Intrapersonal intelligence—capacity to form an accurate, veridical model of
oneself and to be able to use that model to operate effectively in life
Cherniss (2000) added that by the early 1990s, there was a great deal of research
on the role of non-cognitive factors in helping people succeed in life and in the
workplace.
In 1990, Salovey and Mayer (1990) coined the term: emotional intelligence,
which they described in terms of being able to monitor and control one’s own and others’
feelings and to make use of the feelings to guide thought and action. In the early 1990s,
38
Daniel Goleman took an interest in the work of Salovey and Mayer, which led, in 1995,
to the publishing of a book, Emotional Intelligence, which became an international best
seller (Cherniss, 2000).
The concept has continued to spark interest and research in business, education,
and other settings. Cherniss (2000) contended that since Goleman’s book was published
in 1995:
Emotional intelligence has become one of the hottest buzzwords in corporate
America. For instance, when the Harvard Business Review published an article
on the topic two years ago, it attracted a higher percentage of readers than any
other article published in that periodical in the last 40 years. When the CEO of
Johnson & Johnson read that article, he was so impressed that he had copies sent
out to the 400 top executives in the company worldwide (p.3).
Emotional Intelligence and Leadership
Many approaches to leadership have been proposed—researchers have analyzed
what leaders are like, what they do, how they motivate followers, how their styles relate
to situations, and how they can make major changes in their organizations (George,
2000). Although the role of emotions in the leadership process is not often explored,
some earlier leadership approaches, such as the trait approach, described particular
leadership skills or traits that may be subsumed in or partially overlap emotional
intelligence.
While most studies have been fairly recent, Cherniss (2000) cited studies from as
far back as the 1940s as linking emotional characteristics and leadership. He contended
39
that the Ohio State Leadership Studies surmised that “consideration” was an important
aspect of effective leadership. The research suggested that leaders who are able to
establish “mutual trust, respect, and a certain warmth and rapport” with members of their
group would be more effective.
Studies in 1990 and 1995 indicated that leaders’ feelings/emotions may play an
important role in the leadership process. George and Bettenhausen (1990) found that the
extent to which leaders of existing work groups experienced upbeat moods was positively
related to levels of prosocial behavior performed by group members and negatively
related to group turnover rates. George (1995) found that work groups, led by sales
managers who tended to have positive moods at work, delivered higher quality customer
service than groups led by managers who did not tend to experience positive moods at
work. She contended that it is likely that a range of feelings (emotions and moods)
influences leadership effectiveness.
Johnson & Johnson Emotional Competencies and Leadership Study
A study of emotional competencies and leadership excellence in Johnson &
Johnson’s Personal Care Group sought to determine if there were certain leadership
competencies that distinguished high performers from average performers (Cavallo and
Brienza, 2002). The researchers compared the ratings of groups defined by region,
gender, function, and performance and potential. To separate high performers from
average performers, the researchers relied on performance ratings and the potential of
participants for taking on added responsibility in the organization “Both the performance
rating and potential code are success indicators within the organization and determine
promotion, compensation, and position in succession planning schemes” (p. 2).
40
The study found a strong relationship between superior performing leaders and
emotional competence, supporting theorist’s suggestions that the social, emotional, and
relational competency set commonly referred to as Emotional Intelligence, is a
distinguishing factor in leadership performance. According to Cavallo and Brienza
(2002), the outcome is consistent with conclusions reached by McClelland (1998), in a
study of leaders in 30 organizations. McClelland determined that the most powerful
leadership differentiators were self-confidence, achievement drive, developing others,
adaptability, influence, and leadership.
Goleman (1998b) had access to competence models for 181 positions from 121
companies and organizations worldwide, with combined workforces in the millions. The
models illustrated characteristics that management in the organizations agreed captured
the profile of excellence for a specific position. He compared which competencies
deemed critical to a specific position, role, or field, which could be classified as purely
cognitive or technical skills; and which were emotional competencies. He found that 67
percent of the abilities considered essential for effective performance were emotional
competencies. He commissioned Hay/McBer to conduct an independent study to test his
results. Hay/McBer found that emotional competencies were twice as important to
effective performance as pure intellect and expertise.
In a 2000 study, George proposed that four elements of emotional intelligence
(the ability to understand and manage moods and emotions in the self and others)—
appraisal and expression of emotion; use of emotion to enhance cognitive processes and
decision making; knowledge about emotions, and management of emotions—contribute
to effective leadership in organizations.
41
Cherniss and Adler (2000) explained that while some studies have linked high EI
levels with superior performance, other studies have shown what happens when people
lack EI competencies. For instance, a study at the Center for Creative Leadership looked
at reasons why the careers of certain executives had derailed. The reason for their failure
was not a lack of cognitive ability. “The two most common traits of those who failed
were rigidity and poor relationships” (p. 6).
The next generation of workers may exhibit lower EI levels than the levels
exhibited by today’s workers. “The next generation of workers now entering the
workplace is less likely than previous generations to possess the social and emotional
qualities that are essential for effective performance. This trend represents a major
challenge for employers and for HR professionals in particular. It is no wonder that HR
personnel increasingly are being called upon to provide training—not just in technical
skills—but also in social and emotional competencies that are essential for success”
(Cherniss and Adler, 2000, p. 7).
EI Training
Emotional intelligence can be learned (Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cooper and
Sawaf, 1997; Goleman, 1998b; Sala, 2001). Cherniss and Adler (2000) cited best-practice
guidelines for what makes an EI training program effective and described model
programs that include elements from the guidelines. They described 14 programs that met
specified criteria (related to replication, sample size, control group, outcome measures,
and multiple data points) including:
Achievement motivation training developed by David McClelland
Emotional competency training developed at American Express Financial Advisors
42
Leaderlab developed at the Center for Creative Leadership
Weatherhead School of Management Program developed at Case Western Reserve
University’s Weatherhead School of Management
Sala (2002) reported on results of studies in which two groups were assessed with
the Emotional Competency Inventory twice, once before and once after participating in a
Mastering Emotional Intelligence workshop. In general, scores in the first sample were
higher upon reassessment, suggesting that EI improved. Results of second sample were
even more robust; scores were significantly higher following reassessment on 19 of 20
competencies.
Goleman (1998b) offered guidelines for learning emotional intelligence (p. 251-253):
Assess the job
Assess the individual
Deliver assessments with care
Gauge readiness
Motivate
Make change self-directed
Focus on clear, manageable goals
Prevent relapse
Give performance feedback
Encourage practice
Arrange support
Provide models
Encourage
Reinforce change
Evaluate
43
EI and Demographic Characteristics
Studies have been conducted to determine correlations between EI scores and
various demographic characteristics, such as gender, function, age, job function, and job
level (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; Goleman, 1998b; Sala, 2002).
EI and Functional and Position Differences
One example is a study by Cavallo and Brienza (2002) that found some
significant differences among participants in various functional areas at Johnson &
Johnson; however, they reported that the number of participants in each functional
category
was not sufficient to draw
conclusions.
Sala (2002) reported that a study of emotional intelligence and job characteristics
found that participants with higher-level positions (senior-level manager versus entry-
level manager) rated themselves higher on the ECI than those with lower-level positions.
Results indicated that there no relationship between job level and total others’ ratings on
the ECI.
EI and Gender
The literature on gender and EI is mixed. While some emotion- and EI-related
studies (Hart, 2002; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; Sala, 2002) have noted gender
differences, others (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; Eagly, Karau, and Makhijani, 1992,
1995; Landau, 1996; Ragins, 1991) have found little or no differences between the
sexes.
Even though Cavallo and Brienza (2002) found many differences in performance
and potential in the Johnson & Johnson study, they unveiled few gender differences.
Analysis of peer ratings found differences in five emotional competencies: Emotional
44
Self-awareness, Conscientiousness, Developing Others, Service Orientation, and
Communication. Women were rated higher than men in the five competencies.
Supervisor ratings revealed significant differences on two emotional competencies—
women were rated higher on Adaptability and Service Orientation. Ratings of direct
reports showed one significant difference; men were rated higher on the Change Catalyst
competency.
Studies have shown that while men and women do not appear to differ in total EI,
there is evidence that women and men may differ on specific competencies (Cavallo and
Brienza, 2002). Bar-On (2000) surveyed more than 7,700 administrations of the Emotion
Quotient Inventory (EQ-I) and found that while men and women did not differ on the
total EI, women scored significantly higher on Empathy, Interpersonal Relationships, and
Social Responsibility, while men scored higher on Self-actualization, Assertiveness,
Stress Tolerance, Impulse Control, and Adaptability.
Cavallo and Brienza (2002) reported that some studies suggest that the style by
which males and females lead differs. Eagly and Johnson (1990) found that females have
better social skills and are described as “interested in other people.” Compared to male
leaders as a group, women leaders as a group tend to be portrayed as more friendly,
pleasant, and socially sensitive.
Several theories have been presented to explain gender differences, “including
biological differences, differences in early childhood socialization in same-sex
playgroups, and the fulfillment of culturally prescribed gender role expectations”
(Cavallo and Brienza, 2002, pp. 3-4).
45
EI and Performance in the Finance Sector
A study on emotional intelligence and performance in the finance sector (Sevinc,
2001), indicated that self-reported salary was significantly correlated with the ECI
clusters, indicating that those who reported higher salaries tended to be rated by others as
higher on emotional intelligence than those who reported lower salaries. The study
sample included a group of Turkish participants working in the finance sector (banks,
insurance, and securities). A marginal finding related to position level and social skills,
suggesting that those in higher-level positions were rated by others as having higher
social skills than others (Sala, 2002).
Sala (2002) described a relationship between the number of promotions reported
by participants and self-rated EI scores. Although correlations were small, significant
relationships were found between self-reported number of promotions in a five-year span
and self-rated ECI scores. However, no relationship was found between ECI ratings of
“total others” and participants’ self-reported number of promotions. Sala contends the
findings may be related to a halo effect.
Measuring EI
Cherniss (2000) and others provided an overview of the instruments used to gauge
emotional intelligence:
Bar-On’s EQ-I, the oldest instrument, has been in use for more than a decade. It
originated from a clinical, rather than an occupational context. It was designed to assess
personal qualities that enabled some people to possess better “emotional well-being” than
others.
46
The Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) is a test of ability instead of a self-
report measure. Participants perform a series of tasks to assess their ability to perceive,
identify, understand, and work with emotion.
The Emotional Competence Inventory (ECI) is a 360-degree instrument. Others who
know the individual rate him or her on 20 competencies (the ECI 2.0 includes 18
competencies) that Goleman’s research suggests are linked to emotional intelligence.
Approximately 40 percent of the instrument’s items originated with the Self-Assessment
Questionnaire developed by Boyatzis.
The EQ Map was developed by Cooper and Sawaf (1997) to help people chart their
“relative strengths and vulnerabilities across a wide range of characteristics related to
emotional intelligence’ (p. xv).
Schutte, Malouff, Hall, Haggerty, Cooper, Golden, and Dornheim designed a 33-item
self-report instrument based on the early work of Salovey and Mayer (1990).
A sixth measure is described on the EI Consortium’s website. The Work Profile
Questionnaire—EI Version (WPQei) is an 84-item instrument that measures the personal
qualities and competencies employees need to develop to manage emotion at work. It is
based on qualities and competencies identified by researchers such as Mayer and Salovey
and Goleman. The WPQei, based on a conceptual model of emotional intelligence, has
seven components:
1. Innovation
2. Self-awareness
3. Intuition
4. Emotions
5. Motivation
6. Empathy
47
7. Social Skills
Cherniss (2000) added that other instruments that measure specific abilities assess
emotional intelligence and competencies. “Some of these tests seem rather strong. To
name just one example, there is Seligman’s SASQ, which was designed to measure
learned optimism and which has been impressive in its ability to identify high-performing
students, salespeople, and athletes, to name just a few” (Schulman, 1995) (p. 14).
Comparing Self and Others’ Ratings
Practitioners and organizational consultants propose that multi-rater, or 360-
degree feedback systems, enhance self-knowledge and improve managerial actions.
“Scholarly research has confirmed these sentiments and found that higher levels of
congruence between managerial ‘self’ and ‘total others’ behavioral ratings is associated
with managerial effectiveness and performance” (Sala, 2000, p. 27).
Burckle (1999) compared self and total others’ ratings. She found that those who
are not well in tune with their strengths and weaknesses have difficulty evaluating
themselves on EI competencies. She also found that, compared to those who had high
scores in Accurate Self-assessment, those who score low on the Accurate Self-assessment
subscale of the ECI have significantly larger gaps between their views of themselves and
others’ views of them.
In a research report, Burckle (1999) recommended multi-rater over self-
assessments. “These findings suggest that relying on self-assessments alone can be
misleading, lending support to the contention that multi-rater assessments are more
48
desirable than self-assessments when evaluating emotional intelligence competencies” (p.
8).
Performance Appraisal as a Measure of Effectiveness
Performance appraisal systems are at the center of the process of maintaining
satisfactory levels of performance in organizations (Frechette and Wertheim, 1985). A
critical function of performance appraisals is to “define effective performance and to
measure employee job performance and behavior with respect to the criteria for effective
performance, and to use this information as a basis for various personnel decisions, such
as placement, promotion, and compensation.
Grote (1996) contended that while nearly all organizations have performance
appraisal systems in place, “no one seems satisfied with the system they have or content
with the results it produces” (p.1). However, performance appraisal systems continue to
be used to gauge performance. It seems to be “impossible to manage an organization
without good information on how well people are performing and how they go about
meeting their responsibilities” (p.4).
However, a single performance appraisal format may “not be adaptable to the
many situations and positions in the organization” (Frechette and Wertheim, 1985, p.
221). After listing a number of purposes of performance appraisal processes, including
selection, promotion, and management and organizational development, the authors
added, “No one performance appraisal format can achieve such a multitude of
purposes…. It is necessary to select those purposes that must be achieved by the
49
performance appraisal system and then design a format and program that are appropriate
for those purposes” (p. 225).
Summary
Is high emotional intelligence synonymous with effective leadership? Are the
contentions of Goleman (1998a and 1998b) accurate? He asserted that with the recent
emphasis on flexibility, teams, and a strong customer focus in the workplace, “this crucial
set of emotional competencies is becoming increasingly essential for excellence in every
job and in every part of the world” (p. 29).
Goleman (1998b) further declared that effective leaders are alike in one crucial
way: “They all have a high degree of what come to be known as emotional intelligence.
It’s not that IQ and technical skills are irrelevant. …But my research, along with other
recent studies, clearly shows that emotional intelligence is the ‘sine qua non’ of
leadership. Without it, a person can have the best training in the world, an incisive,
analytical mind, and an endless supply of smart ideas, but he still won’t make a great
leader” (p. 2).
The researcher has an interest in leadership theories and practices. She has studied
leadership in academia and participated in leadership development programs and held
leadership positions in the workplace and in various organizations. She has considered
the topic as part of a doctoral research study for some time. After being introduced to
emotional intelligence, the researcher started reading about the topic and began to realize
that there is a connection between the emotional intelligence concept and effective
leadership. She investigated EI and read about comparison between EI and IQ. She
50
found studies that examined the relationships between EI and effective performance in
the workplace as well as studies that examined the influence of various demographic
characteristics on EI levels. The researcher also found a strong contention that EI can be
learned. She uncovered information about model programs and the elements of successful
training programs. This research study draws from elements uncovered in the literature
and those found in previous studies.
The studies cited in the literature suggest that emotional intelligence is related to
the effectiveness of managers in today’s organizations. The purpose of this study is to
determine if differences in EI levels exist among high performers in one organization.
Specifically, the study assesses whether managers in this financial organization who are
deemed most effective according to their performance ratings exhibit higher emotional
intelligence competencies than managers with lower performance ratings. The researcher
also compared EI levels and various demographic characteristics of the sample.
51
Chapter III Methodology
Because of increased competition and changes in the workplace, organizational
leaders are seeking to differentiate and improve their products and services. They are
looking for ways to ensure success of their organizations. Among the many factors that
have been attributed to success in organizations, one factor often cited is effective
leadership.
What makes a leader effective? As Goleman (1995, 1998b) and others (including
Cherniss. 2000; Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cooper and Sawaf 1997; Druskat, 2001;
George, 2000) contended, an effective leader has more than the technical skills to do a job.
He or she employs qualities, such as initiative and empathy, adaptability, and
persuasiveness—competencies of emotional intelligence. Researchers (Cavallo and
Brienza, 2002; George, 1995, 2002; George and Bettenhausen, 1990; Goleman, 1998b)
have found relationships between emotions and competencies and between performance
and leadership. High EI in organizations has been linked to improved financial results,
improved return on investment in change initiatives, improved productivity, increased
retention of top talent, and increased sales (Hay Group, 2002b).
The purpose of the study is to determine if performance levels and various
demographic characteristics are related to emotional intelligence scores in a sample of
high-performing managers within the financial services industry.
To address research questions and hypotheses, the researcher employed a
comparative research design to investigate differences among managers with varying
demographic characteristics in one financial institution. To ascertain if differences in
emotional intelligence levels existed among the sample, an existing survey instrument,
52
the Emotional Competence Inventory 2.0, a demographic form, and an existing
performance appraisal system and rating forms, were used. Data were analyzed with
descriptive statistics and univariate
analysis of variance.
The researcher analyzed the findings using the scored data provided by the Hay
Group, performance ratings, and the demographic information by using a statistical
analysis computing system, SAS 8. The researcher analyzed the data from the ECIs and
demographic forms using the general linear model (GLM) version of analysis of variance
(ANOVA), T-test, and union-intersection test methods.
An ANOVA test determines the existence (or nonexistence) of a statistically
significant difference among several group means (UCLA, 2002). The test actually uses
variances to help ascertain if the various means are equal or not. ANOVA uncovers the
main and interaction effects of categorical independent variables (or “factors”) on interval
dependent variables. The T-test is a test of significance of the difference in the means of a
single interval dependent, for two groups formed by a categorical independent (Garson,
2002). The T-test and ANOVA are generally accepted statistical tests and are equally
valid in the analysis of differences among levels of variables, such as performance
ratings, emotional intelligence scores, and demographic characteristics.
Union-intersection tests compared the mean EI scores of participants with
performance ratings of five with those with performance ratings of four and those with
performance ratings of four with those with performance ratings of three. If both of the
assumptions are accepted statistically, one can conclude that the mean EI of those with
ratings of five is greater than that of those with ratings of four and that the mean EI of
those with ratings of four is greater than that of those with ratings of three.
53
Methods and Processes
A description of the methods and processes employed in the study follow.
Methods and processes included the following elements:
Research design
Data needs and collection steps
Instrumentation
Population and sample
Data collection procedures
Research Design
The researcher employed a quantitative research design, which provides a
numeric description of a fraction of the population—the sample—through the data
collection process of asking people questions (Creswell, 1994). From the data collection,
the researcher may be able to generalize from a sample to a population in order to make
inferences about characteristics, attitudes, and behaviors of the population.
The sample is purposeful (Creswell, 1994)—respondents were selected based on
their convenience and availability. The sampling design is single-staged—the participants
were surveyed directly. The researcher does not know their identities. The participants
voluntarily provided demographic and other information about themselves. The survey is
cross-sectional; the information was collected at one point in time.
Data Needs and Collection Steps
The data needs for this study require assessment of 18 emotional competencies,
performance ratings, and descriptions of specific demographic data (such as gender,
educational level, type of degree, management tenure, title and position, and employment
54
area: support or sales) for each person in the sample. The study also required norms
(averages) of others whose EI competencies have been assessed using the survey
instrument employed in this study. The norms were ascertained from the Emotional
Competence Inventory (ECI) Technical Manual and are based on the North American
and United Kingdom ECI databases (Sala, 2002). Descriptions of the survey instruments
employed in the study follow.
Instrumentation
Three types of instrumentation were required for the study: the Emotional
Competence Inventory, performance appraisal instruments, and a form that captured
demographic data on participants. Following is a description of each instrument type:
ECI Background. The researcher employed an existing instrument, Hay/McBer’s
ECI 2.0, in the study. ECI is a 360-degree tool (See sample in Appendix B) that assesses
the emotional competencies of individuals and organizations (Sala, 2002). The multi-rater
instrument provides self, manager, direct report, peer, and other perspectives on a cluster
of research-validated behaviors, which profile emotional intelligence. Each instrument
may accommodate up to 12 raters (self, manager, direct reports, peers, and others).
The tool is based on emotional competencies identified by Goleman in Working
with Emotional Intelligence (1998b), competencies from Hay/McBer’s Generic
Competency Dictionary (1996), and Boyatzis’s Self-assessment Questionnaire (SAQ)
(Sala, 2002).
Hay/McBer’s Generic Competency Dictionary (1996) originated with Lyle and
Signe Spencer in the book, Competence at Work (1993), and was revised by psychologist
McClelland following an extensive review of the literature (Sala, 2002). It is based on
55
more than 20 years of research initiated by McClelland in 1973 in the article, “Testing for
Competence Rather Than Intelligence.” Hay/McBer and other researchers have
established that each core competency in the dictionary reliably differentiates
performance in a variety of organizations (Sala, 2002). The SAQ was developed by
Boyatzis in 1991 for use with MBA and executive students to assess competencies in the
Generic Model of Management used at the Weatherhead School of Management, Case
Western Reserve University (Sala, 2002). Since 1991, it has been used in numerous
studies, including longitudinal research (Boyatzis, Cowen, and Kolb, 1995; Boyatzis,
1994; Boyatzis, Baker, Leonard, Rhee, and Thompson, 1995; Boyatzis, Leonard, Rhee,
and Wheeler, 1996). The ECI 2.0 assesses 18 individual competencies. Each competency
is defined with specific behaviors and characteristics (Sala, 2002).
Based on a review of approximately 200 competency models, Goleman (1998b)
identified 25 social and emotional competencies that most accurately predict superior
performance in many occupations. He organized the competencies into five dimensions
of EI (Goleman, 1995): self-awareness, self-regulation, self-motivation, social awareness,
and social skills. Cherniss and Adler (2000) added that research employing the
Emotional Competence Inventory, a measure of emotion intelligence, by Boyatzis,
Goleman, and Rhee (2002), led to a refined version of the original model. The
competency framework consists of four dimensions (Self-awareness, Self-management,
Social Awareness, and Relationship Management) and 18 competencies, including
Empathy, Achievement Orientation, Optimism, Self-confidence, and Adaptability
(Boyatzis, Goleman, and McKee, 2002, pp. 253-256) (See Table A, Appendix A). The
competencies were distilled in large part from studies by the U.S. Office of Personnel
56
Management (1996); Spencer and Spencer (1993); and Rosier and Jeffrey (1994 and
1995).
The competencies may be grouped in four clusters. While a number of research
studies, such as those conducted by Sevinc (2001) and Cavallo and Brienza (2002)
investigated EI competencies and EI clusters, others, such as a study by Humphrey,
Kellett and Sleeth (2001) investigated individual competencies. The Humphrey et al.
study was conducted to determine if both empathy and cognitive ability are associated
with perceptions of leadership. Results showed that both influence perceptions of
leadership in small groups (Sala, 2002). Because of the sample size of 57, the researcher
in this study looked at individual competencies rather than groups, or clusters of
competencies. The researcher completed a competency-by-competency analysis of the
data to address research question and hypotheses and made intermittent inferences about
the competencies as related to the EI dimensions.
Reliability
Sala (2002) defined reliability as the consistency or stability of measures or
observations. If someone is measured twice on the same scale, he or she should have the
same, or reliable, score. Two indicators are generally used to assess the reliability of
survey instruments: internal consistency and test-retest reliability.
Internal consistency. Internal consistency is the average of the intercorrelations
among the survey items. Chronbach’s alpha, which is a commonly used indicator of
internal consistency, is used to describe the reliability of the ECI (See Table C, Appendix
C).
57
For total others’ ratings in the ECI, Sala (2002) reported that coefficients range
from .73 to .92. The overall average internal consistency coefficient was .85. For self-
ratings, the alpha coefficients range from .61 to .85, with an overall average internal
consistency coefficient of .75. The findings suggest that total others ratings are more
stable and reliable than self-ratings; however, no statistical tests were run to determine
whether or not the differences were significant.
The researcher in this study analyzed the responses of “total others” (managers,
subordinates, peers, and customers) in an attempt to ensure more accurate responses.
Sala (2002) explained that practitioners and organizational consultants believe
that multi-rater, or 360-degree feedback systems, enhance self-knowledge and improve
managerial actions. “Scholarly research has confirmed these sentiments and found that
higher levels of congruence between managerial ‘self’ and ‘total others’ behavioral
ratings is associated with managerial effectiveness and performance” (p. 27).
Burckle (1999) compared self and total others’ ratings. She found that those who
are not well in tune with their strengths and weaknesses have difficulty evaluating
themselves on EI competencies. She found that those who score low on the Accurate
Self-assessment subscale of the ECI have significantly larger gaps between their views of
themselves and others’ views of them, compared to those who had high scores in
Accurate Self-assessment.
In a research report, Burckle (1999) recommended multi-rater over self-
assessments. “These findings suggest that relying on self-assessments alone can be
misleading, lending support to the contention that multi-rater assessments are more
58
desirable than self-assessments when evaluating emotional intelligence competencies” (p.
8).
Test-retest reliability. According to Sala (2002), while no specific test-retest
reliability studies have been conducted with the ECI, “other pre- and post-assessment
research provides reasonable evidence for adequate levels of test-retest reliability” (p. 5).
He cited a study of 20 Brazilian executives from a large consumer retail organization.
The executives were assessed twice on the ECI; the assessments were seven months
apart. He explained that because the time between assessments was longer than what is
typically recommended and that the executives participated in an EI development
program between assessments, results should be interpreted with caution. “This data also
suggests that the ECI may be sensitive to change because stability coefficients for the
total others ratings were only moderately high, while stability coefficients for self scores
were very low” (Sala, 2002, p.5) (See Table D, Appendix D for test-retest coefficients).
Validity
Validity generally refers to the degree to which a measure or questionnaire
actually measures what it is supposed to measure (Sala, 2002). There are two types of
validity: content, which addresses whether a test adequately samples the relevant material
it is supposed to cover, and construct, which is the degree to which a test or questionnaire
is a measure of the characteristic of interest. Construct validity includes discriminant and
convergent validity, which determines whether or not the test correlates with other
measures with which it should be conceptually related and correlates less with those with
which it should not be associated. For instance, the ECI should correlate positively with
59
self-esteem (convergent) and negatively with depression (convergent). It should not
correlate with cognitive ability (discriminant).
Criterion validity is the degree to which the test of measure correlates with some
outcome criteria (Sala, 2002). Sala and others have conducted research to validate the
ECI against various outcome measures of performance in the workplace. For example,
research might test whether a manager high in EI tends to have lower turnover rates than
does a manager low in EI. Concurrent validity occurs when the measure of interest (such
as the ECI) and the criterion or outcome are assessed simultaneously. Predictive validity
occurs when the outcome is collected after the variable of interest is assessed.
Among other studies, Sala (2002) cites a research effort by Burckle (1999) that
investigated the accurate self-assessment subscale of the ECI as support of content
validity. To examine construct validity of the ECI, he describes a study by
Diamantopoulou (2001) that sought to ascertain if a relationship existed between Type A
and Type B personalities and emotional intelligence. The researcher found that people
with a mixture of Type A and B personalities were higher in emotional intelligence and
that Type B personality was positively correlated with social skills competencies.
To further support construct validity, Sala (2002) cited a study by Burckle (2000)
that sought to determine if a relationship existed between the ECI and the Myers-Briggs
Type Indicator (MBTI). Results of the study showed moderate to strong significant
correlations between several EI competencies and the Sensing/Intuiting and
Thinking/Feeling dimensions of the MBTI. Negative correlations indicated a relationship
existed in the Intuiting direction of the Sensing/Intuiting scale and the Feeling direction
of the Thinking/Feeling scale. The results indicate that Myers-Briggs Intuitive types are
60
strong on a number of EI competencies, especially Empathy and Adaptability and those
in the Social Awareness cluster. The Myers-Briggs Feeling types correlated with many
of the EI competencies, particularly Empathy and competencies in the Social Awareness
cluster. Sala also cites correlations between Feeling types and the competencies in the EI
Social Skills cluster.
Scale Design
The ECI 2.0 contains 73 items with a minimum of three items to assess each
competency. A six-point response format has been employed as follows:
Points Frequency
1 Never
2 Rarely
3 Sometimes
4 Often
5 Consistently
6 Don’t Know
Sample items from the ECI 2.0 include the following (See Appendix B):
Confronts unethical actions
Knows how feelings impact their own performance
Behaves calmly in stressful situations
Seeks information in unusual ways
Sees opportunities rather than threats
Performance Appraisal System of Participating Company
Performance reviews are prepared for employees of the participating organization
during the first quarter of each calendar year. Descriptions of the instruments used to rate
performance of the sales and support managers follow:
61
Sales Performance Analysis
The sales division of the organization uses a standard performance appraisal form.
The sales division incorporates all positions within the bank’s branch network. Positions
are generic throughout the organization (chief human resources officer, personal
communication, October 2, 2002). Based on the information provided by the human
resources officer, 40 of the participants in the study (70 percent) were evaluated using the
sales form (See Appendix E for a copy of the form). The form has been in use for at least
six years; it is updated annually to include specific behaviors. It assesses defined goals
and behaviors. Instructions for completing the forms are provided. (See Appendix E).
Sales personnel receive an overall performance rating based on the scale below:
Level Performance evaluation
5 Achieved Excellence
4 Exceeds Expectations
3 Meets Expectations
2 Needs Improvement
1 Unacceptable
62
Support Performance Analysis
The support departments of the bank report directly to the vice-chairman,
according to the chief human resources officer (personal communication, October 2,
2002). The department personnel are responsible for all activities involved in supporting
the sales functions, as well as traditional corporate functions.
The form used to appraise those in support roles has been in use for eight years,
with very few organizational modifications (See Appendix F). Managers in various
departments and divisions may modify the form to meet the specific needs of their areas.
The form is based on the five-point scale described below. According to the chief human
resources officer, even though the wording used to describe the five categories varies
slightly between the sales and support forms, the meanings/ratings are consistent and are
applied consistently for rewards, promotions, and so forth. The officer said, “Because we
are reworking all forms in 2003, we simply communicated expectations for completion
rather than a redesign of the form at this time” (personal communication, November 12,
2002).
Level Performance evaluation
5 Superior
4 Above expected level
3 Expected level
2 Below expected level
1 Unacceptable
The support form uses the five-point ratings scale described above; however,
evaluators have an option to use two additional ratings for line items gauging behaviors
and goals of managers: “Not Applicable” and “Unknown.” According to the chief human
63
resources officer, the two additional options are rarely used and if they are marked, a
human resources representative works with the evaluator to elicit one of the ratings in the
five-point scale. The officer explained that the form is being eliminated and replaced next
year. “Because we will be eliminating this form in 2003, we have not moved forward
with the changes” (personal communication, November 12, 2002).
Demographic Form
Participants completed a demographic form (See Appendix G) indicating their last
performance rating and demographic characteristics. The researcher analyzed the data to
determine the influence of performance ratings and varying demographic characteristics
on emotional intelligence competencies.
The researcher developed the demographic form following a review of the
literature and with advice from management at the participating organization. Studies
have been conducted to determine if relationships between EI scores and various
demographic characteristics, such as gender, function, age, job function, and job level,
exist (Sala, 2002; Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; and Goleman, 1998b) With input from the
participating organization’s management, the researcher developed classifications that
might help determine if varying categories influenced EI competencies. The manager of
management and leadership development reviewed categories and choices; modified
them slightly to match internal titles, positions, and so forth; and provided approval.
Participants indicated which one of several choices matched their positions and
background on a forced-choice form.
64
Population and Sample
The population of the study includes high-performing and potentially high-
performing managers who were selected to participate in an internal study of emotional
intelligence in a financial organization. Based in North Carolina, the bank has more than
5,000 employees and serves three states with about 350 branches. The organization offers
a complete line of financial services, including personal banking, business banking,
investor services, trust services, and insurance services.
The sample of 57 managers is a subset of the management pool of 79 selected to
participate in a first-of-its kind internal study of emotional intelligence. A selection
committee, comprised of the manager of management and leadership development, group
vice presidents of corporate education and human resources, and the employment
manager, identified 24 of the participants. The other 55 were selected because of their
positions. The company’s vice chairman reviewed the nominations and provided final
approval. According to the manager of management and leadership development
(personal communication, September 27, 2002), participants were chosen based on
diversity goals and specific criteria, including the following:
Performance ratings of three or above for the past two years
At least two years tenure in the company
Participation in or selection to participate in a leadership development program
Contribution to a department, area or organization-wide project
Career aspirations warranting investment in leadership development opportunities
65
Sample Selection and Identification of Managers
Of the 79 managers participating in an internal EI program, 59 were asked to
participate in the doctoral research study. Twenty executive-level participants in the
bank’s study did not have performance ratings, and, therefore, did not qualify for
participation in the doctoral research study. “In order to use the data to obtain a profile of
leaders in the organization, I asked that we also include executives. This was the first
time [the bank had conducted an EI program] so I wanted executives to get a company
[EI] profile [from the Hay Group],” according to the manager of management and
leadership development (personal communication, November 18, 2002). Two of the 59
managers chose not to participate.
According to the chief human resources officer, the 20 executives without
performance ratings participate in a “narrative performance discussion” during the second
quarter of each year. However, ratings are not maintained for these executives. The
executives report directly to the vice chairman or president of the bank and hold the top
leadership positions in the organization, including chief strategist and support executives,
such as chief financial officer, chief information offices, chief human resources officer,
chief credit officer, and so forth (personal communication, November 12, 2002).
Gathering of Survey Information
Data for the study were gathered via the Emotional Competence Inventory 2.0, an
existing instrument. Study participants completed a “self-report” version of the
instrument. Peers, managers, direct reports, and customers evaluated each participant,
completing 360-degree instruments.
66
Participants followed the Hay Group’s requirements for selecting others to
evaluate them using a 360-degree version of the ECI, according to the manager of
management and leadership development (personal communication, November 18,
2002). Participants were allowed to select a maximum of five and a minimum of two
people from the categories that included direct reports, peers, and customers. The Hay
Group required that a minimum of two responders other than self and manager complete
and submit the instruments. The process was conducted via e-mail and the Internet during
a three- to four-week period during the summer of 2002. The Hay Group received
electronic versions of the completed forms, scored them, and prepared reports of the
results for each participant as well as a group report for the organization. The scores of
those completing 360-degree inventories were compiled for each participant and labeled
“total others.” The researcher received the scored data from the Hay Group via emails for
use in the study. The “total others” scores were analyzed to address hypotheses and
research questions. The researcher also compared self and total others’ ratings.
Those who volunteered for the external study completed a form, indicating their
last performance rating, as well as demographic characteristics, and a consent form
required by North Carolina State University. The ratings were confirmed by the manager
of management and leadership development. The company’s manager of management
and leadership development also checked the demographic forms to ascertain that there
was a distribution of the three rating groups (21 with rating 3, 28 with rating 4, and 8 with
rating 5). The chart below compares the percentage of bank employees in each
performance-rating category with the percentage of bank employees participating in the
67
research study (chief human resources officer, personal communication, October 2,
2002).
2002 Performance Summary
Performance
Rating
Overall % Study %
1 0 0
2 3.18 0
3 60.62 36.85
4 34.18 49.12
5 2.02 14.03
Demographic characteristics used to stratify the sample include the following:
Role in the organization
Title
Gender
Function (sales or support)
Number of years with direct reports
Level of education (advanced or not)
Type of degree (BA or BS)
Data collection procedures
The data used in the study was collected via the participating organization’s
administration of the ECI. Participants were selected to participate via a committee based
on criteria specified above. Under the direction of the manager of management and
leadership development and following the guidelines supplied by the Hay Group, which
owns the instrument and scores the surveys, participants completed self-report versions of
68
the ECI during the summer of 2002. Others with whom they work closely completed
360-degree versions of the instrument also during the summer of 2002.
The Hay Group formally scored data compiled from the ECIs, according to an
algorithm. According to Sala (2002):
ECI raw data is scored or weighted using an algorithm that gives more ‘credit’ for
higher ratings on items that correspond to higher levels within a competency.
Items are written to reflect developmental characteristics such that higher-level
items reflect increasingly sophisticated behaviors for each emotional intelligence
competency. For example, for Leadership, a lower-level item is, ‘leads by
example’ while a higher-level item is, ‘articulates a compelling vision.’ Because
the higher-level item is more ‘difficult’ and requires more ‘leadership,’ the
scoring algorithm gives more weight to higher ratings on these competencies.
This technique ensures that higher-level behaviors are recognized, and it also
serves to increase variability of scores (p. 37).
During the internal survey process, the organization’s manager of management
and leadership development explained the proposed doctoral research study and invited
participants to volunteer for the study. She asked volunteers to complete the demographic
and consent forms. The manager devised codes for each participant. To ensure
anonymity, she assigned three-digit codes to the demographic data and consent forms for
each participant. A representative of the Hay Group sent the researcher ECI scores
labeled with six-digit codes for each participant in a spreadsheet. The bank manager
provided the key that matched the three- and six-digit codes. The researcher matched the
69
Hay Group codes with those provided by the bank, adding the three-digit codes to the six-
digit codes on the spreadsheet.
Research Questions, Hypotheses, and Data Analysis
The following research questions, hypotheses, and analysis methods guide the
study:
Analysis of
Research Question One
Research Question One: Do high-performing leaders exhibit high emotional
intelligence?
Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18): High performance ratings and high scores on
each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies listed below are not related:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Developing Others
(H0-8): Empathy
(H0-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-10): Emotional Self-control
(H0-11): Influence
(H0-12): Initiative
(H0-13): Inspirational Leadership
70
(H0-14): Organizational Awareness
(H0-15): Optimism
(H0-16): Self-confidence
(H0-17): Transparency
(H0-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
Alternative Hypotheses 1 (H-1) – (H1-18): High performance ratings and high
scores on each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies are related:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Developing Others
(H1-8): Empathy
(H1-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-10): Emotional Self-control
(H1-11): Influence
(H1-12): Initiative
(H1-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-14): Organizational Awareness
(H1-15): Optimism
(H1-16): Self-confidence
71
(H1-17): Transparency
(H1-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
To address the first null hypotheses and corresponding alternative hypotheses, a
union-intersection test was deployed to determine if those with ratings of five had higher
EI scores than those with ratings of four and if those with ratings of four had higher EI
scores than those with ratings of three. The test compared the mean EI scores of
participants who had performance ratings of five with those with performance ratings of
four at a 97.5 percent confidence level. A second test compared mean EI score of
participants who had performance ratings of four with those who had performance ratings
of three, also at a 97.5 percent confidence level. If both of the assumptions are accepted
statistically, one can conclude that the mean EI of those with ratings of five is greater
than the mean EI of those with ratings of four and that the mean EI of those with ratings
of four is greater than the mean EI of those with ratings of three. The researcher tested the
hypotheses at 95 and 90 percent
confidence levels.
Analysis of
Research Question Two
Research Question 2: How do emotional intelligence competency scores of the
sample compare with emotional intelligence competency scores of other samples
surveyed using the same survey instrument?
Null Hypotheses 2(H0-1)-(H0-16): There are no significant differences among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence he
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
72
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Empathy
(H0-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-9): Emotional Self-control
(H0-10): Influence
(H0-11): Initiative
(H0-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-13): Organizational Awareness
(H0-14): Optimism
(H0-15): Self-confidence
(H0-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
Alternative Hypotheses 2 (H1-1)-(H1-16): Significant differences exist among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
73
(H1-7): Empathy
(H1-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-9): Emotional Self-control
(H1-10): Influence
(H1-11): Initiative
(H1-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-13): Organizational Awareness
(H1-14): Optimism
(H1-15): Self-confidence
(H1-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
The researcher compared the “total others” EI mean scores for each participant in
the study sample with the “total others” EI mean scores of 5,360 people in the North
American and United Kingdom ECI databases (Sala, 2002), using a T-test and a 95
percent confidence level (P< .05).
Analysis of
Research Question Three
Research Question Three: Do various demographic characteristics (position, title,
gender, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
influence scores on 18 emotional intelligence competencies?
Null Hypotheses 3 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor, manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
74
Alternative Hypotheses 3 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor,
manager, senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
The researcher employed an ANOVA procedure at a 95 percent confidence level
to determine if there were statistically significant differences in EI scores among
managers in the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor,
supervisor, manager, senior manager, and executive).
Null Hypotheses 4 (H0-1)-(H0-18)(See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice president, area vice
president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 4 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice
president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
The researcher employed an ANOVA procedure at a 95 percent confidence level
to determine if there were statistically significant differences in EI scores among
managers in the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior
vice president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president).
75
Null Hypotheses 5 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
men and women in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 5 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of men and women in the sample.
The researcher employed an ANOVA procedure and 95 percent confidence level
to determine if there were statistically significant differences in EI scores between males
and females in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
The researcher again used ANOVA to determine if statistically significant (P<.05)
differences in EI scores existed between managers working in sales functions and those
working in support functions.
Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
76
Alternative Hypotheses 7 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports
for more than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct
reports for fewer than five years.
The researcher compared the two groups using ANOVA to determine, with 95
percent confidence (P <.05), if statistically significant differences in EI scores existed
between managers who have been in management roles with direct reports for more than
five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for fewer
than five years.
Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers without
advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s degree).
Alternative Hypotheses 8 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers
without advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s
degree).
The researcher analyzed the data with a T-test to ascertain, with 95 percent
confidence (P <.05), whether statistically significant differences in EI scores existed
between those in the sample with advanced degrees and those in the sample without
advanced degrees.
77
Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with Bachelor of
Arts degrees.
Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
Again deploying a T-test, the researcher analyzed of the data to determine, with 95
percent confidence (P <.05), if statistically significant differences in EI scores existed
between those with a Bachelor of Science degree and those with a Bachelor of Arts
degree.
78
Chapter IV Findings
Purpose of the Study
The purpose of this study was to determine if differences in EI levels existed
among high performers in one organization. Specifically, the study assessed whether
managers in this financial organization who are deemed most effective, according to
their performance ratings, exhibit higher emotional intelligence competencies than
managers with lower performance ratings. The researcher also compared EI levels
and various demographic characteristics of the sample, including, position, title, area
of employment, gender, education level, type of degree, and management tenure.
The data were gathered from a sample of 57 managers participating in an
emotional intelligence program of a financial institution. These managers were
included in a first-time internal EI study. Their selection for participation was based
on their position in the company and specific criteria as described in Chapter 3. The
participants completed self-report versions of the Emotional Competence Inventory
2.0. Selected managers, peers, direct reports, and customers of each participant
completed 360-degree versions of the ECI. The ECI scores, performance ratings, and
demographic data were analyzed to address hypotheses and research questions.
Research questions, null hypotheses, and alternative hypotheses
The following research questions and hypotheses guided the study and the
methodology.
79
Research Question One
Research Question 1: Do high-performing leaders exhibit high emotional
intelligence?
Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18): High performance ratings and high scores on
each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies listed below are not related:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Developing Others
(H0-8): Empathy
(H0-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-10): Emotional Self-control
(H0-11): Influence
(H0-12): Initiative
(H0-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-14): Organizational Awareness
(H0-15): Optimism
(H0-16): Self-confidence
(H0-17): Transparency
(H0-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
80
Alternative Hypotheses 1 (H-1) – (H1-18): High performance ratings and high
scores on each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies are related:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Developing Others
(H1-8): Empathy
(H1-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-10): Emotional Self-control
(H1-11): Influence
(H1-12): Initiative
(H1-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-14): Organizational Awareness
(H1-15): Optimism
(H1-16): Self-confidence
(H1-17): Transparency
(H1-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
81
Research Question Two
Research Question Two: How do emotional intelligence competency scores of the
sample compare with emotional intelligence competency scores of other samples
surveyed using the same survey instrument?
Null Hypotheses 2(H0-1)-(H0-16): There are no significant differences among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence he
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Empathy
(H0-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-9): Emotional Self-control
(H0-10): Influence
(H0-11): Initiative
(H0-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-13): Organizational Awareness
(H0-14): Optimism
(H0-15): Self-confidence
(H0-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
82
Alternative Hypotheses 2 (H1-1)-(H1-16): Significant differences exist among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Empathy
(H1-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-9): Emotional Self-control
(H1-10): Influence
(H1-11): Initiative
(H1-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-13): Organizational Awareness
(H1-14): Optimism
(H1-15): Self-confidence
(H1-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
83
Research Question Three
Research Question Three: Do various demographic characteristics (position, title,
gender, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
influence scores on 18 emotional intelligence competencies?
Null Hypotheses 3 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor, manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 3 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor,
manager, senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Null Hypotheses 4 (H0-1)-(H0-18): (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice president, area vice
president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 4 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice
president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
84
Null Hypotheses 5 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
men and women in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 5 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of men and women in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
Alternative Hypotheses 7 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports
for more than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct
reports for fewer than five years.
85
Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers without
advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s degree).
Alternative Hypotheses 8 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers
without advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s
degree).
Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with Bachelor of
Arts degrees.
Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
Analysis
This section describes the analysis of data gathered during the study. The data
used in the study were collected via internal processes of the financial organization,
following the guidelines of the Hay Group, which formally scored the data. The ECI data
provided analyses of ECI competencies based on self responses and responses of others.
The researcher received the scored and coded data for each of the EI competencies for
86
each participant in a spreadsheet from the Hay Group. She added performance ratings
and demographic data to the spreadsheet. When participants failed to provide answers to
the demographic questions, and, in one case, when a manager chose two responses, the
“missing data points” were not included in the statistical analyses.
The researcher analyzed the findings using the scored data provided by the
Hay Group, the performance ratings, and the demographic information via a
statistical analysis computing system, SAS 8. The researcher analyzed the data from
the ECIs and demographic forms using the general linear model (GLM) version of
analysis of variance (ANOVA), T-test, and union-intersection test methods.
An ANOVA test determines the existence (or nonexistence) of a statistically
significant difference among several group means (UCLA, 2002). The test uses
variances to help ascertain if the various means are equal or not. ANOVA uncovers
the main and interaction effects of categorical independent variables (or “factors”) on
interval dependent variables. The T-test is a test of significance of the difference in
the means of a single interval dependent, for two groups formed by a categorical
independent (Garson, 2002). T-test and ANOVA are generally accepted statistical
tests and are equally valid in the analysis of differences among levels of variables,
such as performance ratings, emotional intelligence scores, and demographic
characteristics. A union-intersection test analyzed Null Hypotheses 1 to ascertain if
participants with ratings of five had higher EI scores than those with ratings of four
and if those with ratings of four had higher EI scores than those with ratings of three.
87
Detailed Research Questions and Null and Alternative Hypotheses Review
The following sections present findings related to each research question and null
and corresponding alternative hypothesis. The results consist of a summary of the
relevant findings, tables depicting the statistical analysis, and a decision about each null
hypothesis based on the statistical analysis.
Research Question One: Do high-performing leaders exhibit high emotional
intelligence?
Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): High
performance ratings and high scores on each of the 18 emotional intelligence
competencies are not related.
Alternative Hypotheses 1 (H1-1) 1- (H1-18): High performance ratings and high
scores on each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies are related.
To address the first null hypothesis and corresponding alternative hypothesis,
a union-intersection test was deployed to determine if those with ratings of five had
higher EI scores than those with ratings of four and if those with ratings of four had
higher EI scores than those with ratings of three. The test compared the mean EI
scores of participants who had performance ratings of five with those with
performance ratings of four at a 97.5 percent confidence level. A second test
compared mean EI score of participants who had performance ratings of four with
those who had performance ratings of three, also at a 97.5 percent confidence level. If
both of the assumptions are accepted statistically, one can conclude that the mean EI
of those with ratings of five is greater than the mean EI of those with ratings of four
and that the mean EI of those with ratings of four is greater than the mean EI of those
88
with ratings of three. The researcher analyzed the hypotheses at 95 and 90 percent
confidence levels.
The sample included 21 participants with ratings of 3; 28 with ratings of 4; and 8
ratings of 5. The list below compares the percentage of bank employees in each
performance-rating category with the percentage of bank employees participating in the
research study (chief human resources officer, personal communication, October 2,
2002).
2002 Performance Summary
Performance
Rating
Overall % Study %
1 0 0
2 3.18 0
3 60.62 36.85
4 34.18 49.12
5 2.02 14.03
Although high scores for 15 competencies (all except Conflict Management,
Developing Others, and Teamwork and Collaboration) appeared to be related to high
ratings based on mean scores (See Table 4.1 and Table H1, Appendix H), further analysis
indicated that there is no statistically significant relationship at the 95 percent confidence
level between high ratings and high emotional intelligence. Therefore, the researcher
does not reject Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18).
89
Table 4.1 Mean scores for each competency by rating
Competency ID Rating Mean
1 Achievement 3 3.39
2 Achievement 4 3.56
3 Achievement 5 3.71
4 Adaptability 3 3.34
5 Adaptability 4 3.52
6 Adaptability 5 3.66
7 Accurate Self-assessment 3 3.35
8 Accurate Self-assessment 4 3.41
9 Accurate Self-assessment 5 3.47
10 Conflict Management 3 2.85
11 Conflict Management 4 3.01
12 Conflict Management 5 2.99
13 Change Catalyst 3 3.3
14 Change Catalyst 4 3.39
15 Change Catalyst 5 3.6
16 Customer Service Orientation 3 3.87
17 Customer Service Orientation 4 3.89
18 Customer Service Orientation 5 3.92
19 Developing Others 3 3.34
20 Developing Others 4 3.59
21 Developing Others 5 3.55
22 Empathy 3 3.58
23 Empathy 4 3.6
24 Empathy 5 3.68
25 Emotional Self-awareness 3 3.26
26 Emotional Self-awareness 4 3.4
27 Emotional Self-awareness 5 3.52
28 Emotional Self-control 3 3.47
29 Emotional Self- control 4 3.6
30 Emotional Self-control 5 3.82
31 Influence 3 3.51
32 Influence 4 3.63
33 Influence 5 3.73
34 Initiative 3 3.31
35 Initiative 4 3.44
90
Table 4.1 continued
36 Initiative 5 3.59
37 Inspirational Leadership 3 3.26
38 Inspirational Leadership 4 3.5
39 Inspirational Leadership 5 3.64
40 Organizational Awareness 3 3.67
41 Organizational Awareness 4 3.67
42 Organizational Awareness 5 3.81
43 Optimism 3 3.67
44 Optimism 4 3.76
45 Optimism 5 3.9
46 Self-confidence 3 3.68
47 Self-confidence 4 3.76
48 Self-confidence 5 3.92
49 Transparency 3 3.39
50 Transparency 4 3.5
51 Transparency 5 3.66
52 Teamwork & Collaboration 3 3.54
53 Teamwork & Collaboration 4 3.62
54 Teamwork & Collaboration 5 3.54
However, further analysis at a 90 percent confidence level (P= <.1) indicated a
statistically significant relationship between high EI levels and high performance ratings
on three competencies: Achievement, Adaptability, and Optimism (See Table 4.2 and
Table H1, Appendix H).
91
Table 4.2 Relationship of high ratings to high EI scores
at 95 % & 90 % confidence levels
Do Not Reject Null
Hypothesis 1 (P<.05)
Reject Null Hypothesis
1 (P=<.1)
Do Not Reject Null
Hypothesis 1 (P=<.1)
Achievement (P=.05) Achievement(P=.055) Accurate Self-assessment
(P=.49)
Adaptability (P=.08) Adaptability (P=.08)
Conflict Management
(P=0.57)
Accurate Self-assessment
(P=0.49)
Optimism (P=.07) Change Catalyst (P=0.18)
Conflict Management
(P=0.57)
Customer Service Orientation
(P=0.49)
Change Catalyst (P=0.18) Developing Others (P=0.63)
Customer Service Orientation
(P=0.49)
Empathy (P=0.56)
Developing Others (P=0.63) Emotional Self-awareness
(P=0.14)
Empathy (P=0.58)
Emotional Self-control
(P=0.13)
Emotional Self-awareness
(P=0.14)
Influence (P=0.22)
Emotional Self-control
(P=0.13)
Initiative (P=0.12)
Influence (P=0.22)
Inspirational Leadership
(P=0.13)
Initiative (P=0.19)
Organizational Awareness
(P=0.54)
Inspirational Leadership
(P=0.13)
Optimism (P=.07)
Organizational Awareness
(P=0.54)
Self-confidence (P=0.14)
Optimism (P=.07) Transparency (P=0.16)
Self-confidence (P=0.15) Teamwork & Collaboration
(P=0.94)
Transparency (P=0.16)
Teamwork & Collaboration
(P=0.94)
Before employing the union-intersection test to determine if those with higher
ratings had higher EI scores, the researcher ran an ANOVA test to ascertain if a
92
statistically significant relationship existed between ratings and the 18 emotional
intelligence competencies. Findings indicated that a statistically significant
relationship (95 percent confidence, P<.05) existed between the ratings and EI scores
in 10 of 18 competencies (Table 4.3).
Table 4.3 Relationship of EI competencies and performance ratings at 95 percent
confidence
Significant Relationship No Significant Relationship
Competency P Value= Competency P Value=
Achievement .0016 Accurate Self-assessment .5714
Adaptability .0056 Change Catalyst .0649
Conflict Management .0206 Customer Service Orientation .5347
Developing Others .0058 Empathy .6324
Emotional Self-control .0371 Emotional Self-awareness .0651
Initiative .012 Influence .0654
Inspirational Leadership .006 Organizational Awareness .2636
Optimism .0182 Teamwork and Collaboration .4517
Self-confidence .0375
Transparency .0485
The fact that the relationships between many of the EI levels and ratings were not
positive does not necessarily indicate that they were related in a negative direction.
There are six possible relationships (1 < 2 < 3; 3 < 2 < 1; 2 < 1 < 3; 3 < 1 < 2; 2 < 3 < 1;
and 1 < 3 < 2).
Research Question Two: How do emotional intelligence competency scores of the
sample compare with emotional intelligence competency scores of other samples
surveyed using the same survey instrument?
Null Hypotheses 2 (H0-1) 1- (H0-16) (See list in Null Hypotheses 2 above): There
are no significant differences among 16 emotional intelligence competency scores of the
93
sample and 16 emotional intelligence competency scores of other groups surveyed using
the same survey instrument.
Alternative Hypotheses 2 (H1-1) 1- (H1-16) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 2
above): Significant differences exist among 16 emotional intelligence competency scores
of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence competency scores of other groups surveyed
using the same survey instrument.
Leaders of the organization in the study used the emotional competence
framework of the ECI 2.0. The study assesses 18 EI competencies as described in
Primal Leadership (Boyatzis, Goleman, and McKee, 2002). The framework of the
earlier version of the ECI, which includes 20 competencies, and the dissertation
research study share 16 EI competencies. The matching competencies measure the
same characteristics; however, they may have different variances. The researcher
compared the norms of these 16 competencies to address the second null hypothesis.
The researcher compared the “total others” EI mean scores of the study
sample with the “total others” EI mean scores of 5,360 people in the ECI North
American Database (Sala, 2002), using a T-test. Based on a 95-percent confidence
level (P< .05), findings indicated there are statistically significant differences between
the sample and those in the database in three competencies: Achievement, Accurate
Self-assessment, and Emotional Self-control. Therefore, the researcher rejects the
Null Hypotheses 2 for (H0-1), (H0-3), and (H0-10) in favor of the Alternative
Hypotheses 2 for (H1-1), (H1-3), and (H1-10). The researcher does not reject the Null
Hypotheses 2 for the remaining 13 common EI competencies: (H0-2), (H0-4), (H0-5)
(H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-8) (H0-9), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-13), (H0-14), (H0-15), and (H0-16).
94
The Table 4.4 shows P values for the comparison between the average EI
scores in the study with the norms compiled for the ECI North American Database.
Table 4.4 EI comparisons of bank sample with other samples
Reject Null Hypotheses Do Not Reject Null Hypotheses
Competency P Value= Competency P Value=
Achievement .0216 Adaptability .6518
Accurate Self-assessment .0118 Conflict Management .4158
Emotional Self-control .008 Change Catalyst .2557
Customer Service Orientation .2471
Developing Others .8386
Empathy .074
Emotional Self-awareness .6629
Influence .4066
Initiative .9948
Inspirational Leadership .8307
Organizational Awareness .8145
Self-confidence .3574
Teamwork and Collaboration .1922
Research Question Three: Do various demographic characteristics (position, title,
gender, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
influence scores on 18 emotional intelligence competencies?
Null Hypotheses 3(H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor, manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 3 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
95
the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor,
manager, senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Using ANOVA, the researcher ascertained, with 95 percent confidence (P <.05),
that there are no statistically significant differences in EI levels among participants in the
five positions. The researcher does not reject Null Hypotheses 3 for the 18 competencies:
(H0-1), (H0-2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-8) (H0-9), (H0-10), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-
13), (H0-14), (H0-15), (H0-16), (H0-17), and (H0-18). (See Table 4.5 below and Table H2,
Appendix H).
Table 4.5 Relationship of EI competencies and positions at 95 percent confidence level
Competency P Value=
Achievement .9649
Adaptability .9217
Accurate Self-assessment .0849
Conflict Management .9126
Change Catalyst .3277
Customer Service Orientation .741
Developing Others .8555
Empathy .1228
Emotional Self-awareness .1181
Emotional Self-control .503
Influence .9654
Initiative .5869
Inspirational Leadership .9293
Organizational Awareness .8301
Optimism .7636
Self-confidence .5039
Transparency .4764
Teamwork and Collaboration .5029
Null Hypotheses 4 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
96
with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice president, area vice
president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 4 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice
president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
Using ANOVA, the researcher ascertained, with 95 percent confidence (P <.05),
that there are no statistically significant differences in EI levels among participants with
the various titles. The researcher does not reject Null Hypotheses 4 for the 18
competencies: (H0-1), (H0-2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-8) (H0-9), (H0-10), (H0-
11), (H0-12), (H0-13), (H0-14), (H0-15), (H0-16), (H0-17), and (H0-18). (See Table 4.6 below and
Table H3, Appendix H).
Table 4.6 Relationship of EI competencies and titles at 95-percent confidence level
Competency P Value=
Achievement .7059
Adaptability .8385
Accurate Self-assessment .8805
Conflict Management .6434
Change Catalyst .1032
Customer Service Orientation .9835
Developing Others .6161
Empathy .2854
Emotional Self-awareness .7848
Emotional Self-control .7914
Influence .6732
Initiative .8754
Inspirational Leadership .7247
97
Table 4.6 continued
Organizational Awareness .4189
Optimism .6307
Self-confidence .6286
Transparency .9255
Teamwork and Collaboration .6511
The findings from the analyses of Null Hypotheses 3 and Null Hypotheses 4
are consistent with findings from a study of EI and employees in varying levels of
positions (Sala, 2002). And even though Cavallo and Brienza (2002) found some
significant differences among participants in various functional areas at Johnson &
Johnson, they reported that the number of participants in each functional category
was not sufficient to draw conclusions.
Null Hypotheses 5 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
men and women in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 5 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of men and women in the sample.
The researcher employed an ANOVA procedure to determine if there were
statistically significant differences between males and females. As indicated in Table 4.7
(also see Table H4, Appendix H), results indicated that there are no significant
differences (95 percent confidence level, P <.05) in EI scores of men and women in the
sample. The researcher does not reject the Null Hypotheses 5 for the 18 competencies:
98
(H0-1), (H0-2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-8) (H0-9), (H0-10), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-
13), (H0-14), (H0-15), (H0-16), (H0-17), and (H0-18).
Table 4.7 Relationship of gender and EI a 95 percent confidence level
Competency P Value=
Achievement .5887
Adaptability .4974
Accurate Self-assessment .9782
Conflict Management .6209
Change Catalyst .132
Customer Service Orientation .6241
Developing Others .8023
Empathy .1747
Emotional Self-awareness .3251
Emotional Self-control .8577
Influence .8611
Initiative .9942
Inspirational Leadership .405
Organizational Awareness .6563
Optimism .6919
Self-confidence .2069
Transparency .9112
Teamwork and Collaboration .5037
While some emotion- and EI-related studies (Hart, 2002, Sala, 2002, and Petrides
and Furnham, 2000) have noted gender differences, others (Sala, 2002 and Cavallo and
Brienza, 2002) have found little or no differences between the sexes.
The literature on gender and EI is mixed. While some emotion- and EI-related
studies (Hart, 2002, Sala, 2002, and Petrides and Furnham, 2000) have noted gender
differences, others (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; Landau, 1996; Eagly, Karau, and
99
Makhijani, 1992, 1995; Ragins, 1991) have found little or no differences between the
sexes.
The researcher ranked the mean scores for each competency for females and
males. Table 4.8 shows mean scores by gender in descending order from highest to
lowest. Although the difference between the highest and lowest mean scores is less than
1, one cannot surmise that the difference is small. Finding indicated that most data likely
lie between 3.867 and 2.967 for females and 3.887 and 2.932 for males.
Table 4.8 Means scores by gender in descending order
Females Males
Competency Mean = Competency Mean =
Customer Service Orientation 3.867 Customer Service Orientation 3.887
Self-confidence 3.83 Optimism 3.75
Optimism 3.72 Self-confidence 3.73
Empathy 3.692 Organizational Awareness 3.696
Organizational Awareness 3.662 Influence 3.597
Influence 3.582 Teamwork & Collaboration 3.587
Emotional Self-control 3.558 Emotional Self-control 3.579
Achievement 3.548 Empathy 3.577
Teamwork & Collaboration 3.534 Achievement 3.504
Adaptability 3.517 Developing Others 3.482
Developing Others 3.507 Transparency 3.48
Inspirational Leadership 3.502 Adaptability 3.457
Transparency 3.469 Change Catalyst 3.419
Emotional Self-awareness 3.436 Initiative 3.41
Initiative 3.411 Inspirational Leadership 3.407
Accurate Self-assessment 3.397 Accurate Self-assessment 3.4
Change Catalyst 3.262 Emotional Self-awareness 3.34
Conflict Management 2.967 Conflict Management 2.932
100
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
The researcher again used ANOVA to determine if differences existed between
managers working in sales functions and those working in support functions. With 95
percent confidence (P <.05), the researcher determined that a statistically significant
difference is evident in the three competencies as listed below. The researcher rejects the
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-8) (H0-9), and (H0-17) in favor of the Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-
8) (H1-9), and (H1-17) for the three competencies (see Table 4.9).
Table 4.9 Significant relationships between EI and area
(Sales and Support) at 95 percent confidence level
Competency P Value = t Value =
Empathy .0352 2.16
Emotional Self-awareness .0435 2.07
Transparency .0164 2.48
The positive t values of each of the three competencies indicate that Support
scores were greater than Sales scores. The researcher does not reject Null Hypotheses 6
for the remaining 13 competencies: (H0-1), (H0-2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-
10), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-13), (H0-14), (H0-15), (H0-16), and (H0-18).
Sala (2002) reports on norms by job function. Even though statistical tests were
not performed to compare groups, the data indicated that those in sales functions were
101
rated highest in Customer Service Orientation (mean score of 3.8818) and Self–
confidence (mean score of 3.77). Sala described a study by Lloyd at Bass Brewers in the
U.K. in 2001, which sought to determine if EI competencies were associated with sales
performance. Results of the Lloyd study indicated a strong relationship between EI
scores; the area development managers who performed best were more likely to have
higher EI scores.
Managers in Support functions in the sample scored highest and lowest in the
competencies shown in Table 4.10 (also see Table 4.12).
Table 4.10 Support managers’ highest- and lowest-rated competencies
Highest-rated competencies Lowest-rated competencies
Customer Service Orientation (3.905) Conflict Management (3.008)
Optimism (3.783) Change Catalyst (3.39)
Self-confidence (3.757) Accurate Self-assessment (3.463)
Organizational Awareness (3.752) Initiative (3.464)
Managers in Sales functions in the sample scored highest and lowest in the
competencies shown in 4.11 (also see Table 4.12).
Table 4.11 Sales managers’ highest- and lowest-rated competencies
Highest-rated competencies Lowest-rated competencies
Customer Service Orientation (3.882) Conflict Management (2.929)
Self-confidence (3.758) Emotional Self-awareness (3.32)
Optimism (3.73) Accurate Self-assessment (3.371)
Organizational Awareness (3.674) Initiative (3.392)
102
Table 4.12 Comparisons of EI means for Sales and Support functions
Competency Sales
Ranking
Sales
Mean =
Support
Ranking
Support
Mean =
Accurate Self-assessment 16 3.371 16 3.463
Achievement 9 3.491 9 3.609
Adaptability 11 3.448 11 3.571
Change Catalyst 14 3.392 17 3.39
Conflict Management 18 2.929 18 3.008
Customer Service Orientation 1 3.882 1 3.905
Developing Others 10 3.478 12 3.532
Emotional Self-awareness 17 3.316 13 3.497
Emotional Self-control 8 3.544 6 3.678
Empathy 7 3.564 5 3.727
Influence 5 3.59 7 3.636
Initiative 15 3.392 15 3.464
Inspirational Leadership 13 3.409 14 3.493
Optimism 3 3.73 2 3.783
Organizational Awareness 4 3.674 4 3.752
Self-confidence 2 3.758 3 3.757
Teamwork & Collaboration 6 3.572 10 3.592
Transparency 12 3.431 8 3.631
The rankings were similar between the groups. Again, although the difference
between the highest and lowest mean scores is less than 1, one cannot surmise that the
difference is small. Findings indicate that most data likely lie between 3.8819 and 2.9289
for managers in Sales and 3.905 and 3.008 for managers in Support functions.
Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
103
Alternative Hypotheses 7 (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1 above): Significant
differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of managers
in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more than five
years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for fewer than
five years.
The researcher compared the two groups using ANOVA and found, with 95
percent confidence (P <.05), that no statistically significant differences existed. The
researcher does not reject Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1), (H0-2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6),
(H0-7), (H0-8), (H0-9), (H0-10), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-13), (H0-14), (H0-15), (H0-16), (H0-17), and
(H0-18). (See Table H6, Appendix H).
Table 4.13 Relationship of EI and tenure a 95-percent confidence level
Competency P Value=
Achievement .8233
Adaptability .8138
Accurate Self-assessment .5121
Conflict Management .6651
Change Catalyst .6941
Customer Service Orientation .8315
Developing Others .0601
Empathy .7089
Emotional Self-awareness .5223
Emotional Self-control .6835
Influence .6462
Initiative .7622
Inspirational Leadership .8112
Organizational Awareness .1515
Optimism .7123
Self-confidence .4035
Transparency .8958
Teamwork and Collaboration .991
104
Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers without
advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s degree).
Alternative Hypotheses 8 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers
without advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s
degree).
The researcher analyzed the data with a T-test to ascertain, with 95 percent
confidence (P <.05), that no statistically significant differences existed within the sample
based on educational level. The researcher does not reject Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1), (H0-
2), (H0-3), (H0-4), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7), (H0-8), (H0-9), (H0-10), (H0-11), (H0-12), (H0-13), (H0-14),
(H0-15), (H0-16), (H0-17), and (H0-18). (See Table H7, Appendix H).
Table 4.14 Relationship of EI and educational level at 95 percent confidence level
Competency P Value=
Achievement .0.376
Adaptability .0.564
Accurate Self-assessment 0.433
Conflict Management 0.182
Change Catalyst 0.862
Customer Service Orientation 0.788
Developing Others 0.825
Empathy 0.888
Emotional Self-awareness 0.882
Emotional Self-control 0.911
Influence 0.478
Initiative 0.861
Inspirational Leadership 0.495
105
Table 4.14 continued
Organizational Awareness 0.971
Optimism 0.841
Self-confidence 0.614
Transparency 0.439
Teamwork and Collaboration 0.58
Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with Bachelor of
Arts degrees.
Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
Again deploying a T-test, the researcher analyzed of the data to determine that,
with 95 percent confidence (P <.05), statistically significant differences existed between
those with a Bachelor of Science degree and those with a Bachelor of Arts degree on four
competencies: Conflict Management, Emotional Self-awareness, Initiative, and
Organizational Awareness. The t values in the table below (Table 4.15) indicate the
direction of the relationship. The negative values indicated that scores of those with BS
degrees were greater than scores of those with BA degrees (Conflict Management,
Initiative, and Organizational Awareness) while the positive value for Emotional Self-
106
awareness indicated that scores of those with BA degrees were greater than those with BS
degrees.
The researcher rejects Null Hypotheses 9 for (H0-4), (H0-9), (H0-12), and (H0-14) in
favor of Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H1-4), (H1-9), (H1-12), and (H1-14). The researcher does
not reject Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1), (H0-2) (H0-3), (H0-5) (H0-6), (H0-7) (H0-8), (H0-10), (H0-
11), (H0-13), (H0-15), (H0-16) (H0-17), and (H0-18). (See Table H8, Appendix H).
Table 4.15 EI scores significantly related to BA versus BS degrees
Competency P Value = t Value =
Conflict Management .0214 -2.39
Emotional Self-awareness .0271 2.29
Initiative .0431 -2.09
Organizational Awareness .0422 -2.1
Additional findings: self versus others ratings
Another finding of note relates to self-ratings compared to ratings by total others
(managers, direct reports, peers, and customers). The scored data provided by the Hay
Group included self ratings as well as total others’ ratings. Even though the researcher
employed “total others’” ratings in the study, she was able to compare self versus others’
ratings. She found, with 95 percent confidence (P <.05), that participants tended to rate
themselves lower than others rated them. (See Table H9, Appendix H).
Results indicated that statistically significant differences existed in four
competencies: Emotional Self-control, Influence, Inspirational Leadership, and Self-
confidence. Negative t values indicated that for each of the four competencies, total
others’ scores were greater than self-reported scores. See Table 4.16.
107
Table 4.16 Statistically significant differences
Competency P Value = t Value =
Emotional Self-control .0008 -3.53
Influence .0404 -2.1
Inspirational Leadership .0011 -3.45
Self-confidence .0099 -2.67
The findings do not support findings reported by the Hay Group. Self ratings of
those included in the ECI North American Database tended to be higher than ratings of
total others (Sala, 2002). “People clearly seem to view themselves more favorably than
others view them; however, the moderate correlation between self and total others’
ratings suggest that ratings tend to be aligned. That is, those that rate themselves higher
tend to be rated higher by others” (p. 32).
The researcher also ranked and compared the mean EI scores for the sample from
highest to lowest for self and total others’ ratings. Total others’ mean scores ranged from
a high of 3.885 (Customer Service Orientation) to a low of 2.947 (Conflict Management).
Self ratings ranged from 3.7 (Customer Service Orientation) to 2.83 (Conflict
Management). The rankings were similar between the groups (see Table 4.17).
Table 4.17 EI competency scores for self and total others’ ratings from highest to lowest
Competency Others
ranking
Others
mean
Self
ranking
Self
mean
Achievement 9 3.517 9 3.451
Adaptability 12 3.475 12 3.372
Accurate Self-assessment 15 3.399 11 3.39
Conflict Management 18 2.947 18 2.83
Change Catalyst 16 3.390 14 2.358
Customer Service Orientation 1 3.885 1 3.7
Developing Others 10 3.491 12 3.372
Empathy 5 3.606 5 3.542
108
Table 4.17 continued
Emotional Self-awareness 17 3.365 8 3.479
Emotional Self-control 7 3.581 15 3.274
Influence 6 3.598 10 3.423
Initiative 14 3.411 16 3.258
Inspirational Leadership 13 3.43 17 3.13
Organizational Awareness 4 3.693 4 3.551
Optimism 3 3.747 2 3.698
Self-confidence 2 3.755 6 3.525
Transparency 11 3.484 3 3.567
Teamwork & Collaboration 8 3.577 7 3.507
The following competencies had the same ranking for both self and others:
Competency Ranking
Customer Service Orientation 1
Organizational Awareness 4
Empathy 5
Achievement 9
Adaptability 12
Conflict Management 18
Although the differences between the highest and lowest mean scores for self and
total others scores are less than 1, one cannot surmise that the difference is small. Finding
indicated that most data likely lie between 3.885 and 2.947 (see Table 4.17).
Summary
The analysis addressed research questions and null hypotheses posed to determine
if high performance ratings and high emotional intelligence were related in a sample; if
EI scores of the sample were similar to those of others surveyed using the same
instrument; and if various demographic characteristics of the sample (gender, position,
title, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
109
influenced emotional intelligence. Table 4.18 shows a summary of the research question
and null hypothesis analysis.
Table 4.18 Summary of null and alternative hypothesis testing
Test Analysis Hypotheses Reject/Not Reject
(95 % confidence level; P<.05) 1. Cross-
intersection
Test
High performance
ratings and high
scores on each of the
18 emotional
intelligence
competencies are not
related.
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 1 for the 18
competencies.
At 90 percent confidence level (P<.1),
reject Null Hypotheses 1 in favor of
Alternative Hypotheses 1 for:
Achievement (P=.0547)
Adaptability (P=.0838)
Optimism (P=.01675)
At 90 percent confidence level,
do not reject (H0) 1 for remaining 15
competencies.
2. T-test There are no
significant differences
among 16 emotional
intelligence
competency scores of
the sample and 16
emotional intelligence
he competency scores
of other groups
surveyed using the
same survey
instrument.
Reject Null Hypotheses 2 in favor of
Alternative Hypotheses 2 for:
Achievement (P=.0216)
Accurate Self-assessment (P=.0118)
Emotional Self-control (P=.008)
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 2 for the
remaining 13 competencies.
3. ANOVA There are no
significant differences
in the emotional
intelligence scores of
managers in the
sample in different
positions (those
classified as individual
contributor, supervisor,
manager, senior
manager, and
executive) on each of
18 competencies.
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 3 for the 18
EI competencies.
110
Table 4.18 continued
4. ANOVA There are no significant
differences in the emotional
intelligence scores of
managers in the sample
with various titles (assistant
vice president, vice
president, senior vice
president, area vice
president, and
executive/group vice
president) on each of 18
competencies.
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 4 for the
18 EI competencies.
5.
ANOVA
There are no significant
differences in each of the
18 emotional intelligence
competency scores of men
and women in the sample.
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 5 for the
18 EI competencies.
6. ANOVA There are no significant
differences in each of the
18 emotional intelligence
competency scores of
managers in sales and
managers in support roles
in the sample.
Reject the Null Hypotheses 6 in favor of
the Alternative Hypotheses 6 for:
Empathy (P=.0352) (Support > Sales)
Emotional Self-awareness (P=.0435)
(Support > Sales)
Transparency (P=.0164) (Support >
Sales)
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 6 for the
remaining 15 competencies.
7. ANOVA There are no significant
differences in each of the
18 emotional intelligence
competency scores of
managers in the sample
who have been in
management roles with
direct reports for more than
five years and those who
have been in management
roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 7 for the
18 EI competencies.
8. T-test There are no significant
differences in each of the
18 emotional intelligence
competency scores of
managers with advanced
degrees (master’s degree
or higher) and managers
without advanced degrees
(those with a high school
diploma and those with a
bachelor’s degree).
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 8 for the
18 EI competencies
111
Table 4.18 continued
9. T-test There are no significant
differences in each of the
18 emotional intelligence
competency scores of
managers in the sample
with Bachelor of Science
degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
Reject Null Hypotheses 9 in favor of
Alternative Hypotheses 9 for:
Conflict Management (P=.021) (BS>BA)
Emotional Self-awareness (P=.027)
(BA>BS)
Initiative (P=.043)(BS>BA)
Organizational Awareness.
(P=.042)(BS>BA)
Do not reject Null Hypotheses 9 for the
remaining 14 competencies.
High ratings and high emotional intelligence were not significantly related at a 95
percent confidence level. They were significantly related on three competencies—
Achievement, Adaptability, and Optimism—at a 90 percent confidence level. The EI
average scores of the sample differed significantly from the average scores of other
groups in the ECI North American Database on three competencies: Achievement,
Accurate Self-assessment, and Emotional Self-control. Statistically significant
differences between Sales and Support functions were evident in three competencies:
Empathy, Emotional Self-awareness, and Transparency. For all three, Support EI levels
were greater than Sales EI levels. Position, title, gender, and management tenure did not
significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
While educational level did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of
the sample, statistically significant differences existed between those with a Bachelor of
Science degree and those with a Bachelor of Arts degree on four competencies: Conflict
Management, Emotional Self-awareness, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness. EI
levels of those with BS degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BA degrees in
Conflict Management, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness while EI levels of those
112
with BA degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BS degrees on one
competency: Emotional Self-
awareness.
Results of the self versus total others ratings indicated that statistically significant
differences exist in four competencies: Emotional Self-control, Influence, Inspirational
Leadership, and Self-confidence. For each of the four competencies, EI levels of total
others were greater than self-reported EI levels.
113
Chapter V Summary, Conclusions, and Recommendations
Summary
The purpose of this study was to determine if differences in EI levels existed
among high performers in one organization. Specifically, the study assessed whether
managers in this financial organization who are deemed most effective, according to
their performance ratings, exhibited higher emotional intelligence competencies than
managers with lower performance ratings. The researcher also compared EI levels
and various demographic characteristics of the sample, including, position, title, area
of employment, gender, education level, type of degree, and management tenure.
In a study of emotional intelligence and leadership performance, it is important to
examine the concepts related to emotions, emotional intelligence, leadership, leadership
theories, and the relationship between emotional intelligence and leadership.
The literature indicated that as companies increasingly are required to do more
with less, seemingly “soft” skills, based on emotions, are associated with leadership
effectiveness and organizational success. Research suggested that emotional
“competencies” such as those related to empathy, adaptability, self-control, emotional
self-awareness, ability to develop others, and so forth, contribute significantly to leader
effectiveness.
The analysis addressed research questions and null hypotheses posed to determine
if performance had an influence on emotional intelligence in a sample; if EI scores of the
sample were similar to those of others surveyed using the same instrument; and if various
demographic characteristics of the sample (gender, position, title, sales or support role,
114
management tenure, educational level, and type of degree) influenced emotional
intelligence.
Research Questions and Hypotheses
The researcher conducted data analysis to address the following research
questions and null hypotheses.
Research Question One: Do high-performing leaders exhibit high emotional
intelligence?
Null Hypotheses 1 (H0-1)-(H0-18): High performance ratings and high scores on
each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies listed below are not related:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Developing Others
(H0-8): Empathy
(H0-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-10): Emotional Self-control
(H0-11): Influence
(H0-12): Initiative
(H0-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-14): Organizational Awareness
115
(H0-15): Optimism
(H0-16): Self-confidence
(H0-17): Transparency
(H0-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
Alternative Hypotheses 1 (H1-1) – (H1-18.): High performance ratings and high
scores on each of the 18 emotional intelligence competencies are related:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Developing Others
(H1-8): Empathy
(H1-9): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-10): Emotional Self-control
(H1-11): Influence
(H1-12): Initiative
(H1-13): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-14): Organizational Awareness
(H1-15): Optimism
(H1-16): Self-confidence
(H1-17): Transparency
116
(H1-18): Teamwork and Collaboration
Research Question Two: How do emotional intelligence competency scores of the
sample compare with emotional intelligence competency scores of other samples
surveyed using the same survey instrument?
Null Hypotheses 2(H0-1)-(H0-16): There are no significant differences among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence he
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H0-1): Achievement
(H0-2): Adaptability
(H0-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H0-4): Conflict Management
(H0-5): Change Catalyst
(H0-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H0-7): Empathy
(H0-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H0-9): Emotional Self-control
(H0-10): Influence
(H0-11): Initiative
(H0-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H0-13): Organizational Awareness
(H0-14): Optimism
(H0-15): Self-confidence
(H0-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
117
Alternative Hypotheses 2 (H1-1)-(H1-16): Significant differences exist among 16
emotional intelligence competency scores of the sample and 16 emotional intelligence
competency scores of other groups surveyed using the same survey instrument:
(H1-1): Achievement
(H1-2): Adaptability
(H1-3): Accurate Self-assessment
(H1-4): Conflict Management
(H1-5): Change Catalyst
(H1-6): Customer Service Orientation
(H1-7): Empathy
(H1-8): Emotional Self-awareness
(H1-9): Emotional Self-control
(H1-10): Influence
(H1-11): Initiative
(H1-12): Inspirational Leadership
(H1-13): Organizational Awareness
(H1-14): Optimism
(H1-15): Self-confidence
(H1-16): Teamwork and Collaboration
Research Question Three: Do various demographic characteristics (position, title,
gender, sales or support role, management tenure, educational level, and type of degree)
influence scores on 18 emotional intelligence competencies?
118
Null Hypotheses 3 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor, manager,
senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 3 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample in different positions (those classified as individual contributor, supervisor,
manager, senior manager, and executive) on each of 18 competencies.
Null Hypotheses 4 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in the emotional intelligence scores of managers in the sample
with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice president, area vice
president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18 competencies.
Alternative Hypotheses 4 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist among emotional intelligence scores of managers in
the sample with various titles (assistant vice president, vice president, senior vice
president, area vice president, and executive/group vice president) on each of 18
competencies.
Null Hypotheses 5 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
men and women in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 5 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of men and women in the sample.
119
Null Hypotheses 6 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Alternative Hypotheses 6 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in sales and managers in support roles in the sample.
Null Hypotheses 7 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct reports for
fewer than five years.
Alternative Hypotheses 7 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample who have been in management roles with direct reports
for more than five years and those who have been in management roles with direct
reports for fewer than five years.
Null Hypotheses 8 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers without
advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s degree).
Alternative Hypotheses 8 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers with advanced degrees (master’s degree or higher) and managers
120
without advanced degrees (those with a high school diploma and those with a bachelor’s
degree).
Null Hypotheses 9 (H0-1)-(H0-18) (See list in Null Hypotheses 1 above): There are
no significant differences in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency scores of
managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with Bachelor of
Arts degrees.
Alternative Hypotheses 9 (H1-1)-(H1-18) (See list in Alternative Hypotheses 1
above): Significant differences exist in each of the 18 emotional intelligence competency
scores of managers in the sample with Bachelor of Science degrees and those with
Bachelor of Arts degrees.
To address research questions and null and alternative hypotheses, the researcher
employed a comparative research design to investigate differences among managers with
varying demographic characteristics in one financial institution. An existing survey
instrument, Hay/McBer’s Emotional Competence Inventory 2.0, and existing
performance appraisal system and rating forms were used to ascertain if differences
existed among the sample. Data were analyzed with descriptive statistics and univariate
analysis of variance.
Key Findings
The following key findings are derived from the results of the analysis of research
questions and null hypothesis testing.
1. High performance ratings and high emotional intelligence were not related at a
95-percent confidence level. At a 90-percent confidence level, the two were related in
three competencies: Achievement, Adaptability, and Optimism.
121
2. The EI average scores of the sample differed significantly from the average
scores of other groups in the ECI North American Database in three competencies:
Achievement, Accurate Self-assessment, and Emotional
Self-control.
3. Position did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
4. Title did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
5. Gender did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
6. Significant differences existed between those in sales and support in three
competencies: Empathy, Emotional Self-awareness, and Transparency. For all three,
Support EI levels were greater than Sales EI levels.
7. Management tenure (a comparison of managers with direct reports for more
than five years and those who have had direct reports for fewer than five years) did not
significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
8. Educational level did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the
sample.
9. Significant differences existed between those in the sample with BS degrees
and those in the sample with BA degrees in four competencies: Conflict Management,
Emotional Self-awareness, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness. EI levels of those
with BS degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BA degrees in Conflict
Management, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness while EI levels of those with BA
degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BS degrees in Emotional Self-
awareness.
10. Significant difference existed between self-ratings and total others ratings in
four competencies: Emotional Self-control, Influence, Inspirational Leadership, and Self-
122
confidence. For each of the four competencies, EI levels of total others were greater than
self-reported EI levels.
Discussion of Key Findings
The following findings and subsequent discussion were formed based on the
findings of the study and on the analysis described in Chapter IV.
Discussion of Finding 1. High performance ratings and high emotional
intelligence were not related at a 95-percent confidence level. At a 90-percent confidence
level, the two were related in three competencies: Achievement, Adaptability, and
Optimism.
Before employing the union-intersection test to determine if higher ratings were
related to higher EI scores, the researcher ran an ANOVA test to ascertain if a
relationship existed between performance ratings and EI scores. Findings indicated that a
statistically significant relationship existed between performance ratings and EI scores in
10 competencies—Achievement, Adaptability, Conflict Management, Developing
Others, Emotional Self-control, Initiative, Inspirational Leadership, Optimism, Self-
confidence, and Transparency. However, further analysis on the direction of the
relationship indicated that there is no statistically significant relationship at the 95 percent
confidence level between high ratings and high emotional. High scores and high
competency levels were significantly related in a positive direction in three
competencies—Achievement, Adaptability, and Optimism—at a 90 percent confidence
level.
Interestingly, the three competencies—Achievement, Adaptability, and
Optimism—are included in the six-competency Self-management EI cluster. Boyatzis,
123
Goleman, and McKee (2002) describe Self-management: “From self-awareness—
understanding one’s emotions and being clear about one’s purpose—flows self-
management, the focused drive that all leaders need to achieve their goals” (p. 45). They
added that self-management “is the component of emotional intelligence that frees us
from being a prisoner of our feelings. It’s what allows the mental clarity and
concentrated energy that leadership demands, and what keeps disruptive emotions from
throwing us off track. Leaders with such self-mastery embody an upbeat, optimistic
enthusiasm that tunes resonance to the positive range” (p. 46).
Achievement is defined as “striving to improve or meeting a standard of
excellence” (Hay Group, 2002c). Adaptability is defined as “flexibility in handling
change.” An Optimistic leader can “roll with the punches, seeing an opportunity rather
than a threat in a setback” (Boyatzis, Goleman, and McKee, 2002, p. 255).
Two of the three competencies (Achievement and Adaptability) are among those
cited by McClelland (1998), in a study of leaders in 30 organizations. He determined that
the most powerful leadership differentiators were Self-confidence, Achievement Drive,
Developing Others, Adaptability, Influence, and Leadership.
It is logical that managers with higher scores in the Achievement competency
would have higher ratings. One might assume that higher scores in achievement are
linked to higher performance. The ability to handle change (adaptability) would
seemingly enable the managers to be able to adapt well to changes in requirements of
superiors who are evaluating them. Optimistic leaders would be expected to handle
changes and difficulties in a positive manner.
124
Discussion of Finding 2. The EI average scores of the sample differed
significantly from the average scores of other groups in the ECI North American
Database in three competencies: Achievement, Accurate Self-assessment, and Emotional
Self-control.
The sample was limited to managers. Those in the database were not limited to
managers. Therefore, if a premise of the study—that high EI levels are linked to higher
performance ratings in management—holds true, one expects there to be differences
between the sample and the database. Results indicate that differences indeed exist in
three competency levels.
Discussion of Findings 3 and 4. Position did not significantly impact the
emotional intelligence of the sample. Title did not significantly impact the emotional
intelligence of the sample.
Although the researcher had no expectations about the results of the analysis of
null hypotheses 3 and 4, she included it in the study as another dimension for segmenting
the sample to determine if varying characteristics impacted EI competencies. Other
research, such as a study at Johnson & Johnson by Cavallo and Brienza (2002), has
looked for significant differences among participants in various functional areas.
Although Cavallo and Brienza (2002) found significant differences, they reported that the
number of participants in each functional category was not sufficient to draw
conclusions.
On the surface, position and title do not appear to be related to performance or EI.
Results indicated they do not impact EI in the sample.
125
Discussion of Finding 5. Gender did not significantly impact the emotional
intelligence of the sample.
The literature on gender and EI is mixed. While some emotion- and EI-related
studies (Hart, 2002, Sala, 2002, and Petrides and Furnham, 2000) have noted gender
differences, others (Cavallo and Brienza, 2002; Landau, 1996; Eagly, Karau, and
Makhijani, 1992, 1995; Ragins, 1991) have found little or no differences between the
sexes.
Based on gender differences such as those noted by Hart (2002), the researcher
expected differences in competencies related to emotion and social skills. Hart cites a
study indicating that a woman’s brain is better suited than a man’s for sensing,
processing, and remembering emotions is. One EI study (Petrides and Furnham, 2000)
revealed that females scored higher than males on social scales. This study found
significant differences between genders in the Emotional Self-control competency.
Discussion of Finding 6. Significant differences existed between those in sales
and support in three competencies: Empathy, Emotional Self-awareness, and
Transparency. For all three, Support EI levels were greater than Sales EI levels.
Again, the researcher included functional area in the study as a dimension for
segmenting the sample to determine if varying characteristics impacted EI competencies.
Managers in both Support and Sales functions in the sample organization scored among
the highest four scores in Customer Service Orientation, Optimism, Self-confidence, and
Organizational Awareness. Having managers with relatively high EI scores in these
areas, particularly Customer Service Orientation, seems very beneficial for a financial
126
institution like the one in the study. Managers in both areas scored among the lowest four
scores on Accurate Self-assessment, Conflict Management, and Initiative.
Findings indicated that the impact of function (Sales or Support) is not
statistically significant in the other 15 competencies. Forty study participants indicated
that they worked in Sales while 14 cited Support as their area of employment.
Discussion of Finding 7. Management tenure (a comparison of managers with
direct reports for more than five years and those who have had direct reports for fewer
than five years) did not significantly impact the emotional intelligence of the sample.
Although the researcher was not aware of a reference to management tenure and
its potential impact on emotional intelligence, she included the analysis as another
dimension for segmenting the sample to determine if varying characteristics impacted EI
competencies. It is possible to expect that people who had managed others for long
periods of time would become increasingly in tune with their leadership behaviors and
how others with whom they work perceive them. This study found no statistically
significant differences between management tenure and EI.
Discussion of Finding 8. Educational level did not significantly impact the
emotional intelligence of the sample.
Based the literature citing that EI is as important as IQ and other qualities, such as
technical skills, to effective performance (Goleman, 1998b; Cooper and Sawaf, 1997;
Cherniss and Adler, 2000; Cherniss, 2000; and Druskat, 2001), one would not expect
formal education to impact emotional intelligence competencies.
In particular, Goleman (1998b) contends that effective leaders in today’s
organizations are not necessarily the most intelligent or highly skilled ones.
127
Discussion of Finding 9: Significant differences existed between those in the
sample BS degrees and those in the sample with BA degrees in four competencies:
Conflict Management, Emotional Self-awareness, Initiative, and Organizational
Awareness. EI levels of those with BS degrees were greater than EI levels of those with
BA degrees in Conflict Management, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness while EI
levels of those with BA degrees were greater than EI levels of those with BS degrees in
Emotional Self-awareness.
As explained in the discussion of finding 9, one would not expect formal
education, including type of degree, to impact emotional intelligence competencies. In
this sample, perhaps Conflict Management, Initiative, and Organizational Awareness are
more closely related to the strengths of those with BS degrees than those with BA
degrees while Emotional Self-awareness is more closely related to strengths of those with
BA degrees than those with BS degrees.
Discussion of Finding 10: Significant difference existed between self-ratings and
total others ratings in four competencies: Emotional Self-control, Influence, Inspirational
Leadership, and Self-confidence. For each of the four competencies, EI levels of total
others were greater than self-reported EI levels.
Sala (2002) reported that self-ratings of those included in the ECI North American
Database tended to be higher than ratings of total others seem logical. He contended that
people viewed themselves more favorably than others viewed them. One would expect
high-performing leaders to exhibit self-confidence, thereby rating themselves relatively
highly on EI competencies. It is possible that halo effects affected the others’ ratings in
128
the sample. The managers in the sample also may, to some extent, be modest about their
abilities.
Limitations
The recommendations for leaders and organizations interested in implementing EI
programs as a way to increase leader awareness of EI and to improve leader and
organizational effectiveness are based on the findings of this study. It is important to
note that the study was exploratory with limitations that may influence its reflection of
general practice. The sample in the study was limited to 57 of 79 high-performing or
potentially high-performing managers participating in an internal EI program in one
financial institution. The sample of 57 managers included volunteers from the
management pool selected by a committee to participate in an internal study of emotional
intelligence. Of the 79 in the bank’s study, 20 executive-level participants did not have
performance ratings, and, therefore, did not qualify for the doctoral research study. Two
managers did not volunteer for the study. Conducting this study with a larger sample size
should help strengthen the
findings.
Further, even though the same five-point ratings system is used for all employees,
performance of managers in the sample was evaluated with two instruments, one of
which is sometimes modified by managers in various departments and divisions. Use of a
consistent performance appraisal instrument could help strengthen the study findings.
However, performance appraisal may not be effective in discriminating between
effective and ineffective performers (Frechette and Wertheim, 1985). Frechette and
Wertheim concluded that one appraisal format should not be expected to cover a
multitude of purposes. They also pointed out a number of potential problems inherent in
129
performance appraisal systems. Others problems included rater errors, biases, and lack of
credibility and validity.
Recommendations
According to one of the organizational supporters of the EI study, its purpose was
to increase awareness of leaders and emerging leaders of their leadership behaviors as
related to the EI competency framework and as perceived by others with whom they
work. A goal was to encourage leaders to take responsibility for their learning and to use
the survey data to help guide further leadership development.
The sponsors were interested in ascertaining information about proficient and
deficient leadership behaviors of participants and investigating organizational and
business impacts of the behaviors. “The information has helped introduce the concept of
leadership competencies that is paving the way for a thoughtful and thorough discussion
at the executive level about our company’s strategic vision to develop talent within the
organization as a business strategy. These results are currently being blended into
competencies for leaders, managers, and supervisors. Appropriate training and
educational opportunities will be matched to the competencies identified through the
competency modeling project that is currently under way” (manager of management and
leadership development, personal communication, September 27, 2002).
It is important to note that although the researcher identified significant findings
while analyzing research questions and hypotheses, design limitations of the study restrict
the full understanding of the role of EI in leadership effectiveness. Therefore, in addition
to proposing continuing research, the researcher is making recommendations based on the
literature review and the value of an integrated human resources strategy. Further, one
130
element of a human resources strategy, such as selection or performance assessment, can
not function in isolation. Selection strategies combined with effective professional
development and coordinated performance assessment provide the best prospect for
leadership development and organizational effectiveness.
In addition, organizational sponsors should consider the following
recommendations:
Selection:
1. Use the ECI framework as part of the criteria for selecting candidates who have
potential to succeed in leadership roles.
2. Include the ECI framework in the organization’s selection process as part of a
behavioral/situational interviewing process. For example, human resources
representatives could include EI descriptors in interviews to elicit demonstration
of specific competencies.
Development:
1. Include pertinent EI competencies in processes to determine promotions and
design succession plans.
2. Add language to the bank’s performance appraisal system documentation and
instruments to help define and detail measurable, stated objectives from the ECI
framework.
Training:
1. Add EI training to the organization’s leadership development program.
2. Assess if EI training impacts organizational effectiveness.
131
3. Ascertain if competencies in which the overall sample scored lowest are
necessary for success in the organization and, if so, design training programs to
address weaknesses.
The ECI may be used to evaluate individuals within an organization and the
organization overall (Hay Group, 2002a). Results provide precise and focused feedback
on an individual’s strengths and limitations. Based on the feedback from various rater
groups, the ECI helps identify the specific emotional competencies in which development
is needed to enhance the individual’s emotional intelligence. The ECI may be used to
diagnose an organizational unit, profiling overall strengths and weaknesses.
Emotionally intelligent leaders may help organizations build competitive
advantages that result in benefits such as the following (Hay Group, 2002b):
Increased performance
Enhanced innovation
Effective leaders
Effective use of time and resources
More teamwork
Improved motivation
Restored trust
Recommendations for Future Research
This study examined whether ratings and specific demographic characteristics
impact EI competency levels of a select group of managers in one financial institution.
The study’s findings form the basis for additional research. The following are
suggestions for future investigation:
132
1. Expand the study with a larger sample within the same organization and with
samples in similar organizations, such as banks, security firms, and insurance
agencies, to compare findings.
2. Conduct the study in organizations outside of the financial sector and compare
findings.
3. Develop and implement an EI training program for sample participants and re-
survey the sample following training to determine if training has an impact of EI
levels in the sample.
4. Assess if the EI training has an impact on organizational effectiveness.
133
Final Comments
Results of the study show minimal ties between high levels of EI and high levels
of performance (as determined via performance ratings) in this sample. However, it is
important to note that all managers in the sample are considered high performers. All
received, at a minimum, scores of three out of five for the past two performance review
cycles, and there may be little variation among the three rating groups. They were
selected to participate in the internal EI program because of their high performance
and/or high potential.
Findings from this study generate questions such as the following:
1. How effective is the performance appraisal system used to distinguish managers
in the sample who received ratings of three versus four versus five. What
objective data separates the three rating groups? Do subjective criteria bias the
rater?
2. What criteria of the performance appraisal system are and are not reflected in the
EI competencies?
3. Would the objectively stated criteria from the EI competencies be important data
to include in an objectively oriented performance appraisal system?
4. Was this sample too restricted to ascertain the relationship between high EI and
high performance ratings?
Additional studies are needed to determine if managers scoring high in EI
competencies are actually more effective than managers with lower scores in the EI
competencies are. However, this organization’s leaders are implementing an EI program
internally, and, therefore, support the principles of EI. As the manager of management
134
and leadership development explained, a goal of the program is to generate “thoughtful
and thorough discussion at the executive level about our company’s strategic vision to
develop talent within the organization as a business strategy.”
As the literature pointed out, attracting, training, and retaining leaders high in
emotional intelligence might help organizations gain a competitive edge. Bank leaders
may consider implementing programs to identify and enhance levels of emotional
competencies in managers as one way to respond to changing times.
135
Reference List
Abraham, R. (1999). Emotional Intelligence in Organizations: A Conceptualization.
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 125(2), 209-224.
Bar-On, R. (2000). Emotional and social intelligence: insights from the Emotional
Quotient Inventory. In R. Bar-On and J.S.A. Parker (Eds.). The Handbook of
Emotional Intelligence (pp. 363-388). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Batten, J. (1989). Tough-minded leadership. New York: AMACOM, a Division of
American Management Association.
Blank, W. (1995). The 9 Natural Laws of Leadership. New York: AMACOM Books.
Boyatzis, R. (1994). Stimulating self-directed change: A required MBA course called
managerial assessment and development. Journal of Management Education,
18(3), 304-323.
Boyatzis, R., Baker, A., Leonard, D., Rhee, K., & Thompson, L. (1995). Will it make a
difference? Assessing a value-based, outcome-oriented, competency-based
professional program. In Boyatzis, R., Cowen, S., & Kolb, D. (Eds.). Innovating
in professional education: Steps on a journey from teaching to learning. San
Francisco: Josses-Bass.
Boyatzis, R., Cowen, S., & Kolb, D. (1995). Innovation in professional education: Steps
on a journey from teaching to learning. San Francisco: Josses-Bass.
Boyatzis, R., Goleman, D.& McKee, A. (2002). Primal leadership: Realizing the power
of emotional intelligence.
Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
Boyatzis, R., Leonard, D., Rhee, K., & Wheeler, J. (1996). Competencies can be
developed, but not the way we thought. Capability, 2(2), 25-41.
Boyatzis, R., Wheeler, J., & Wright, R. (in press). Competency development in graduate
education: A longitudinal perspective. Proceedings of the First World Conference
on Self-Directed Learning. Montreal: GIRAT.
136
Burckle. M. (1999). Can you assess your own emotional intelligence? Evidence
supporting multi-rater assessment. Hay/McBer Research Report.
Burckle. M. (2000). ECI and MBTI. Hay/McBer Research Report.
Capezio, P. and Morehouse, D. (1997). Secrets of Breakthrough Leadership. New Jersey:
The Career Press.
Cavallo, K. & Brienza, D., Emotional competence and leadership excellence at Johnson
& Johnson: The emotional intelligence and leadership study. Retrieved from
http://www.eiconsortium.org/research/jj_ei_study.htm on October 28, 2002.
Cacioppo, J., &.Gardner, W. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology. 1999. 50
(1), 191-214.
Chatterjee, C. (2000). Emotional ignorance. Psychology Today. 33(6), 12.
Cherniss, C. The business case for emotional intelligence. Retrieved November 5, 2002,
from (http://www.eiconsortium.org/research/business_case_for_ei.htm).
Cherniss, C. (2000). What is emotional intelligence and why it matters? Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Industrial and Organizational
Psychology, New Orleans, LA, April 15, 2000. Retrieved October 14, 2002, from
(http://www.eiconsortium.org/research/what_is_emotional_intelligence.htm).
Cherniss, C., & Adler, M. (2000). Promoting emotional intelligence in organizations.
Alexandria, VA: American Society for Training & Development.
Cherniss, C., & Goleman, D. (1998). A technical report issued by the Consortium for
Research on Emotional Intelligence in Organizations. The Consortium for
Research on Emotional Intelligence in Organizations. Retrieved on November 6,
2002, from http://www.eiconsortium.org/research/technical_report.htm.
137
Cooper. R. and Sawaf, A. (1997). Executive EQ. Emotional intelligence in leadership and
organizations. New York: The Berkley Publishing Group.
Cornella, K. (2000, October). E-mail correspondence from the HayGroup.
Creswell, J. (1994). Research design: Qualitative & quantitative approaches. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Crosby, P. (1988). The Eternally successful organization: The art of corporate wellness.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Diamantopoulou, M. (2001). An investigation of type A and type B behavior patterns in
relation with the emotional intelligence of bank employees in their working
environment. An unpublished Doctoral dissertation. DEI-Middlesex University
of London, Department of Social Psychology.
Druskat, V. (2001, March). Building the emotional intelligence of groups. Harvard
Business Review, 79 (3), 80.
Eagly, A. & Johnson, B. (1990.) Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin. 108 (2), 233-256)
Eagly, A., Karau, S. & Makhijani, M. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 3-22.
Eagly, A., Karau, S. & Makhijani, M. (1995). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (1), 125-145.
EI Consortium (2002). Retrieved from
http://www.eiconsortium.org/measures/wpi_ei.htm on October 16, 2002.
Fineman, S. (Ed.) (1993). Emotion in organizations. London: Sage.
138
Frechette, H. & Wertheim, E. (1985). Performance appraisal. In W.R. Tracey (Ed.)
Human Resources Management & Development (pp. 218-243). New York:
American Management Associations.
Forgas, J. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 39-66.
Frechette, H. and Wertheim, E. (1985). Performance appraisal. In W.R. Tracey (Ed.).
Human Resources Management & Development (pp. 218-243). New York:
American Management Associations.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences. The theory in practice. New York: Basic
Books.
Garson, G. (2002). ANOVA. Retrieved from
http://www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/pa765/anova.htm on October 16, 2002.
George, J. (1995). Leader positive mood and group performance: The case of customer
service. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299-307.
George, J. (2000). Emotions and leadership: The role of emotional intelligence. Human
Relations, 53(8), 1027-1055.
George, J. & Battenhausen, K. (1990). Understanding prosocial behavior, sales
performance, and turnover: A group-level analysis in a service context. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 112, 310-329.
Goleman, D. (1998a) What makes a leader? Harvard Business Review, 76(6), 93-104
Goleman, D. (1998b). Working with emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Gordon, J. (1998, November). My leader, myself: Faux freethinkers and the new cult of
the CEO. TRAINING Magazine, 35 (11), 54.
139
Grote, R. (1996). The complete guide to performance appraisal. New York: AMACOM
Books.
Hampton, D., Summer, C., & Webber, R. (1987). Organizational behavior and the
practice of management. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman, and Company.
Hart, B. (2002, July 28). Men and women should celebrate differences. Salt Lake City,
UT: The Deseret News Publishing Co.
Hay Group (2002a). How can the ECI be used? Retrieved October 14, 2002, from
http://ei.haygroup.com/products_and_services/content_assessment_tools.html.
Hay Group (2002b). How has emotional intelligence made an impact on the bottom line?
Retrieved October 14, 2002, from “EI in Organizations” at
http://ei.haygroup.com/about_ei/
Hay Group (2002c). The emotional competency framework. Retrieved October 15, 2002
from “Framework” at http://ei.haygroup.com/about_ei/.
Hay/McBer (1996). Generic competency dictionary. Boston: Hay/McBer.
Hay/McBer (2002). Emotional Competency Inventory. Boston: Hay/McBer (used with
permission).
Hickman, C., & Silva, M. (1984). Creating excellence: Managing corporate culture,
strategy, and change in the new age. New York: New American Library.
Humphrey, R., Kellett, J. & Sleeth, R. (2001). Emotional competence, complex task
choice, and leadership emergence. Unpublished paper: Virginia Commonwealth
University, School of Business.
Kotter, J. (1998). What leaders really do. Harvard Business Review on Leadership.
Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
140
Kouzes, J. & Posner, B. (1997). The leadership challenge: How to keep getting
extraordinary things done in organizations. San Francisco: John Wiley & Sons, Inc
Landau, J. (1996). The relationship of race and gender to managers’ ratings of promotion
potential. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16 (4), 391-400.
Mayer, J. & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence: Implications for
educators. In P. Salovey and D. Sluyter (Eds.) Emotional development, emotional
literacy, and emotional intelligence. New York: Basic Books.
Mayer, J., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (1998). Competing models of emotional intelligence. In R.J.
Sternberg (Ed.) Handbook of human intelligence (2nd ed.). New York:
Cambridge University Press, 3-31.
Mayer J., & Cobb, C. (2000). Educational policy on emotional intelligence: Does it make
sense? Educational Psychology Review, 12 (2), 163-183.
McClelland, D. (1973). Testing for competence rather than for intelligence. American
Psychologist, 28, 1-14.
McClelland, D. (1998). Identifying competencies with behavioral event interviews.
Psychological Science, 9(5), 331-340.
Naisbitt, J., & Aburdene, P. (1985). Re-inventing the Corporation. New York: Warner
Books.
Peters, T., & Waterman R. (1982). In search of excellence: Lessons from America’s best-
run companies. New York: Harper & Row Publishers.
Petrides, K., & Furnham, A. (2000, March). Gender differences in measured and self-
estimated trait emotional intelligence. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research 42(5-6),
449-461.
Ragins, B. (1991). Gender effects in subordinate evaluations of leaders: Real or artifact?
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12(3), 259-268.
141
Roberts, W. (1987). Leadership secrets of Attila the Hun. New York: Warner Books.
Rodriguez, D., Patel, R., Bright, A., Gregory, D., & Gowing, M. (2002). Developing
competency models to promote integrated human resources practices. Human
Resources Management, 41(3), 309-324.
Rosier, R. & Jeffrey, P. (1994). The competence model handbook (Volume 1). Boston:
Linkage Incorporated.
Rosier, R. & Jeffrey, P. (1995). The competence model handbook (Volume 2). Boston:
Linkage Incorporated.
Rosier, R. (Ed.). (1994-1997). The competency model handbook: Volumes 1-4.
Lexington, MA: Linkage Incorporated.
Sala, F. (2001). Do programs designed to increase emotional intelligence at work–work?
Retrieved October 10, 2002, from http://www.eiconsortium.org/
Sala, F. (2002). Emotional competency inventory technical manual. Hay Acquisition
Company. (e-mail correspondence from author).
Salovey, P. & Mayer, J. (1990). Emotional Intelligence. Imagination, cognition, and
personality, 9(3), 185-211.
Schulman, P. (1995). Explanatory style and achievement in school and work. In G.
Buchanan, M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.) Explanatory Style. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Sevinc, L. (2001). The effect of emotional intelligence on career success: Research on the
1990 graduates of Business Administration Faculty of Istanbul University.
Unpublished Master Thesis: Istanbul University.
Schutte, N., Malouff, J., Hall, L., Haggerty, D., Cooper, J., Golden, C., & Dornheim, L.
(1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence.
Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177.
142
Spencer, L. & Spencer, S. (1993). Competence at work: Models for superior
performance. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
UCLA Department of Statistics. Statistics Handout 5. Retrieved on November 6, 2002,
from http://www.stat.ucla.edu/~vie//ANOVA .
Truskie, S. (1999). Leadership in high-performance organizational cultures. Westport,
Conn.: Quorum Books.
U.S. Office of Personnel Management. (1996). Mosaic competencies for professionals
and administrators. Washington, DC: Author.
Yukl, G. (1994). Leadership in Organizations. Third Edition. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall
Inc.
Zaleznik, A. (1998). Managers and Leaders: Are They Different? Harvard Business
Review on Leadership. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
143
Appendix A Emotional intelligence leadership competencies.
Emotional intelligence leadership competencies (Boyatzis, Goleman, and McKee,
2002, pp. 253-256).
Table A1 Self-awareness competencies
Emotional self-awareness. Leaders high in this competence:
Are attuned to their inner signals, recognizing how their feelings affect
them and their performance
Are attuned to their guiding values and often see the big picture in a
complex situation
Can be candid and authentic
Accurate self-assessment. Leaders high in this competence:
Know their strengths and weaknesses
Exhibit a sense of humor about themselves
Welcome constructive criticism and feedback
Know when to ask for help
Know where to focus to acquire new leadership strengths
Self-confidence. People with this competence:
May welcome a difficult assignment
Have a sense of presence (self assurance)
Are able to play to their strengths
144
Table A2 Self-management competencies
Self-control. Managing disruptive emotions and impulses.
Manage their impulsive feelings and disturbing emotions
Stay composed, positive, and unflappable even in trying moments
Think clearly and stay focused under pressure
Transparency. Authentic openness to others about one’s feelings, beliefs, and
actions. People with this competency:
Live their values others
Admit mistakes or faults
Confront unethical behaviors in
Adaptability. Flexibility in handling change. People with this competency:
Smoothly handle multiple demands without losing focus or energy
Are comfortable with inevitable ambiguities of organizational life
Are flexible in adapting to new challenges, nimble in adjusting to change, and limber in
their thinking
Achievement. Leaders high in the achievement competency:
Have high standards that drive them to constantly seek performance improvements
Are pragmatic, setting measurable and challenging goals; they are able to calculate risk
so that goals are worthy yet attainable
Are continually learning and teaching ways to improve
Initiative. Leaders with this competence:
Have a sense of efficacy—that they are able to control their own destiny
Seize or create opportunities
Do not hesitate to cut through red tape or bend the rules to create better possibilities
Optimism. Leaders high in the optimism competency:
Roll with the punches; they see an opportunity rather than a threat in a setback
See others positively, expecting the best of others
Have a “glass-half-full” outlook, which leads them to expect that changes in the future
will be for the better
145
Table A3 Social Awareness competencies
Empathy. Leaders high in the empathy competency:
Are attentive to a wide range of emotional signals
Listen attentively and can grasp others’ perspectives
Are able to get along well with diverse people
Organizational awareness. A leader with a keen organizational
awareness can:
Be politically astute, able to detect crucial social networks and key power relationships
Understand the political forces at work as well as the guiding values and unspoken rules
Service. Leaders with a high service competency:
Foster an emotional climate so that people directly in touch with customers will keep the relationship on track
Monitor customer satisfaction
Make themselves available as needed
146
Table A4 Relationship management competencies
Inspiration. Leaders with this competency
Inspire others, create resonance and move people with a compelling
vision or shared mission
Embody what they ask of others
Can articulate a shared mission in a way to inspire others
Offer a sense of common purpose and help make work exciting
Influence. People with this competency:
Find the right appeal for a given listener
Know how to build buy-in from key people and a network of support for
an initiative
Are persuasive and engaging
Developing others: Leaders with this competency:
Show a genuine interest in those they are helping, understanding their
goals, strengths, and weaknesses
Are able to provide timely and constructive feedback
Are natural mentors and coaches
Change catalyst. People with this competency:
Recognize the need for change
Challenge the status quo to acknowledge the need for change
Champion the change and enlist others in its pursuit
Find ways to overcome barriers to change
Conflict management. Leaders with this competency:
Draw out all parties
Understand differing perspectives
Find a common ideal that everyone can endorse
Surface the conflict, acknowledge the feelings and views of all
sides, and redirect the energy toward a shared ideal
Teamwork and collaboration. Leaders with this competency:
Generate an atmosphere of friendly collegiality
Are models of respect, helpfulness, and cooperation
Draw others into active, enthusiastic commitment to the collaborative
effort
Build spirit and identify
Forge and cement close relationships beyond work obligations
147
Appendix B ECI
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
Appendix C Chronbach’s alpha coefficients
Chronbach’s alpha coefficients for self and total others ECI ratings from the Hay
North American Database. Scores based on average item scores.
Table C Chronbach’s alpha coefficients
Competency Total Others Rating
(N=3,931)
Alpha Coefficient
Self Rating
(N=4,001) ECI
Alpha Coefficient
Emotional Self-awareness .74 .61
Accurate self-Assessment .83 .68
Self-confidence .88 .80
Self-control .89 .78
Trustworthiness .73 .74
Conscientiousness .90 .81
Adaptability .77 .60
Achievement Orientation .87 .78
Initiative .83 .72
Empathy .92 .81
Org. Awareness .84 .75
Service Orientation .91 .85
Developing Others .88 .77
Leadership .80 .69
Influence .83 .73
Communication .86 .77
Change Catalyst .91 .84
Conflict Management .86 .75
Building Bonds .84 .75
Teamwork & Collaboration .91 .81
Self-awareness .76 .61
Self-management .88 .79
Social Awareness .81 .71
Social Skills .96 .92
158
Appendix D Test-retest stability
Test-retest stability coefficients for Self and Total Others ECI ratings with
Brazilian consumer retail executives (N=20).
Table D Test-retest stability coefficients
Competency Total Others Rating
Stability Coefficients
Self Rating
Stability Coefficients
Emotional Self-awareness .55 .23
Accurate Self-assessment .58 .26
Self-confidence .69 .33
Self-control .49 .43
Trustworthiness .67 .22
Conscientiousness .92 .56
Adaptability .52 .55
Achievement Orientation .60 .19
Initiative .45 .15
Empathy .62 .61
Organizational Awareness .82 .22
Service Orientation .41 .05
Developing Others .75 .55
Leadership .56 .47
Influence .19 .30
Communication .56 .08
Change Catalyst .69 .35
Conflict Management .39 .43
Building Blocks .72 .44
Teamwork & Collaboration .57 .82
159
Appendix E Sales Appraisal Form Sales Appraisal Form
Confidential
160
Appraisal Detail Form – Area Executive Appraisal Detail Form – Area Executive
161
162
163
164
165
2002 Sales Appraisal Form Workbook
Instructions
166
2002 Sales Appraisal Workbook Instructions
167
2002 Sales Appraisal Workbook Instructions2002 Sales Appraisal Workbook Instructions
168
Appendix F Support Appraisal Checklist
169
170
171
172
173
Appendix G Demographics
Study Demographic Information
1. What is your position at the Company?
Individual Contributor
Supervisor
Manager
Senior Manager
Executive
2. What is your title?
Assistant Vice President
Vice President
Senior Vice President
Executive/Group Vice President
No title
Other
3. In which area of the Company do you work?
Sales
Support
Other (please name)
4. Gender:
Male
Female
5. Education (please check all that apply):
High School or equivalent
Bachelor of Science
Bachelor of Arts
Advanced degree (master’s degree or higher)
6. Have you been in a management position with direct reports for
more than five years or
less than five years?
7. What was the rating on your 2002 performance appraisal?
1
2
3
4
5
Participant Code: __________________
174
Appendix H Statistical analysis
Table H1 ANOVA for Ratings
Competency ID NAME
SOURC
E TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.5358847469 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.6856078933 7.2997848745 0.001562982
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.6856078933 7.2997848745 0.001562982
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.2749000305 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.6921352237 5.706327175 0.005646296
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.6921352237 5.706327175 0.005646296
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.3043828707 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.0901432729 0.5654395625 0.571437495
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.0901432729 0.5654395625 0.571437495
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.1862437933 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.3382253087 4.1770654138 0.020571706
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.3382253087 4.1770654138 0.020571706
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.7489429023 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.506155152 2.8777328736 0.064928497
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.506155152 2.8777328736 0.064928497
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 0.796925776 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.0186902927 0.6332307452 0.534769608
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.0186902927 0.6332307452 0.534769608
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.5469236174 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.7445209454 5.6674650187 0.005830488
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.7445209454 5.6674650187 0.005830488
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.5239522914 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.0603071723 0.4620646131 0.6324492
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.0603071723 0.4620646131 0.6324492
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.1896935063 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.4459869983 2.8741121364 0.065141304
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.4459869983 2.8741121364 0.065141304
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 5.8217337648 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.7553339565 3.5030830418 0.037114254
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.7553339565 3.5030830418 0.037114254
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.2452514974 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.3448037066 2.8687145159 0.06545989
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.3448037066 2.8687145159 0.06545989
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.2586874905 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.5085364617 4.2135014499 0.01993308
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.5085364617 4.2135014499 0.01993308
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 5.2868930947 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others rating SS1 2 1.1029984974 5.6329793123 0.005999154
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others rating SS3 2 1.1029984974 5.6329793123 0.005999154
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.587368478 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.1309798263 1.3668154886 0.263594211
175
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.1309798263 1.3668154886 0.263594211
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.0870906189 . .
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.3336252218 4.3159989834 0.018244515
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.3336252218 4.3159989834 0.018244515
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.5765527733 . .
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.3330888579 3.4904773758 0.03753078
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.3330888579 3.4904773758 0.03753078
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.7610466387 . .
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.4460309675 3.2019906368 0.048513352
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.4460309675 3.2019906368 0.048513352
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.9154021244 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others rating SS1 2 0.0870849676 0.8065076533 0.451718166
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others rating SS3 2 0.0870849676 0.8065076533 0.451718166
176
Table H2 ANOVA for Position
Obs Competency ID NAME SOURCE TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.17854 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others position SS1 4 0.03588 0.14392 0.96487
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others position SS3 4 0.03588 0.14392 0.96487
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.89688 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others position SS1 4 0.06954 0.22752 0.92174
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others position SS3 4 0.06954 0.22752 0.92174
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.72758 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others position SS1 4 0.63600 2.17540 0.08492
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others position SS3 4 0.63600 2.17540 0.08492
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 2.43870 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others position SS1 4 0.04646 0.24291 0.91264
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others position SS3 4 0.04646 0.24291 0.91264
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 4.78360 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others position SS1 4 0.44535 1.18702 0.32771
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others position SS3 4 0.44535 1.18702 0.32771
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 0.78113 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others position SS1 4 0.03019 0.49284 0.74097
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others position SS3 4 0.03019 0.49284 0.74097
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 4.10256 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others position SS1 4 0.10668 0.33155 0.85548
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others position SS3 4 0.10668 0.33155 0.85548
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.11699 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others position SS1 4 0.46724 1.91124 0.12276
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others position SS3 4 0.46724 1.91124 0.12276
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 4.02061 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others position SS1 4 0.61137 1.93876 0.11815
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others position SS3 4 0.61137 1.93876 0.11815
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 6.11700 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others position SS1 4 0.40562 0.84545 0.50299
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others position SS3 4 0.40562 0.84545 0.50299
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.48619 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others position SS1 4 0.03901 0.14265 0.96543
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others position SS3 4 0.03901 0.14265 0.96543
177
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.51276 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others position SS1 4 0.19644 0.71301 0.58687
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others position SS3 4 0.19644 0.71301 0.58687
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 6.19729 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others position SS1 4 0.10422 0.21441 0.92925
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others position SS3 4 0.10422 0.21441 0.92925
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 2.64199 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others position SS1 4 0.07631 0.36826 0.83013
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others position SS3 4 0.07631 0.36826 0.83013
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 2.33014 . .
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others position SS1 4 0.08434 0.46151 0.76359
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others position SS3 4 0.08434 0.46151 0.76359
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 2.71850 . .
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others position SS1 4 0.17993 0.84387 0.50394
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others position SS3 4 0.17993 0.84387 0.50394
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 3.92826 . .
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others position SS1 4 0.27438 0.89057 0.47638
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others position SS3 4 0.27438 0.89057 0.47638
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 51 2.80129 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others position SS1 4 0.18580 0.84566 0.50286
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others position SS3 4 0.18580 0.84566 0.50286
178
Table H3 ANOVA for Titles
Obs Competency ID NAME SOURCE TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.99526 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others title SS1 6 0.22623 0.62942 0.70594
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others title SS3 6 0.22623 0.62942 0.70594
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 3.76195 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others title SS1 6 0.20508 0.45429 0.83851
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others title SS3 6 0.20508 0.45429 0.83851
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 4.19693 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others title SS1 6 0.19760 0.39235 0.88048
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others title SS3 6 0.19760 0.39235 0.88048
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.32637 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others title SS1 6 0.19809 0.70960 0.64340
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others title SS3 6 0.19809 0.70960 0.64340
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 4.28878 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others title SS1 6 0.96631 1.87760 0.10318
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others title SS3 6 0.96631 1.87760 0.10318
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 0.79923 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others title SS1 6 0.01638 0.17082 0.98346
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others title SS3 6 0.01638 0.17082 0.98346
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 3.93929 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others title SS1 6 0.35216 0.74497 0.61615
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others title SS3 6 0.35216 0.74497 0.61615
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 3.10837 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others title SS1 6 0.47589 1.27582 0.28539
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others title SS3 6 0.47589 1.27582 0.28539
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 4.35978 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others title SS1 6 0.27590 0.52735 0.78484
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others title SS3 6 0.27590 0.52735 0.78484
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 6.19172 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others title SS1 6 0.38535 0.51864 0.79141
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others title SS3 6 0.38535 0.51864 0.79141
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 3.32241 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others title SS1 6 0.26764 0.67131 0.67319
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others title SS3 6 0.26764 0.67131 0.67319
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 3.59462 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others title SS1 6 0.17260 0.40014 0.87543
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others title SS3 6 0.17260 0.40014 0.87543
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 5.95711 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others title SS1 6 0.43278 0.60541 0.72469
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others title SS3 6 0.43278 0.60541 0.72469
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.42009 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others title SS1 6 0.29826 1.02702 0.41893
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others title SS3 6 0.29826 1.02702 0.41893
179
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.22672
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others title SS1 6 0.19400 0.72603 0.63070
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others title SS3 6 0.19400 0.72603 0.63070
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.67567 . .
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others title SS1 6 0.23397 0.72871 0.62864
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others title SS3 6 0.23397 0.72871 0.62864
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 4.05321
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others title SS1 6 0.15386 0.31634 0.92547
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others title SS3 6 0.15386 0.31634 0.92547
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 50 2.76993 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others title SS1 6 0.23255 0.69963 0.65112
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others title SS3 6 0.23255 0.69963 0.65112
180
Table H4 ANOVA for Gender
Obs Competency ID NAME
SOURC
E TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.15343 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.01728 0.29597 0.58866
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.01728 0.29597 0.58866
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.78377 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.03271 0.46681 0.49738
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.03271 0.46681 0.49738
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.39278 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00006 0.00075 0.97820
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00006 0.00075 0.97820
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.33973 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.01072 0.24742 0.62092
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.01072 0.24742 0.62092
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 5.02505 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.21767 2.33916 0.13199
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.21767 2.33916 0.13199
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 0.80283 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00361 0.24289 0.62412
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00361 0.24289 0.62412
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.25004 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00498 0.06328 0.80234
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00498 0.06328 0.80234
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.33951 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.11698 1.89160 0.17470
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.11698 1.89160 0.17470
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.45321 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.08132 0.98609 0.32513
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.08132 0.98609 0.32513
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 6.42722 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00386 0.03245 0.85772
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00386 0.03245 0.85772
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.55415 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00204 0.03093 0.86106
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00204 0.03093 0.86106
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.76480 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00000 0.00005 0.99419
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00000 0.00005 0.99419
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 6.23456 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.08131 0.70425 0.40506
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.08131 0.70425 0.40506
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.66472 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00988 0.20024 0.65632
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00988 0.20024 0.65632
181
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.39560 . .
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00704 0.15870 0.69193
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00704 0.15870 0.69193
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.79047 . .
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.08432 1.63176 0.20693
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.08432 1.63176 0.20693
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.09124 . .
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.00095 0.01256 0.91117
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.00095 0.01256 0.91117
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.97749 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others gender SS1 1 0.02498 0.45312 0.50373
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others gender SS3 1 0.02498 0.45312 0.50373
182
Table H5 ANOVA for Area
Obs Competency ID NAME SOURCE TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.97956 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others area SS1 1 0.15147 2.69432 0.10663
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others area SS3 1 0.15147 2.69432 0.10663
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 3.71232 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others area SS1 1 0.16493 2.35470 0.13085
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others area SS3 1 0.16493 2.35470 0.13085
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 4.21716 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others area SS1 1 0.09095 1.14298 0.28987
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others area SS3 1 0.09095 1.14298 0.28987
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.42944 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others area SS1 1 0.06880 1.50100 0.22593
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others area SS3 1 0.06880 1.50100 0.22593
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 5.23872 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others area SS1 1 0.00066 0.00663 0.93542
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others area SS3 1 0.00066 0.00663 0.93542
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 0.77499 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others area SS1 1 0.00563 0.38471 0.53775
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others area SS3 1 0.00563 0.38471 0.53775
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 4.23546 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others area SS1 1 0.03183 0.39827 0.53070
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others area SS3 1 0.03183 0.39827 0.53070
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 3.27908 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others area SS1 1 0.28915 4.67359 0.03517
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others area SS3 1 0.28915 4.67359 0.03517
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 4.26349 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others area SS1 1 0.34424 4.27933 0.04347
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others area SS3 1 0.34424 4.27933 0.04347
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 6.25993 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others area SS1 1 0.19592 1.65880 0.20336
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others area SS3 1 0.19592 1.65880 0.20336
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 3.52631 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others area SS1 1 0.02246 0.33758 0.56369
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others area SS3 1 0.02246 0.33758 0.56369
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 3.70573 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others area SS1 1 0.05736 0.82041 0.36916
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others area SS3 1 0.05736 0.82041 0.36916
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 6.30411 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others area SS1 1 0.07743 0.65100 0.42336
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others area SS3 1 0.07743 0.65100 0.42336
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.63680 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others area SS1 1 0.06658 1.33834 0.25251
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others area SS3 1 0.06658 1.33834 0.25251
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.37940 . .
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others area SS1 1 0.03023 0.67331 0.41558
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others area SS3 1 0.03023 0.67331 0.41558
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.89001 . .
183
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others area SS1 1 0.00002 0.00044 0.98331
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others area SS3 1 0.00002 0.00044 0.98331
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 3.76317 . .
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others area SS1 1 0.43661 6.14911 0.01636
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others area SS3 1 0.43661 6.14911 0.01636
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 53 2.94486 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others area SS1 1 0.00438 0.07875 0.78009
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others area SS3 1 0.00438 0.07875 0.78009
184
Table H6ANOVA for Tenure
Obs Competency ID NAME SOURCE TYPE DF SS F PROB
1 ECI2002ACH Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.18432 . .
2 ECI2002ACH Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00297 0.05036 0.82328
3 ECI2002ACH Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00297 0.05036 0.82328
4 ECI2002ADA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.96285 . .
5 ECI2002ADA Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00411 0.05601 0.81381
6 ECI2002ADA Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00411 0.05601 0.81381
7 ECI2002ASA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.07147 . .
8 ECI2002ASA Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.03283 0.43549 0.51211
9 ECI2002ASA Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.03283 0.43549 0.51211
10 ECI2002CFM Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.49387 . .
11 ECI2002CFM Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00875 0.18943 0.66512
12 ECI2002CFM Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00875 0.18943 0.66512
13 ECI2002CHC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.92573 . .
14 ECI2002CHC Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.01426 0.15630 0.69414
15 ECI2002CHC Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.01426 0.15630 0.69414
16 ECI2002CSO Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 0.80149 . .
17 ECI2002CSO Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00068 0.04573 0.83148
18 ECI2002CSO Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00068 0.04573 0.83148
19 ECI2002DEV Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.00251 . .
20 ECI2002DEV Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.27324 3.68649 0.06014
21 ECI2002DEV Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.27324 3.68649 0.06014
22 ECI2002EMP Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.52233 . .
23 ECI2002EMP Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00919 0.14084 0.70892
24 ECI2002EMP Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00919 0.14084 0.70892
25 ECI2002ESA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.58187 . .
26 ECI2002ESA Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.03520 0.41481 0.52226
27 ECI2002ESA Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.03520 0.41481 0.52226
28 ECI2002ESC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 6.45622 . .
29 ECI2002ESC Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.02009 0.16807 0.68346
30 ECI2002ESC Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.02009 0.16807 0.68346
31 ECI2002INFL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.57464 . .
32 ECI2002INFL Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.01411 0.21316 0.64616
33 ECI2002INFL Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.01411 0.21316 0.64616
34 ECI2002INI Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 3.72862 . .
35 ECI2002INI Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00638 0.09246 0.76224
36 ECI2002INI Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00638 0.09246 0.76224
37 ECI2002INL Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 6.27214 . .
38 ECI2002INL Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00669 0.05762 0.81121
39 ECI2002INL Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00669 0.05762 0.81121
40 ECI2002OA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.48934 . .
41 ECI2002OA Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.09757 2.11653 0.15151
42 ECI2002OA Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.09757 2.11653 0.15151
43 ECI2002OPT Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.41169 . .
44 ECI2002OPT Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00614 0.13740 0.71233
45 ECI2002OPT Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00614 0.13740 0.71233
46 ECI2002SC Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.83631 . .
185
47 ECI2002SC Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.03724 0.70905 0.40347
48 ECI2002SC Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.03724 0.70905 0.40347
49 ECI2002TRA Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 4.17179 . .
50 ECI2002TRA Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00134 0.01731 0.89581
51 ECI2002TRA Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00134 0.01731 0.89581
52 ECI2002TW Total_Others ERROR ERROR 54 2.91709 . .
53 ECI2002TW Total_Others tenure SS1 1 0.00001 0.00013 0.99104
54 ECI2002TW Total_Others tenure SS3 1 0.00001 0.00013 0.99104
186
Table H7 T-Test for Educational Level
(master’s degree versus high school and bachelor’s degrees)
Competency ID Variable Method Variances t Value DF Pr > |t|
1 ECI2002ACH Total Others Pooled Equal 0.89 55 0.3761
2 ECI2002ACH Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.85 10.8 0.4148
3 ECI2002ADA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.58 55 0.5639
4 ECI2002ADA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.57 11.1 0.5788
5 ECI2002ASA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.79 55 0.4333
6 ECI2002ASA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.76 10.9 0.4623
7 ECI2002CFM Total Others Pooled Equal 1.35 55 0.1818
8 ECI2002CFM Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 1.04 9.43 0.3252
9 ECI2002CHC Total Others Pooled Equal 0.18 55 0.8616
10 ECI2002CHC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.20 13 0.8427
11 ECI2002CSO Total Others Pooled Equal -0.27 55 0.7880
12 ECI2002CSO Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.26 10.7 0.8033
13 ECI2002DEV Total Others Pooled Equal 0.22 55 0.8248
14 ECI2002DEV Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.18 9.63 0.8623
15 ECI2002EMP Total Others Pooled Equal -0.14 55 0.8877
16 ECI2002EMP Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.11 9.27 0.9186
17 ECI2002ESA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.15 55 0.8824
18 ECI2002ESA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.12 9.83 0.9042
19 ECI2002ESC Total Others Pooled Equal 0.11 55 0.9111
20 ECI2002ESC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.12 11.7 0.9085
21 ECI2002INFL Total Others Pooled Equal 0.71 55 0.4780
22 ECI2002INFL Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.71 11.2 0.4917
23 ECI2002INI Total Others Pooled Equal 0.18 55 0.8607
24 ECI2002INI Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.18 11.4 0.8606
25 ECI2002INL Total Others Pooled Equal 0.69 55 0.4948
26 ECI2002INL Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.71 11.6 0.4889
27 ECI2002OA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.04 55 0.9709
28 ECI2002OA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.04 11.1 0.9716
29 ECI2002OPT Total Others Pooled Equal 0.20 55 0.8413
30 ECI2002OPT Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.19 10.8 0.8515
31 ECI2002SC Total Others Pooled Equal 0.51 55 0.6142
187
32 ECI2002SC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.72 18.3 0.4791
33 ECI2002TRA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.78 55 0.4390
34 ECI2002TRA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.64 9.79 0.5349
35 ECI2002TW Total Others Pooled Equal 0.56 55 0.5802
36 ECI2002TW Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.60 12 0.5617
188
Table H8 T-Test for BA and BS
Competency ID Variable Method Variances t Value DF Pr > |t|
1 ECI2002ACH Total Others Pooled Equal -0.61 41 0.5435
2 ECI2002ACH Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.63 31.5 0.5313
3 ECI2002ADA Total Others Pooled Equal -0.68 41 0.5006
4 ECI2002ADA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.74 35.8 0.4659
5 ECI2002ASA Total Others Pooled Equal 1.47 41 0.1496
6 ECI2002ASA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 1.53 32.1 0.1365
7 ECI2002CFM Total Others Pooled Equal -2.39 41 0.0214
8 ECI2002CFM Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -2.27 24.9 0.0320
9 ECI2002CHC Total Others Pooled Equal 0.23 41 0.8160
10 ECI2002CHC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.22 23.8 0.8288
11 ECI2002CSO Total Others Pooled Equal -0.81 41 0.4220
12 ECI2002CSO Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.79 26.8 0.4361
13 ECI2002DEV Total Others Pooled Equal -0.03 41 0.9740
14 ECI2002DEV Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.04 38.3 0.9709
15 ECI2002EMP Total Others Pooled Equal 0.50 41 0.6189
16 ECI2002EMP Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.49 27.1 0.6276
17 ECI2002ESA Total Others Pooled Equal 2.29 41 0.0271
18 ECI2002ESA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 2.79 40.7 0.0080
19 ECI2002ESC Total Others Pooled Equal -0.26 41 0.7928
20 ECI2002ESC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.30 38.3 0.7692
21 ECI2002INFL Total Others Pooled Equal -0.33 41 0.7397
22 ECI2002INFL Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.34 30.4 0.7352
23 ECI2002INI Total Others Pooled Equal -2.09 41 0.0431
24 ECI2002INI Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -1.95 23.7 0.0636
25 ECI2002INL Total Others Pooled Equal 0.08 41 0.9382
26 ECI2002INL Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.09 39.3 0.9300
27 ECI2002OA Total Others Pooled Equal -2.10 41 0.0422
28 ECI2002OA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -2.01 25.6 0.0550
29 ECI2002OPT Total Others Pooled Equal -0.24 41 0.8093
30 ECI2002OPT Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal -0.27 36.8 0.7912
31 ECI2002SC Total Others Pooled Equal 0.79 41 0.4340
189
32 ECI2002SC Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.80 29.8 0.4297
33 ECI2002TRA Total Others Pooled Equal 0.33 41 0.7431
34 ECI2002TRA Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.37 39.3 0.7100
35 ECI2002TW Total Others Pooled Equal 0.04 41 0.9716
36 ECI2002TW Total Others Satterthwaite Unequal 0.04 29.8 0.9713
Table H9 T-test for self and total others
Ob
s
Competency
ID
Variable
1
Variable
2 Difference
T
Value DF Probt
1 ECI2002ACH Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.87 56 0.3888
2 ECI2002ADA Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.27 56 0.2094
3 ECI2002ASA Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.15 56 0.8827
4 ECI2002CFM Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.62 56 0.1107
5 ECI2002CHC Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.39 56 0.6985
6 ECI2002CSO Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.87 56 0.0671
7 ECI2002DEV Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.35 56 0.1830
8 ECI2002EMP Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.78 56 0.4366
9 ECI2002ESA Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others 1.36 56 0.1795
10 ECI2002ESC Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -3.53 56 0.0008
11 ECI2002INFL Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -2.10 56 0.0404
12 ECI2002INI Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.95 56 0.0566
13 ECI2002INL Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -3.45 56 0.0011
14 ECI2002OA Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -1.68 56 0.0978
15 ECI2002OPT Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.65 56 0.5190
16 ECI2002SC Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -2.67 56 0.0099
17 ECI2002TRA Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others 0.97 56 0.3370
18 ECI2002TW Self Total_Others Self – Total_Others -0.80 56 0.4271
190
Appendix I Consent forms
191
North Carolina State University
INFORMED CONSENT FORM
Title of Study: Emotional Competencies of Leaders: A Comparison of Managers in One
Organization
Principal Investigator: Joni King Brooks Faculty Sponsor: Dr. James Burrow
You are invited to participate in a research study. The purpose of this study is to determine if a relationship exists
between a leader’s level of emotional intelligence and his or her workplace effectiveness in one company.
The purpose of the study is to assess if leaders in a company differ on levels of emotional intelligence. The
results of the ECI may be used to identify gaps in emotional intelligence, gaps which may be limiting
maximum effectiveness, and/or to design development/training programs. Previous research indicates that
benefits of emotionally intelligent organizations include: improved financial results, improved return on
investment, improved productivity, increased retention of top talent, and increased sales.
INFORMATION
1. List all procedures, preferably in chronological order, which will be employed in the study. Be sure to use lay
language.
Each manager and others with whom he or she works closely are being asked by the
organization’s management to complete the Emotional Competency Inventory. The data will be shared with
the researcher. The identity of each manager will remain completely confidential and unknown to the
researcher. The researcher will analyze the data aggregately from the two groups using a “key” provided by
the HayGroup to address hypotheses and research questions. Each participant is asked to complete a brief
demographic form and agree to the terms of this NCSU IRB consent form.
2. It is estimated that each survey requires 20 minutes to complete.
RISKS
There are no foreseeable risks or discomforts involved in this study.
BENEFITS
The results of the ECI may be used to identify gaps in emotional intelligence, gaps which may be limiting
maximum effectiveness, and/or to design development/training programs. Previous research indicates that benefits of
emotionally intelligent organizations include: improved financial results, improved return on investment, improved
productivity, increased retention of top talent, and increased sales.
CONFIDENTIALITY
The information in the study records will be kept strictly confidential. Data will be stored securely and will
be made available only to persons conducting the study unless you specifically give permission in writing to do
otherwise. No reference will be made in oral or written reports which could link you to the study.
COMPENSATION
There is no compensation for participating in this study.
CONTACT
If you have questions at any time about the study or the procedures, you may contact the researcher, Joni
King Brooks, at 11613 Trottenham St., Raleigh, NC 27614, jonibrooks@ipass.net, or (919-518-2081). If you feel you
have not been treated according to the descriptions in this form, or your rights as a participant in research have been
violated during the course of this project, you may contact Dr. Matthew Zingraff, Chair of the NCSU IRB for the Use
of Human Subjects in Research Committee, Box 7514, NCSU Campus (919/513-1834) or Mr. Matthew Ronning,
Assistant Vice Chancellor, Research Administration, Box 7514, NCSU Campus (919/513-2148)
PARTICIPATION
Your participation in this study is voluntary; you may decline to participate without penalty. If you decide to
participate, you may withdraw from the study at any time without penalty and without loss of benefits to which you are
otherwise entitled. If you withdraw from the study before data collection is completed your data will be returned to
you or destroyed.
192
CONSENT
I have read and understand the above information. I have received a copy of this form. I agree to participate
in this study.
Subject’s signature_______________________________________ Date _________________
Investigator’s signature__________________________________ Date _________________
UMI Number: 3070569
________________________________________________________
UMI Microform 3070569
300 North Zeeb Road
PO Box 1346
We provide professional writing services to help you score straight A’s by submitting custom written assignments that mirror your guidelines.
Get result-oriented writing and never worry about grades anymore. We follow the highest quality standards to make sure that you get perfect assignments.
Our writers have experience in dealing with papers of every educational level. You can surely rely on the expertise of our qualified professionals.
Your deadline is our threshold for success and we take it very seriously. We make sure you receive your papers before your predefined time.
Someone from our customer support team is always here to respond to your questions. So, hit us up if you have got any ambiguity or concern.
Sit back and relax while we help you out with writing your papers. We have an ultimate policy for keeping your personal and order-related details a secret.
We assure you that your document will be thoroughly checked for plagiarism and grammatical errors as we use highly authentic and licit sources.
Still reluctant about placing an order? Our 100% Moneyback Guarantee backs you up on rare occasions where you aren’t satisfied with the writing.
You don’t have to wait for an update for hours; you can track the progress of your order any time you want. We share the status after each step.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
Although you can leverage our expertise for any writing task, we have a knack for creating flawless papers for the following document types.
From brainstorming your paper's outline to perfecting its grammar, we perform every step carefully to make your paper worthy of A grade.
Hire your preferred writer anytime. Simply specify if you want your preferred expert to write your paper and we’ll make that happen.
Get an elaborate and authentic grammar check report with your work to have the grammar goodness sealed in your document.
You can purchase this feature if you want our writers to sum up your paper in the form of a concise and well-articulated summary.
You don’t have to worry about plagiarism anymore. Get a plagiarism report to certify the uniqueness of your work.
Join us for the best experience while seeking writing assistance in your college life. A good grade is all you need to boost up your academic excellence and we are all about it.
We create perfect papers according to the guidelines.
We seamlessly edit out errors from your papers.
We thoroughly read your final draft to identify errors.
Work with ultimate peace of mind because we ensure that your academic work is our responsibility and your grades are a top concern for us!
Dedication. Quality. Commitment. Punctuality
Here is what we have achieved so far. These numbers are evidence that we go the extra mile to make your college journey successful.
We have the most intuitive and minimalistic process so that you can easily place an order. Just follow a few steps to unlock success.
We understand your guidelines first before delivering any writing service. You can discuss your writing needs and we will have them evaluated by our dedicated team.
We write your papers in a standardized way. We complete your work in such a way that it turns out to be a perfect description of your guidelines.
We promise you excellent grades and academic excellence that you always longed for. Our writers stay in touch with you via email.